PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of jintaidssocLink to Publisher's site
 
J Int AIDS Soc. 2017; 20(1): 21412.
Published online 2017 June 14. doi:  10.7448/IAS.20.1.21412
PMCID: PMC5515037

Cross-cultural assessment of HIV-associated cognitive impairment using the Kaufman assessment battery for children: a systematic review

Abstract

Introduction: Despite improved efficacy of, and access to, combination antiretroviral therapy (cART), HIV-associated cognitive impairments remain prevalent in both children and adults. Neuropsychological tests that detect such impairment can help clinicians formulate effective treatment plans. The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (KABC), although developed and standardized in the United States, is used frequently in many different countries and cultural contexts to assess paediatric performance across various cognitive domains. This systematic review investigated the cross-cultural utility of the original KABC, and its 2nd edition (KABC-II), in detecting HIV-associated cognitive impairment in children and adolescents.

Methods: We entered relevant keywords and MeSH terms into the PubMed, PsycInfo, EBSCOHost, ProQuest, and Scopus databases, with search limits set from 1983–2017. Two independent reviewers evaluated the retrieved abstracts and manuscripts. Studies eligible for inclusion in the review were those that (a) used the KABC/KABC-II to assess cognitive function in children/adolescents aged 2–18 years, (b) featured a definition of cognitive impairment (e.g. >2 SD below the mean) or compared the performance of HIV-infected and uninfected control groups, and (c) used a sample excluded from population on which the instruments were normed.

Results and discussion: We identified nine studies (eight conducted in African countries, and one in the United Kingdom) to comprise the review’s sample. All studies detected cognitive impairment in HIV-infected children, including those who were cART-naïve or who were cART treated and clinically stable. KABC/KABC-II subtests assessing simultaneous processing appeared most sensitive. Evaluation of the methodological quality of the selected studies by two independent reviews suggested that shortcomings included reporting and selection biases.

Conclusions: This systematic review provides evidence for the cross-cultural utility of the KABC/KABC-II, particularly the simultaneous processing subtests, in detecting cognitive impairment in HIV-infected children (including those who are clinically stable). Although the current results suggest there is justification for using the KABC/KABC-II primarily in East Africa, further investigation is required to explore the instrument’s utility in other HIV-prevalent regions of the globe.

Keywords: cognitive impairment, cross-cultural, HIV, Kaufman assessment battery for children, paediatric, systematic review

Introduction

Recent global estimates suggest that 3.2 million children under 15 years of age are living with HIV. Ninety-one per cent (more than 2.9 million) of those children reside in Sub-Saharan Africa [1]. The effects of HIV infection on children’s physical growth, psychological health, and neurodevelopment ranges from mild to devastating. These effects extend to cognitive development: A wealth of evidence indicates that HIV-infected children are likely to present with some form of cognitive impairment, with reported deficits in domains including attention, processing speed, language, motor skills, learning and memory, visual-spatial abilities, and executive functioning [24].

Research investigating the cognitive development of African, Indian, Asian, European, and South American HIV-infected children has reported a high (up to 90%) prevalence of cognitive and neurodevelopmental delays [511]. Despite this state of affairs, HIV-infected children are not routinely screened or formally assessed for cognitive delays or deficits. Although Boyede and colleagues [12] reported on the validation of a screening tool for rapid screening of moderate-to-severe global developmental delays in HIV-infected South African children, that tool is suitable only for those aged 9–36 months, and has not been validated for use elsewhere. Furthermore, although screening tools are useful in offering a basic determination of the presence or absence of cognitive deficits, they often lack the sensitivity and the theoretical framework required of comprehensive diagnostic instruments, and cannot deliver in-depth critical analysis of potential deficits [13,14].

Clinicians weighing the appropriateness of a cognitive measure for their particular context must consider whether a test developed and standardized on a specific population continues to measure the same construct when applied in a different setting [15,16]. Measured consideration of the cross-cultural equivalence of neuropsychological tests is often undermined by the grim practical reality of a severe lack of approved test material, however [17,18]. In low- and middle-income countries (LAMICs), especially, clinical neuropsychologists are hampered in their practice by a paucity of locally developed, standardized, and normed tests [19,20]. This situation is concerning in light of the prevalence of cultural and language differences, educational inequalities, and socio-political disadvantages that are often present in those countries, and that influence performance on standardized measures of cognitive function [2123].

The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (KABC), and its revised second edition, the KABC-II, are measures of intellectual functioning, developed and standardized in the United States, with strong psychometric characteristics [24,25]. As Figure 1 shows, the KABC and KABC-II both assess a wide range of cognitive domains, including those commonly affected by HIV in children. The KABC is suitable for administration to children aged from 2 years 6 months to 12 years 6 months, whereas the KABC-II is suitable for children aged from 3 years 0 months to 18 years 11 months. Each battery can be administered in 25–100 min, depending on the child’s age. Whereas the KABC was grounded in the Horn and Cattell’s theory of crystallized versus fluid intelligence [26], the KABC-II’s results can be interpreted according to either Luria’s neuropsychological theory of processing [27] or the Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) psychometric model [28]. What this means, in practice, is that the KABC-II measures the same abilities as the KABC, but also measures abilities in two additional cognitive domains (viz., Planning and Learning).

Figure 1.
Subtests and indices comprising the KABC and KABC-II, and the cognitive abilities assessed by each. The left panel shows subtests unique to the KABC, the right panel subtests unique to KABC-II, and the middle panel subtests common to the two. The KABC ...

Both the KABC and KABC-II have been used across the globe to assess cognitive functioning comprehensively [29,30]. Their widespread use is attributable largely to the fact that (a) they incorporate teaching items to increase familiarity with the test materials, (b) test responses require very little verbalization from examinees, and (c) early psychometric studies suggested they were culture-fair when applied to different ethnic groups within the United States [3133]. Subsequently, validation studies conducted in Africa and Asia have demonstrated that the instruments maintain their construct validity, and are sensitive to socio-economic factors and disease effects [e.g. 34,35,36]. Furthermore, a meta-analysis of KABC validation studies across cultures supported the factor integrity of the distinction between the Sequential Processing versus Simultaneous Processing indices [37]. In summary, strong theoretical and psychometric foundations, culture-fair assessment techniques, and cross-cultural adaptability have positioned the KABC/KABC-II as the best-choice instrument for researchers or clinicians who operate in a variety of cultural contexts and who require a well-standardized measure of cognitive ability within specific domains [30,36,38].

Despite the KABC/KABC-II’s popularity, and its widespread use in regions where HIV is highly prevalent (e.g. sub-Saharan Africa), no study has formally evaluated whether these instruments are suited to identify cognitive impairment, across distinct and independent domains, in HIV-infected children. (Indeed, there are currently no validated neuropsychological tests, or test batteries, designed specifically to detect such deficits in children or in adolescents.) Hence, this systematic review aimed to determine whether the KABC/KABC-II identifies HIV-associated cognitive impairment in children who reside in cultural contexts outside of that in which the instrument was developed, standardized, and validated.

Methods

Figure 2 is a PRISMA flowchart documenting the process by which we arrived at the final sample of studies that met the eligibility criteria and that were included in the systematic review. Below, we describe each stage of that process in detail.

Figure 2.
PRISMA flowchart documenting search process and results.

Search strategy

We used electronic databases (EBSCOHost, ProQuest, PsycInfo, PubMed, and Scopus) to search for published and unpublished studies, posters, PowerPoints, and abstracts. The search limits were set from 1983 (the year the KABC was published) to February 2017. Keywords were MeSH and non-MeSH search terms covering HIV/AIDS, children, HIV medication (e.g. HAART), KABC, and cognitive development/functioning (see Additional File 1). The search identified 38 studies. We then conducted a manual search of the references from the identified articles and published conference proceedings to ensure all relevant articles were identified. This arm of the search strategy yielded an additional 28 articles.

Study selection

Two authors (KvW and TvdW) screened abstracts (and full text if needed) to determine inclusion status. Disagreements were resolved by discussion. We included studies that met the following criteria: (1) Used a sample that comprised HIV-infected children (who may or may not have been on cART, and may or may not have been co-infected); (2) used a sample consisting of children between the ages of 2 and 18 years, inclusive; (3) used (a subtest of) the KABC/KABC-II; (4) provided a method to indicate cognitive impairment in the HIV-infected sample (e.g. comparison to normative or control group, definition of impairment provided); and (5) used a sample excluded from the original normative data on which the KABC was standardized (i.e. we excluded studies that used as their samples individuals who identified as African-American, Hispanic, American White, American Indian, Alaskan Native, Asian-American, or Pacific Islander).

There were no exclusion criteria based on language or format of publication. All study designs were included, with the exception of reviews. The latter were used to search for any other studies that could be included. If a full text was unavailable online, we emailed the authors and followed up weekly. If there was no response after 3 months, the article was considered excluded/missing data. Items coded as “not specified” (NS) indicates that we did not receive a response (see Additional File 2).

Data extraction and quality assessment

Nine studies (seven peer-reviewed journal articles, one poster, and one Master’s thesis) fulfilled the inclusion criteria and were reviewed [3947]. Two authors (KvW and TvdW) performed data extraction using a spreadsheet (see Additional File 1) based on the STROBE statement for observational studies [48]. STROBE is a detailed checklist developed to ensure that adequate data are extracted from all studies (e.g. cohort, cross-sectional, case-control) included in systematic reviews. Here, we were particularly interested in the place of study, inclusion of confounding variables, reporting of study limitations and strengths, and detailed information on HIV status and KABC results. In addition, the same two authors independently assessed the quality of all included studies (see Table 1 and Additional File 1) using a modified version of the Downs and Black [49] checklist. Originally designed for the assessment of the methodological quality of randomized and non-randomized studies, the checklist consists of 26 items representing six sub-scales: reporting, internal validity, external validity, bias, confounding, and power. Because none of the included studies reported power calculations to determine whether there was an adequate sample size to assess the ability of the KABC to determine between-group differences, item 27 was excluded from the standard checklist. Hence, the maximum score for the modified checklist was 26. Again, disagreements were resolved by discussion. Since the Downs and Black checklist does not stipulate a cutoff for suitable studies, we used the mid-point score of 13 to differentiate between lower- and higher-quality studies [50].

Table 1.
Study Quality Assessment

Results and discussion

We set out to determine, via systematic review, whether a popular, widely used, and psychometrically sound cognitive test battery, the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, identifies HIV-associated cognitive impairment in children who reside in cultural contexts outside of that in which the instrument was developed, standardized, and validated.

Study characteristics

As Table 1 shows, 8 of the 9 studies that formed the final sample were conducted in Africa, where the vast majority of HIV-infected children reside. The other study was conducted in the United Kingdom. Uganda was the most represented country (five studies), followed by South Africa (two studies, one of which was a multi-site study that also featured data collected from Malawi, Uganda, and Zimbabwe), England, and the Democratic Republic of the Congo (one each). Four studies featured cross-sectional designs, three were longitudinal, one used a prospective design, and one was a randomized control trial.

As Table 2 shows, the studies featured a total of 1792 participants, including 720 (51%) who were HIV infected. Of the latter participants, 329 (46%) were cART-naïve at study initiation. Across all studies, the age range of participants was 2–14 years (M for HIV-infected participants = 7.9 years; M for controls, across the seven studies that reported this statistic, was 7.4 years). Regarding the sex distribution across the comparison groups, in the six studies that reported this statistic 369 of the 720 HIV-infected participants (51%) were female, as were 473 of the 842 uninfected controls (56%).

Table 2.
Description of study characteristics and findings

Four studies [39,41,43,44] used the KABC, whereas the rest used the KABC-II. All adapted the instrument’s administration and/or scoring to improve its fairness to their sample. For instance, six [3941,4446] did not administer (or, at least, do not report results related to) the Achievement and/or Knowledge subtests, which assess crystallized intelligence and therefore are likely to rely heavily on exposure to the mainstream culture within which the test was developed [51]. Similarly, five [3941,45,46] reported administering the instrument in the participants’ home language.

Quality assessment

Table 1 presents the findings from our critical evaluation of the quality of each of the eight studies. Most were rated as being of relatively higher methodological quality, with all except one scoring above the mid-point score of 13 on the Downs and Black checklist, and four scoring more than 20 out of the maximum possible 26. All studies considered potentially confounding variables (e.g. age, sex, CD4 and viral loads, and whether participants were cART treated or cART naïve) in their interpretation of results, and all except one included such potential confounders in their statistical analyses. During data extraction, we noted that reporting limitations primarily related to the selection and recruitment of samples. Hence, we cannot eliminate the possibility of selection bias based on the information provided in the articles. We also noted that, in two of the eight studies, some results were based on data dredging.

KABC/KABC-II identification of HIV-associated cognitive impairment

Overall, our review suggests that the KABC/KABC-II can be used successfully across different countries and cultural contexts to identify cognitive impairment in HIV-infected children and adolescents. Hence, although there is no current consensus regarding whether adult diagnostic criteria for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) might be applied to children and adolescents [52], it appears the KABC/KABC-II might usefully serve, alongside assessments of functional competency, as a core component of a battery that describes where along the HAND spectrum HIV-infected individuals younger than 18 years might be placed.

In each of the reviewed studies, the KABC/KABC-II successfully identified cognitive impairment in HIV-infected children, either relative to uninfected counterparts or to their own baseline. Eight of the nine studies used local normative data, or a local reference group, against which to compare cognitive performance of HIV-infected children. These studies identified impairment at the group level (i.e. they classed the performance of the group of HIV-infected children as “impaired” if there were significant between-group differences, in favour of the control/normative group, on the particular subtest or index under consideration). Gosling et al. [44] reported a decline in cognitive functioning across longitudinal follow-up, but did not specify (a) whether this decline suggested impairment relative to healthy controls, or (b) the subtests/scales that formed the bases for this observation. As Table 2 shows, six studies reported significant between-group differences on the Simultaneous Processing index, suggesting that HIV-infected children might have particular difficulty on visual-perceptual tests that require them to disintegrate, manipulate, and reintegrate component parts of a whole unit. Together with the fact that four studies also detected significant between-group differences on the Sequential Processing index (i.e. on tests that assess the ability to encode, store, and then organize items of information into a logical sequence), one might conclude that the cognitive processes involving activity in the posterior regions of the brain are particularly susceptible to impairment in HIV-infected children [32,52].

Across all studies, there was no single KABC/KABC-II subtest or scale on which HIV-infected children performed consistently poorly. Hence, this review suggests that no single subtest offers the potential to be adapted into a stand-alone screening tool. This conclusion is consistent with research indicating that numerous independent cognitive domains are affected in HIV-infected children [3,53]. Appropriate assessment of these children should therefore include administration of, at least, Simultaneous Processing, and Sequential Processing subtests of the KABC/KABC-II, and should probably include the Planning and Learning subtests as well.

Our review also demonstrates that the KABC/KABC-II is sensitive to cognitive impairment in HIV-infected children, with and without cART, when compared to controls. This finding is promising because a growing body of research demonstrates that subtle cognitive impairments may persist even in HIV-infected children who are well controlled on cART [5456].

To improve the culture-fairness of the instrument, researchers across the reviewed studies typically implemented a three-part strategy: (1) They translated it into the local language; (2) they excluded either or both the Knowledge and Achievement components (i.e. those subtests that rely heavily on crystalized intelligence, or learned, culture-specific environmental experiences); and (3) where local normative data were unavailable, they applied either conventional cutoffs (1 SD below the standardization sample mean), or stricter cutoffs (2 SD below the mean of a local control group), to classify impairment, depending on whether standardization sample data were judged applicable or not.

Finally, we identified an interesting trend in the reviewed studies: The KABC/KABC-II was not always administered by a registered or licensed clinical psychologist or neuropsychologist. Rather, administration fell to local teachers, research assistants, or psychometrists. Although not all of the manuscripts make it clear, perusal of the author lists and acknowledgements suggests that these test administrators all operated under appropriate supervision. In light of the scarcity of highly-trained professionals in low- and middle-income countries where HIV is prevalent [39,57], it is useful to know that the instrument can be administered by trained lay professionals, with the ethical proviso that these individuals work (a) according to guidelines offered by the International Test Commission, (b) under the supervision of a qualified expert, and (c) with permission of the test publisher.

Limitations

The strength of the conclusions one might draw from this systematic review are limited by the characteristics of the reviewed studies and by the nature of the reviewed instrument. We therefore offer the following caveats.

First, although authors of the included studies noted that they had translated the instrument from English into the local language, most did not describe their translation procedures in detail. So, for example, it is unknown whether rigorous back-translations procedures were in place, and whether community members were consulted about idiomatic aspects of the translation. The absence of this information raises questions around linguistic equivalence of the various translated versions of the KABC/KABC-II.

Second, no KABC/KABC-II subtest measures the cognitive construct of information processing speed directly. This limits the value of the instrument in assessment of HIV-infected children in different countries and cultural contexts, given that (a) strong recent evidence suggests that processing speed is an important component of HANI [53], and (b) there are cross-cultural differences in the rate at which processing speed matures and develops across childhood and adolescence [58].

Third, HIV-1 subtype distribution is not consistent across the globe [7], and so the studies reviewed here were not focused on cognitive impairment associated with a single clade type. For instance, the studies in Central and East Africa likely included a predominance of clade A-infected children, whereas those conducted in sub-Saharan Africa likely included a predominance of clade C-infected children. Hence, one must exercise caution when generalizing these findings because of the possibility that clade-specific neuropathogenic differences might manifest in differing degrees of disease severity [40,59,60].

Conclusions

The findings of this review suggest that the KABC/KABC-II has cross-cultural utility. It appears that the instrument can provide comprehensive information regarding cognitive impairment in HIV-infected children, regardless of the country or cultural context in which it is administered. The instrument is especially useful because it can be administered by laypersons, and because it is sensitive enough to identify impairment in children who are otherwise well managed (i.e. who are clinically stable on cART). However, the review also highlights the need for more cross-cultural validity studies of the KABC/KABC-II, and, particularly, for research investigating whether the instrument is sensitive to clade-specific variations in cognitive impairment. In such future research (and, indeed, in any research using the KABC/KABC-II with HIV-infected children), we suggest that the adaptation procedures described in the studies reviewed here be used as a baseline to ensure culture-fair testing. We further recommend that, when adapting test material, researchers apply the standard procedures set out by the International Test Commission [61,62], and that they describe all adaptations clearly in the published material.

Acknowledgements

We thank Mrs. Alvina Mathee and Ms. Tamsyn Suliaman for their advice and assistance with the systematic search.

Biography

• 

KvW was responsible for the conceptualization of the paper, conducting the groundwork regarding the systematic search (i.e. developing the search terms and strategies, conducting the systematic search, recording all information regarding the systematic search process and results), evaluating titles and/or abstracts for inclusion, evaluating full-text articles for inclusion, extracting the data, performing the quality appraisal, interpreting the results, writing up the final manuscript, and approving the final manuscript as submitted. TvdW was responsible for evaluating titles and/or abstracts for inclusion, assessing full-text articles for inclusion, preparing spreadsheets, extracting data, and assisting KvW with the quality appraisal of articles. Furthermore, TvdW reviewed the manuscript and suggested/made relevant changes prior to submission to the journal. MJB reviewed the manuscript and suggested/made relevant changes prior to submission to the journal. MFC reviewed the manuscript and suggested/made relevant changes prior to submission to the journal. KGFT assisted with the conceptualization of the paper and provided supervision of this work throughout the entire process. He critically reviewed the manuscript and suggested/made relevant changes prior to submission to the journal. KGFT gave final approval of the manuscript as submitted to the journal, is responsible for submission, and will be the corresponding author. All authors approve the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Funding Statement

This research was not directly funded. KVW receives funding from the South African National Research Foundation (Free-Standing NRF Doctoral Scholarship) and from the University of Cape Town (UCT Doctoral Scholarship).

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Disclaimer

The views expressed are the authors’ and do not necessarily reflect the views and policies of the funders.

To access the supplementary material to this article please see Supplementary Files under Article Tools online.

References

[1] UNAIDS The gap report. Geneva, Switzerland: Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS (UNAIDS); 2014. [PubMed]
[2] Nassen R, Donald K, Walker K, Paruk S, Vujovic M, Duncan W, et al Management of mental health disorders and central nervous system sequelae in HIV-positive children and adolescents. South Afr J HIV Med. 2014;15(3):81–11.
[3] Koekkoek S, De Sonneville LMJ, Wolfs TFW, Licht R, Geelen SPM. Neurocognitive function profile in HIV-infected school-age children. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2008;12(4):290–97. [PubMed]
[4] Van Rie A, Harrington PR, Dow A, Robertson K Neurologic and neurodevelopmental manifestations of pediatric HIV/AIDS: a global perspective. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2007;11(1):1–9. [PubMed]
[5] Isaranurug S, Chompikul J Emotional development and nutritional status of HIV/AIDS orphaned children aged 6–12 years old in Thailand. Matern Child Health J. 2009;13(1):138–43. [PubMed]
[6] Lowick S, Sawry S, Meyers T Neurodevelopmental delay among HIV-infected preschool children receiving antiretroviral therapy and healthy preschool children in Soweto, South Africa. Psychol Health Med. 2012;17(5):599–610. [PubMed]
[7] Geretti AM. HIV-1 subtypes: epidemiology and significance for HIV management. Curr Opin Infect Dis. 2006;19(1):1–7. [PubMed]
[8] Ravindran OS, Rani MP, Priya G Cognitive deficits in HIV infected children. Indian J Psychol Med. 2014;36(3):255–59. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[9] Rice ML, Buchanan AL, Siberry GK, Malee KM, Zeldow B, Frederick T, et al Language impairment in children perinatally infected with HIV compared to children who were HIV-exposed and uninfected. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2012;33(2):112–23. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[10] Serchuck LK, Williams PL, Nachman S, Gadow KD, Chernoff M, Schwartz L Prevalence of pain and association with psychiatric symptom severity in perinatally HIV-infected children as compared to controls living in HIV-affected households. AIDS Care. 2010;22(5):640–48. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[11] Thomaidis L, Bertou G, Critselis E, Spoulou V, Kafetzis DA, Theodoridou M Cognitive and psychosocial development of HIV pediatric patients receiving highly active anti-retroviral therapy: a case-control study. BMC Pediatr. 2010;10(1):1. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[12] Boyede G, Eley B, Donald K Preliminary validation of a new developmental screening tool for neurodevelopmental delay in HIV-infected South African children. J Child Neurol. 2016;31(2):145–52. [PubMed]
[13] Harvey PD. Clinical applications of neuropsychological assessment. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2012;14(1):91–99. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[14] Mosdell J, Balchin R, Ameen O Adaptation of aphasia tests for neurocognitive screening in South Africa. S Afr J Psychol. 2010;40(3):250–61.
[15] Pedraza O, Mungas D Measurement in cross-cultural neuropsychology. Neuropsychol Rev. 2008;18(3):184–93. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[16] Van De Vijver FJR, Poortinga YH Towards an integrated analysis of bias in cross-cultural assessment. Eur J Psychol Assess. 1997;13(1):29.
[17] Van De Vijver F, Tanzer NK Bias and equivalence in cross-cultural assessment: an overview. Eur Rev Appl Psychol. 2004;54(2):119–35.
[18] Foxcroft CD. Planning a psychological test in the multicultural South African context. SA J Ind Psychol. 2004;30(4):8–15.
[19] Anderson SJ. On the importance of collecting local neuropsychological normative data. S Afr J Psychol. 2001;31(3):29–34.
[20] Scazufca M, Almeida OP, Vallada HP, Tasse WA, Menezes PR Limitations of the Mini-Mental State Examination for screening dementia in a community with low socioeconomic status. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2009;259(1):8–15. [PubMed]
[21] Olson K, Jacobson K Cross-cultural considerations in pediatric neuropsychology: A review and call to attention. Appl Neuropsychol Child. 2015;4(3):166–77. [PubMed]
[22] Shuttleworth-Edwards AB, Kemp RD, Rust AL, Muirhead JG, Hartman NP, Radloff SE Cross-cultural effects on IQ test performance: a review and preliminary normative indications on WAIS-III test performance. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2004;26(7):903–20. [PubMed]
[23] Ferrett HL, Thomas KG, Tapert SF, Carey PD, Conradie S, Cuzen NL, et al The cross-cultural utility of foreign- and locally-derived normative data for three WHO-endorsed neuropsychological tests for South African adolescents. Metab Brain Dis. 2014;29(2):395–408. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[24] Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL Kaufman assessment battery for children. San Antonio (TX): Pearson; 1983.
[25] Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL Kaufman assessment battery for children: second edition (KABC-II). San Antonio (TX): Pearson; 2004.
[26] Horn JL, Cattell RB Refinement and test of the theory of fluid and crystallized general intelligences. J Educ Psychol. 1966;57(5):253–70. [PubMed]
[27] Luria AR. The functional organization of the brain. Sci Am. 1970;222(3):66–78. [PubMed]
[28] McGrew KS, Keith TZ, Flanagan DP, Vanderwood M Beyond g: the impact of Gf-Gc specific cognitive abilities research on the future use and interpretation of intelligence tests in the schools. School Psych Rev. 1997;26(2):189–210.
[29] De Sousa D. Cognitive processing skills in monolingual and bilingual South African children: implications for assessment in linguistically diverse societies. Language Matters. 2012;43(1):97–112.
[30] Boivin MJ, Giordani B Improvements in cognitive performance for schoolchildren in Zaire, Africa, following an iron supplement and treatment for intestinal parasites. J Pediatr Psychol. 1993;18(2):249–64. [PubMed]
[31] Keith TZ, Reynolds MR Cattell–Horn–Carroll abilities and cognitive tests: what we’ve learned from 20 years of research. Psychol Sch. 2010;47(7):635–50.
[32] Kaufman AS, Lichtenberger EO, Fletcher-Janzen E, Kaufman NL Essentials of KABC-II assessment. Hoboken (NJ): Wiley; 2005.
[33] Reynolds CR, Kamphaus RW, Rosenthal BL Applications of the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) in neuropsychological assessment In: Reynolds CR, Fletcher-Janzen E, editors. Handbook of clinical child neuropsychology. New York: Springer; 1989. p. 205–26.
[34] Bangirana P, Seggane M, Allebeck P, Giordani B, John CC, Opoka OR, et al A preliminary examination of the construct validity of the KABC-II in Ugandan children with a history of cerebral malaria. Afr Health Sci. 2009;9(3):186–92. [PubMed]
[35] Malda M, Van De Vijver FJ, Srinivasan K, Transler C, Sukumar P Traveling with cognitive tests: testing the validity of a KABC-II adaptation in India. Assessment. 2010;17(1):107–15. [PubMed]
[36] Moon S-B, McLean JE, Kaufman AS A cross-cultural validation of the Sequential-Simultaneous theory of intelligence in children. Sch Psychol Int. 2003;24(4):449–61.
[37] Ochieng CO. Meta-analysis of the validation studies of the Kaufman assessment battery for children. Int J Testing. 2003;3(1):77–93.
[38] Boivin MJ, Giordani B Neuropsychological assessment of African children: evidence for a universal brain/behavior omnibus within a coconstructivist paradigm. Prog Brain Res. 2009;178:113–35. [PubMed]
[39] Bagenda D, Nassali A, Kalyesubula I, Sherman B, Drotar D, Boivin MJ, et al Health, neurologic, and cognitive status of HIV-infected, long-surviving, and antiretroviral-naive Ugandan children. Pediatrics. 2006;117(3):729–40. [PubMed]
[40] Boivin MJ, Ruel TD, Boal HE, Bangirana P, Cao H, Eller LA, et al HIV-subtype A is associated with poorer neuropsychological performance compared with subtype D in antiretroviral therapy-naive Ugandan children. Aids. 2010;24(8):1163–70. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[41] Boivin MJ, Green SDR, Davies AG, Giordani B, Mokili JKL, Cutting WAM A preliminary evaluation of the cognitive and motor effects on pediatric HIV infection in Zairian children. Health Psychol. 1995;14(1):13–21. [PubMed]
[42] Boivin MJ, Busman RA, Parikh SM, Bangirana P, Page CF, Opoka RO, et al A pilot study of the neuropsychological benefits of computerized cognitive rehabilitation in Ugandan children with HIV. Neuropsychology. 2010;24(5):667–73. [PubMed]
[43] Brahmbhatt H, Boivin M, Ssempija V, Kagaayi J, Kigozi G, Serwadda D, et al Impact of HIV and antiretroviral therapy on neurocognitive outcomes among school aged children. JAIDS. 2017. February 6;75(1):1–8. [PubMed]
[44] Gosling AS, Burns J, Hirst F Children with HIV in the UK: A longitudinal study of adaptive and cognitive functioning. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;9(1):25–37.
[45] Merkle V. The cognitive development of HIV-infected and uninfected children in South Africa [dissertation]. Würzburg, Germany: Julius Maximilian University of Würzburg; 2015.
[46] Ruel TD, Boivin MJ, Boal HE, Bangirana P, Charlebois E, Havlir DV, et al Neurocognitive and motor deficits in HIV-infected Ugandan children with high CD4 cell counts. Clin Infect Dis. 2012;54(7):1001–09. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[47] Boivin MJ, Chernoff M, Lindsey J, Zimmer B, Laughton B, Joyce C, et al Initial findings from IMPAACT P1104s: a neuropsychological evaluation of HIV-infected and uninfected children in sub-Saharan Africa. International Maternal Pediatric Adolescent AIDS Clinical Trials Network; 2015. June 18; Washington, DC.
[48] Von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, Pocock SJ, Gøtzsche PC, Vandenbroucke JP The strengthening the reporting of observational studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. Lancet. 2007;370(9596):1453–57. [PubMed]
[49] Downs SH, Black N The feasibility of creating a checklist for the assessment of the methodological quality both of randomised and non-randomised studies of health care interventions. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1998;52(6):377–84. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[50] Stangl AL, Lloyd JK, Brady LM, Holland CE, Baral S A systematic review of interventions to reduce HIV-related stigma and discrimination from 2002 to 2013: how far have we come? J Int AIDS Soc. 2013;16(3Suppl2):18734. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[51] Cocodia EA. Cultural perceptions of human intelligence. J Intell. 2014;2(4):180–96.
[52] Giordani B, Boivin MJ, Opel B, Nseyila DND, Lauer RE Use of the K-ABC with children in Zaire, Africa: an evaluation of the sequential-simultaneous processing distinction within an intercultural context. Intl J Disabil Dev Educ. 1996;43(1):5–24.
[53] Hoare J, Phillips N, Joska JA, Paul R, Donald KA, Stein DJ, et al Applying the HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder diagnostic criteria to HIV-infected youth. Neurology. 2016;87(1):86–93. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[54] Le Doare K, Bland R, Newell ML Neurodevelopment in children born to HIV-infected mothers by infection and treatment status. Pediatrics. 2012;130(5):e1326–44. [PubMed]
[55] Crowell CS, Malee KM, Yogev R, Muller WJ Neurologic disease in HIV-infected children and the impact of combination antiretroviral therapy. Rev Med Virol. 2014;24(5):316–31. [PubMed]
[56] Paramesparan Y, Garvey LJ, Ashby J, Foster CJ, Fidler S, Winston A High rates of asymptomatic neurocognitive impairment in vertically acquired HIV-1-infected adolescents surviving to adulthood. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2010;55(1):134–36. [PubMed]
[57] Robbins RN, Joska JA, Thomas KGF, Stein DJ, Linda T, Mellins CA, et al Exploring the utility of the montreal cognitive assessment to detect HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder: the challenge and need for culturally valid screening tests in South Africa. Clin Neuropsychol. 2013;27(3):437–54. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[58] Kail RV, McBride-Chang C, Ferrer E, Cho J-R, Shu H Cultural differences in the development of processing speed. Dev Sci. 2013;16(3):476–83. [PubMed]
[59] Witten JA, Thomas KGF, Westgarth-Taylor J, Joska JA Executive dyscontrol of learning and memory: findings from a Clade C HIV-positive South African sample. Clin Neuropsychol. 2015;29(7):956–84. [PubMed]
[60] Yndart A, Kaushik A, Agudelo M, Raymond A, Atluri VS, Saxena SK, et al Investigation of neuropathogenesis in HIV-1 clade B and C infection associated with IL-33 and ST2 regulation. ACS Chem Neurosci. 2015;6(9):1600–12. [PubMed]
[61] International Test Commission International guidelines for test use. 2000. [cited 2016 August1]. Available from: http://www.intestcom.org
[62] International Test Commission International test commission guidelines for translating and adapting tests. 2010. [cited 2016 August1]. Available from: http://www.intestcom.org

Articles from Journal of the International AIDS Society are provided here courtesy of The International AIDS Society