PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of eneuroeNeuroThis ArticleFor AuthorsAlertsSubmit a Manuscript
 
eNeuro. 2016 May-Jun; 3(3): ENEURO.0064-16.2016.
Published online 2016 May 31. Prepublished online 2016 May 11. doi:  10.1523/ENEURO.0064-16.2016
PMCID: PMC4886221

A Transgenic Rat for Specifically Inhibiting Adult Neurogenesis1,2,3

Abstract

The growth of research on adult neurogenesis and the development of new models and tools have greatly advanced our understanding of the function of newborn neurons in recent years. However, there are still significant limitations in the ability to identify the functions of adult neurogenesis in available models. Here we report a transgenic rat (TK rat) that expresses herpes simplex virus thymidine kinase in GFAP+ cells. Upon treating TK rats with the antiviral drug valganciclovir, granule cell neurogenesis can be completely inhibited in adulthood, in both the hippocampus and olfactory bulb. Interestingly, neurogenesis in the glomerular and external plexiform layers of the olfactory bulb was only partially inhibited, suggesting that some adult-born neurons in these regions derive from a distinct precursor population that does not express GFAP. Within the hippocampus, blockade of neurogenesis was rapid and nearly complete within 1 week of starting treatment. Preliminary behavioral analyses indicate that general anxiety levels and patterns of exploration are generally unaffected in neurogenesis-deficient rats. However, neurogenesis-deficient TK rats showed reduced sucrose preference, suggesting deficits in reward-related behaviors. We expect that TK rats will facilitate structural, physiological, and behavioral studies that complement those possible in existing models, broadly enhancing understanding of the function of adult neurogenesis.

Keywords: adult neurogenesis, hippocampus, olfactory bulb, plasticity

Significance Statement

Adult neurogenesis contributes to the physiological and behavioral functions of the brain, but our understanding is subject to the limitations of available animal models. Although mice have been invaluable research models, their small size and limited behavioral repertoire hinders certain types of experiments. We have therefore created a transgenic rat in which neurogenesis can be inhibited completely and selectively in adulthood in two brain regions, the hippocampus and subventricular zone-olfactory bulb. We expect that these rats will complement existing approaches and enhance understanding of the function of adult neurogenesis.

Introduction

Although the existence of adult hippocampal neurogenesis is no longer disputed, its function is still uncertain. Additional anatomical, behavioral, and physiological studies are needed to understand how new neurons affect circuits and cognition. Historically, most rodent neuroscience studies were done in rats. However, increased focus on genetics and the development of transgenic technology have tipped the balance strongly in favor of mice. Nonetheless, rat studies may be increasing again due to an increase in sequence data as well as recognition that greater similarity to humans in some genes, such as those encoding tau proteins, can lead to transgenic rat models that better replicate the spectrum of human pathology (Abbott, 2004; Cohen et al., 2013).

Their large body size, relative to mice, has long made rats the model of choice for physiology studies (Abbott, 2004). This is equally true for neurophysiology: nearly all of the Nobel-prize winning place cell work, for example, was done in rats (Moser et al., 2008). This is due in large part to size, which gives rats a better ability to support large multielectrode arrays and investigators greater accuracy in stereotaxic targeting. However, hippocampal place cell representations are also less stable in mice than in rats, which can make it challenging to relate firing patterns to environmental inputs and ongoing behavior (Kentros et al., 2004). Understanding the function of new hippocampal neurons at the circuit level will almost certainly be advanced by studying the network consequences of altering neurogenesis in rats.

Advantages in behavioral testing may provide the greatest motivation for developing models of altered adult neurogenesis in rats. Thus far, the majority of behavioral studies of hippocampal neurogenesis have focused on spatial or episodic-like memory and innate emotional behaviors. However, early studies, as well as more recent findings implicate the hippocampus, and therefore potentially new dentate gyrus neurons, in a number of additional behaviors including orienting and attention, behavioral inhibition, delay-discounting, and salience attribution (Lodge and Grace, 2011; Cameron and Glover, 2015). Most of these behaviors have been studied extensively in rats and very little in mice, so testing the role of neurogenesis in these behaviors will almost certainly be more straightforward in rats. Cognitive scientists in general are increasingly embracing the rat as an ideal behavioral test subject for a broad spectrum of behavioral studies, arguing that they are capable of more complex tasks than mice but with greater potential for genetic manipulation than nonhuman primates (Abbott, 2004).

Options for manipulating neurogenesis in rats currently exist: chemical, radiological, and viral strategies have all been successfully used to inhibit adult neurogenesis (Tada et al., 2000; Shors et al., 2001; Jessberger et al., 2009). However, the advantages of transgenic lines, which include the ability to eliminate neurogenesis noninvasively, are largely limited to mice. To address this problem, we have developed a transgenic rat in which neurogenesis can be specifically inhibited, either partially or completely, at any age. The approach uses a well characterized pharmacogenetic strategy through which cell-specific expression of the simplex virus thymidine kinase (HSV-TK) gene enables selective ablation of proliferating cells. HSV-TK (but not endogenous mammalian TK) phosphorylates nucleoside analogs such as ganciclovir, which are then incorporated into the DNA of replicating cells, leading to inhibition of DNA synthesis and apoptosis (Elion et al., 1977; Furman et al., 1980). Depending on the TK expression pattern and the timing of ganciclovir administration, specific populations of precursor cells that are active at various stages of development can be selectively ablated (Borrelli et al., 1989; Heyman et al., 1989; Delaney et al., 1996). This approach has been used to inhibit neurogenesis in adult mice (Garcia et al., 2004; Saxe et al., 2006; Deng et al., 2009; Singer et al., 2009; Snyder et al., 2011; Cummings et al., 2014; Seo et al., 2015). It has recently been tried in rats as well, though with limited success, likely related to random insertion effects (Groves et al., 2013). Here, we describe a new transgenic rat strain that shows HSV-TK expression under the control of the glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP) promoter and ganciclovir-induced loss of adult neurogenesis in two regions that generate new neurons from GFAP-expressing radial cells: the dentate gyrus and subventricular zone-olfactory bulb. Initial behavioral analyses indicate that neurogenesis-deficient GFAP-TK rats are similar to neurogenesis-intact wild-type littermates in general locomotion and baseline anxiety but display reduced sucrose preference, suggesting alterations in reward-related behaviors. We expect that these rats, and rat models in general, will complement existing mouse models and facilitate understanding of the functions of adult neurogenesis.

Methods

All procedures followed the Institute of Laboratory Animal Research guidelines and were approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the National Institute of Mental Health.

Transgenic animals

Transgenic rats expressing HSV-TK under the human GFAP promoter (GFAP-TK, or TK rats) were generated on a Long–Evans background using the pGfa2-TK1 plasmid (Delaney et al., 1996) applying standard techniques. Female rats were used for breeding, as males of this strain fail to pass on the transgene, as is commonly seen in lines that express HSV-TK under any promoter (Braun et al., 1990; al-Shawi et al., 1991; Delaney et al., 1996). Male rats were used for all experiments. Rats were weaned at 21–28 d of age, pair-housed, and genotyped by PCR. Rats were subjected to a 12 h light/dark schedule with lights on at 6:00 A.M. Neurogenesis was suppressed by using the orally available prodrug, valganciclovir, which is enzymatically converted to ganciclovir. For initial determination of valganciclovir efficacy, rats were given 7.5 mg valganciclovir on weekdays (ie, 5 d on, 2 d off) for 8 weeks beginning at 8 weeks of age. Valganciclovir was delivered in a 0.5 g pellet of a 1:1 mixture of ground chow and peanut butter. To minimize neophobia, rats were exposed to the chow–peanut butter mixture in their home cage for several days prior to treatment. On treatment days, individual valganciclovir pellets were specifically given to each rat in the cage by hand to ensure consistent dosing. For behavioral experiments, rats were treated on weekdays for 3 weeks (at which point neurogenesis was found to be maximally reduced) and then treated only twice per week (Monday and Thursday) for the remainder of the experiment. This reduced treatment protocol effectively prevented recovery of neurogenesis while minimizing unnecessary costs and time spent treating rats. We refer to valganciclovir-treated wild-type (WT) and TK rats as v-WT and v-TK rats. These rats will be made available to other investigators through the Rat Resource and Research Center.

Immunohistochemical analyses

To determine the temporal onset of neurogenesis inhibition, WT and TK rats were treated with valganciclovir for 8 weeks, and each rat was injected once with bromodeoxyuridine (BrdU, 200 mg/kg, i.p.) on the 5th, 10th, 15th, or 20th day of valganciclovir treatment (ie, weeks 1, 2, 3, or 4). At the end of the 8 weeks, all rats were perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde and brains were collected. Coronal sections (40 µm) of the olfactory bulb (unilateral) and hippocampus (bilateral) were cut on a freezing sliding microtome and processed for immunohistochemical analyses. Briefly, to quantify adult-born neurons, a 1 in 12 series of sections through the entire dentate gyrus and entire olfactory bulb was immunostained using a mouse anti-BrdU primary antibody (BD Biosciences), biotinylated goat anti-mouse secondary antibody (Sigma-Aldrich), and peroxidase-DAB detection. Cells were counted throughout the entire series to provide stereological estimates. For assessing the phenotype of BrdU+ cells, sections were double-labeled with rat anti-BrdU (Accurate) and mouse anti-NeuN (Millipore) primary antibodies and AlexaFluor secondary antibodies. Approximately 50 BrdU+ cells were examined, and the proportion that exhibited NeuN immunoreactivity, a marker of neuronal phenotype, was multiplied by the total number of (peroxidase-DAB stained) BrdU+ cells to estimate the total number of labelled adult-born neurons in v-WT and v-TK rats. GFAP+ astrocytes and radial glial cells were identified with goat anti-GFAP antibody (Santa Cruz Biotechnology) and rabbit anti-HSV-TK antibody (a gift from GlaxoSmithKline). In a separate experiment, rats were treated with valganciclovir for 8 weeks and perfused 0, 4, or 8 weeks after stopping treatment to determine whether neurogenesis recovers. Here, immature neurons were identified by immunostaining with goat anti-doublecortin (DCX) antibody (Santa Cruz Biotechnology) and quantified from a 1 in 12 series of sections through the entire dentate gyrus.

Behavioral testing

Rats were treated with valganciclovir for 6–8 weeks before undergoing behavioral testing. Different cohorts of rats were used for each test. Open field: rats were placed in an open field (white plastic box, 50 × 50 × 50 cm) for 15 mins and the distance travelled and time spent in concentric outer, middle and center 10-cm-wide zones, measures of general locomotion and anxiety-related behavior, were calculated by Ethovision software (Noldus). To encourage exploration of the center/anxiogenic portion of the open field, a small toy was secured to the floor of the center of the open field (metal wire cylinder filled with marbles, ~ 3 cm wide × 5 cm high). Novelty-suppressed feeding: rats were food deprived for 24 h prior to testing. On the test day, rats were placed in the novel open field (the same one used for open-field testing) with five pellets of rodent chow present in the center. The latency to begin consuming the food, which is reduced by antidepressants and anxiolytic drugs (Bodnoff et al., 1988, 1989) and modulated by adult neurogenesis in mice (Santarelli et al., 2003; Snyder et al., 2011), was recorded as a measure of innate anxiety. Sucrose preference: rats were given two water bottles containing water and a 1% sucrose solution (location of each bottle counterbalanced across cages). Preference for the sucrose solution was measured by weighing the bottles before and after 4 d of constant access to the water bottles. Immediately after this measure, the water bottles were removed for 8 h, and placed back in the same location for a 10 min preference test during the early portion of the dark/active cycle (~7:00–8:00 P.M.).

Results

Driving HSV-TK expression with the human GFAP promoter led to clear TK immunostaining in GFAP+ astrocytes, as well as GFAP+ radial glial stem cells of the hippocampus (Seri et al., 2001; Fig. 1A ). Following treatment with valganciclovir (7.5 mg/d, 5 d/week), v-TK rats appeared generally healthy and gained weight, but weighed slightly (7%) less than v-WT rats after prolonged treatment (genotype × time interaction, p < 0.01; Fig. 1B ). Treatment with valganciclovir for 8 weeks did not affect numbers of GFAP+ astrocytes in the dentate gyrus molecular layer (Fig. 1C) or the appearance of the GI tract (Fig. 1D), consistent with previous findings in v-TK mice (Snyder et al., 2011), suggesting that postmitotic GFAP+ cells are not impacted by the TK transgene.

Figure 1.
The GFAP-TK rat model. A, Directing HSV-TK expression to GFAP+ cells led to TK+ radial precursor cells (arrow) and TK+ stellate astrocytes (arrowheads) in transgenic rats. B, Following valganciclovir treatment beginning at 8 weeks of age (7.5 mg/d, weekdays), ...

Effective inhibition of adult neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus

Eight-week-old WT and TK rats were treated with valganciclovir for 8 weeks and received BrdU injections to determine the extent of neurogenesis inhibition. BrdU was injected after either 1, 2, 3, or 4 weeks to determine the temporal onset of neurogenesis inhibition in TK rats, and rats were treated with valganciclovir for another 4 weeks, at which point all BrdU+ cells express strong levels of NeuN and have passed immature cell death stages (Dayer et al., 2003; Snyder et al., 2009). In v-WT rats, many BrdU+ cells were observed, and there was a modest reduction with age (Fig. 2C). In v-TK rats, however, there were significantly fewer BrdU+NeuN+ cells after 1 week of treatment (88% reduction) and even fewer of these cells at later time points (93% reduction at 2 weeks, 99% at 3 weeks, and 98% at 4 weeks; Fig. 2C ). Because BrdU acts as a pulse label and could fail to detect transient recovery of neurogenesis in TK rats (for example, on weekends when valganciclovir was not administered), we also performed immunohistochemistry for DCX, which is expressed in a large proportion of neurons born during the 4 weeks prior to death (Brown et al., 2003; Snyder et al., 2009). Qualitative analysis revealed near complete loss of DCX+ cells in v-TK brains, indicating that ablation of neurogenesis was complete (Fig. 2F ). This marker, therefore, provides a simple method for validating the genotype and valganciclovir effectiveness in each rat. After treating 8-week-old WT and TK rats with valganciclovir for 8 weeks, drug was discontinued for 4 or 8 weeks to investigate the recovery of adult neurogenesis. DCX+ cell number partially recovered after 4 weeks without valganciclovir but remained at 30% of the WT value after 8 weeks post-valganciclovir (Fig. 2F ).

Figure 2.
Inhibition of dentate gyrus neurogenesis in GFAP-TK rats. A, Timeline for inhibition experiment. B, Confocal images illustrating adult-born BrdU+NeuN+ cells in v-WT rats and their absence in v-TK rats that were injected with BrdU after 4 weeks of valganciclovir ...

Effective inhibition of adult neurogenesis in the olfactory bulb

The subventricular zone (SVZ) also contains neurogenic, GFAP+ radial glia precursors (Doetsch et al., 1999; Morshead et al., 2003; Garcia et al., 2004). These cells give rise in adulthood to neurons that migrate to the olfactory bulb, where they modify circuits and olfactory behaviors (Lepousez et al., 2013; Cummings et al., 2014). To determine whether olfactory neurogenesis was disrupted in GFAP-TK rats, we quantified BrdU+NeuN+ cells in the olfactory bulb of rats that received BrdU after 4 weeks of valganciclovir treatment and survived for an additional 4 weeks, a time point at which all BrdU-labeled cells will have exited the rostral migratory stream (Lemasson et al., 2005). v-TK rats had a 95% reduction in BrdU+NeuN+ cells in the granule cell layer of the olfactory bulb, the primary target of SVZ neurogenesis (Fig. 3). A smaller number of physiologically distinct neurons are added to other regions of the olfactory bulb. Neurogenesis in these regions was less affected in GFAP-TK rats: BrdU+NeuN+ periglomerular neurons were reduced by only 51% in v-TK rats (p = 0.002), and no significant decrease was observed in the external plexiform layer (p = 0.4; Fig. 3).

Figure 3.
Inhibition of adult neurogenesis in the olfactory bulb. A, Experimental timeline. B, Confocal images illustrating adult-born BrdU+NeuN+ cells in v-WT rats but not v-TK rats. C, Insets (left to right) show BrdU+NeuN+ neurons in the granule cell layer (GCL), ...

Normal open-field behavior in v-TK rats

Following 6 weeks of valganciclovir treatment, general activity and anxiety levels were assessed in v-WT and v-TK rats in an open-field test with a novel object in the center. Innate anxiety-related behavior was examined by quantifying time spent in concentric inner, middle and outer zones of the open field. v-WT and v-TK rats explored the open field similarly, spending the majority of the trial in the outer regions of the open field, near the wall (Fig. 4A ). v-WT and v-TK rats also traveled equivalent distances, covering progressively less territory as they habituated to the environment over the course of the 15 min test (Fig. 4A ).

Figure 4.
GFAP-TK rat behavior. A, v-WT and v-TK rats displayed similar behavior in the open field. Both genotypes spent progressively more time in the outer portions of the open field, near the walls [left graph; genotype×zone repeated-measures (RM) ANOVA; ...

Normal behavior in the novelty-suppressed feeding paradigm in v-TK rats

Adult neurogenesis in mice has been shown to regulate anxiety/depression-related behavior in the novelty-suppressed feeding test, where food-deprived mice are placed in an anxiogenic novel environment and are tested for their latency to begin consuming food. Neurogenesis does not affect baseline performance in this task, but is required for the effects of antidepressants and minimizes the effects of stress on behavior in this paradigm (Santarelli et al., 2003; Snyder et al., 2011). We therefore tested naive rats as well as rats that were exposed to a 30 min restraint session immediately prior to the test. v-WT and v-TK rats did not differ at baseline or following restraint when analyzed by ANOVA, in contrast to previous studies in mice (Snyder et al., 2011). However, restraint significantly reduced the latency to begin consuming food in both v-WT and v-TK rats, also in contrast to its effects in mice (Fig. 4B ). We also performed nonparametric analyses of the survival/distribution curves because not all of the groups passed tests of normality (specifically the restrained v-WT group). While restraint significantly shifted the distribution of feeding latencies in v-WT rats it had no significant effect on v-TK rats (Fig. 4B ; Table 1).

Table 1.
Statistical table

v-TK rats show reduced sucrose preference

To examine whether loss of adult neurogenesis leads to anhedonia-like behavior, v-WT and v-TK rats were given free access to both water and a 1% sucrose solution. Consistent with our previous findings in v-TK mice (Snyder et al., 2011), we found that v-TK rats showed reduced preference for sucrose solution. However, whereas the sucrose preference deficit in mice required switching the locations of the two solutions, v-TK rats showed reduced sucrose preference when the sucrose and water bottle locations remained constant during a 4 d habituation period and an acute 10 min preference test that followed 8 h of water deprivation (Fig. 4C ).

Discussion

We have generated a GFAP-TK rat line and found that neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus can be completely inhibited in these rats by oral treatment with valganciclovir. Blockade of neurogenesis was nearly complete within 1 week of starting treatment. If treatment was stopped after 8 weeks, neurogenesis partially recovered over the next 4 weeks but remained low after 8 weeks without valganciclovir. Generation of olfactory bulb granule cells was also completely inhibited by valganciclovir in these GFAP-TK rats. In contrast, neurogenesis in the glomerular and external plexiform layers of the olfactory bulb was only partially inhibited, suggesting that some adult-born neurons in these layers derive from a different precursor population that does not express GFAP. Initial behavioral analyses indicate that general anxiety levels and patterns of exploration are largely unaffected in neurogenesis-deficient rats. However, v-TK rats showed reduced sucrose preference and a small decrease in weight gain, suggesting effects on reward-related behaviors.

Specificity of the model

A concern with any ablation model is the possibility of nonspecific cell killing. The HSV-TK approach was designed such that viral TK expression, exogenous delivery of the nucleoside analog ganciclovir, or prodrug valganciclovir, and cell division are all required for cell toxicity. HSV-TK, unlike mammalian TK, phosphorylates ganciclovir, which becomes incorporated into DNA and stops further replication, killing the cell (Borrelli et al., 1988; Heyman et al., 1989). The drug, therefore, provides temporal specificity, whereas both the GFAP promoter and the requirement for cell division provide cell-type specificity. Although astrocytes express GFAP, they are generated during the early postnatal period (Sauvageot and Stiles, 2002; Petrik et al., 2013) and do not normally divide in adulthood (Amat et al., 1996; Burns et al., 2009). They are not, therefore, expected to be vulnerable to valganciclovir in adulthood in the GFAP-TK model, consistent with evidence from healthy GFAP-TK mice (Bush et al., 1998; Snyder et al., 2011). To test whether mature astrocytes were killed in our model, we quantified GFAP+ stellate astrocytes in the dentate gyrus of v-TK rats. We observed no loss of astrocytes in v-TK rats and comparable morphology between v-WT and v-TK rats, indicating that ablation is specific to dividing GFAP+ cells, ie, radial glial cells. It is worth noting, however, that astrocytogenesis may occur following injury and be inhibited in GFAP-TK mice (Amat et al., 1996; Bush et al., 1999; Faiz et al., 2015; but see Burns et al., 2009). Thus, the GFAP-TK rat may be a useful tool for identifying functions of adult-generated astrocytes in brain repair, though this has not yet been investigated. Finally, a previous GFAP-TK mouse model has identified an essential role for GFAP+ enteric glial cells in gastrointestinal health (Bush et al., 1998), though not all GFAP-TK mouse lines display gut-related illness upon treatment with valganciclovir (Snyder et al., 2011). We found no intestinal inflammation, hemorrhage, or other gastrointestinal pathology in v-TK rats. Although nonspecific side effects can never be completely ruled out, we find that GFAP-TK rats are healthy and generally comparable to WT littermates, making them an effective model for investigating functions of adult neurogenesis.

Behavioral phenotype of GFAP-TK rats

We observed a small but significant difference in weight gain, with lower weights in v-TK rats after long valganciclovir treatment duration. The lack of GI pathology in these animals suggests that weight differences may be a downstream effect of reducing adult neurogenesis rather than an off-target effect. Weight loss was greater at higher doses (15 mg/kg/d, data not shown) possibly reflecting an effect on other brain regions or an effect on enteric glia that only occurs at high doses. However, a specific role for adult neurogenesis is consistent with evidence for decreased weight gain in a very different ablation model, cranial irradiation (Snyder et al., 2005). A role for the hippocampus in feeding behavior is supported by evidence that hippocampal lesions impair rats’ ability to inhibit appetitive responding to formerly food-predicting cues, increasing feeding frequency in food-related contexts and leading to weight gain (Tracy et al., 2001). Furthermore, increasing activity in the dentate gyrus–CA3 network suppresses feeding behavior through projections to the lateral septum and BNST (Sweeney and Yang, 2015). If immature adult-born neurons have a net inhibitory effect (Lacefield et al., 2012; Restivo et al., 2015; Drew et al., 2016), our data are consistent with these previous findings and suggest that blocking adult neurogenesis may inhibit feeding behavior by elevating hippocampal activity.

Like decreased weight gain, our observation that v-TK rats show reduced sucrose preference is consistent with a neurogenic role in motivation to consume calories. Reduced sucrose preference is generally viewed as a measure of anhedonia, or a motivational change specific to highly pleasurable activity. However, the reduced sucrose preference in combination with small but significant weight decrease observed in v-TK rats suggests that this behavioral change may reflect general appetitive changes rather than anhedonia per se. Interestingly, unlike v-TK rats, which showed reduced sucrose preference throughout the habituation and testing periods, v-TK mice were different from v-WT mice only during testing and only when the location of the sucrose and water was switched (Snyder et al., 2011). Interestingly, in that study v-TK mice also did not weigh less than v-WT mice, suggesting that roles for neurogenesis in reward and feeding-related behaviors may be weaker, or otherwise different, in mice relative to rats.

NSF behavior in v-TK rats was markedly different from in our previous study of v-TK mice (Snyder et al., 2011). In that study, restraint stress increased latency to feed in v-TK mice and had no effect on latency in v-WT mice (Snyder et al., 2011). Here, restraint had the opposite effect and reduced latency in both v-WT and v-TK rats. This decreased latency in both rat genotypes is similar to what has been observed in mice with a 30 min post-stress delay (Snyder et al., 2012), so the differential effects seen in v-TK mice and v-TK rats may reflect differences in the complex series of stress responses across species. Because the NSF feeding latencies were not normally distributed in all groups, we also tested whether restraint altered the distribution of latencies. These analyses revealed a restraint-induced shift in the survival curves of v-WT rats but not v-TK rats (Fig. 4B ). Examining these curves, restraint uniformly shifted v-WT rats toward shorter feeding latencies. In contrast, in v-TK rats, restraint appears to reduce latencies in “early feeders” but not “late feeders”, suggesting that individual differences might dictate when neurogenesis regulates emotional behavior in rats. NSF behavior has been tested in an independently generated GFAP-TK rat, however, anxiety-like effects observed in untreated rats in this strain suggest a random gene insertion effect on anxiety that precludes interpretation of neurogenesis-dependent effects on neophagia (Groves et al., 2013).

The lack of a genotype difference in the open field in the current study is not surprising. A meta-analysis of elevated plus maze and open-field tests from 25 datasets found no evidence for an effect of adult neurogenesis in these innate anxiety tests. Meta analyses of contextual fear conditioning and spatial water maze behavior also found no significant spatial phenotype (Groves et al., 2013). Negative findings on spatial tasks, including contextual fear conditioning, spatial learning/reference memory in the water maze, spatial working memory in the radial maze, and behavioral pattern separation in the radial maze, in the Groves GFAP-TK rat also provide direct evidence, regardless of potential random insertion effects, that rats lacking adult neurogenesis can perform many spatial behaviors normally (Groves et al., 2013). However, several studies have reported spatial learning-related functions in neurogenesis-ablated animals under specific conditions (Drew et al., 2010; Burghardt et al., 2012). Given that adult neurogenesis affects emotion regulation (Revest et al., 2009; Snyder et al., 2011), it may be that differences in affective components of testing procedures contribute to this variability. Together, variability in cross-species and cross-laboratory comparisons of the effects of neurogenesis suggest that new neurons may not directly mediate anxiety, feeding, or spatial behavior but may instead alter other, as yet unidentified, cognitive processes involved in these behaviors.

Strategies for manipulating neurogenesis

Elucidating the function of adult neurogenesis will require many complementary approaches; it is clear that there is no universally ideal model and that instead each approach presents it own advantages and limitations. A longstanding approach for causally testing behavioral functions of adult neurogenesis is the ablation model. This is essentially a cell-type-specific form of lesion, and was first performed with cytotoxic agents, such as MAM, AraC, and TMZ (Seri et al., 2001; Shors et al., 2001; Garthe et al., 2009) and irradiation (Parent et al., 1999; Madsen et al., 2003; Snyder et al., 2005). Chemical approaches, and potentially radiological approaches (provided that equipment is available), have the advantages of being relatively easy to administer and cost effective. However, even if they kill only dividing cells, they have the potential to affect non-neurogenic processes such as gliogenesis or peripheral cytogenesis, affecting immune, digestive, and other systems. Although central infusions or localized irradiation can minimize these off target effects, there are concerns about the specificity of these methods (Dupret et al., 2005). As an alternative, a number of transgenic mice have been developed in which neurogenesis can be conditionally suppressed in adulthood (Imayoshi et al., 2011). These include HSV-TK models that are designed to ablate Nestin+ and GFAP+ stem cells (Garcia et al., 2004; Saxe et al., 2006; Deng et al., 2009; Singer et al., 2009; Snyder et al., 2011) or DCX+ neuronal precursors (Seo et al., 2015), and mice that are engineered to express proapoptotic genes, such as Bax (Revest et al., 2009) or diptheria toxin (Imayoshi et al., 2008), in precursor cells and/or their progeny. Because any ablation model can potentially have off-target effects, an optimal strategy may be to employ multiple distinct approaches to confirm functions for new neurons (Saxe et al., 2006; Kitamura et al., 2009; Snyder et al., 2011).

Several new genetic methods have recently been developed to provide precise temporal control of effects on new neurons. Diphtheria toxin in combination with genetic expression of diphtheria toxin receptor, can be used to ablate adult-born neurons after they mature (Arruda-Carvalho et al., 2011). Retroviral or tamoxifen-inducible expression of light-sensitive ion channels or synthetic ligand-sensitive receptors in newborn neurons allows bidirectional control of new neuron activity on the order of milliseconds to hours with the ability to target specific cohorts of new neurons and reversibility that enables within-animal comparisons (Toni et al., 2008; Gu et al., 2012; Temprana et al., 2015). Ablation models that target neuronal precursors, including the GFAP-TK rat, lack this temporal precision. However, GFAP-TK transgenic lines have the advantage of allowing large numbers of animals to be treated rapidly and non-invasively, without the use of anesthetics or surgery, providing cell-type specificity through the GFAP promoter and temporal control via administration of valganciclovir. The virtually complete elimination of adult neurogenesis in this model allows for relatively straightforward interpretation of negative results without the possibility that stronger or more widespread inhibition would be effective. Inhibition of neurogenesis in the GFAP-TK rat, as in most ablation models, is largely maintained for several weeks after drug treatment is stopped. This can be a benefit, because the ganciclovir treatment can be stopped prior to behavior testing, though it hinders the ability to test for recovery of function. Another concern with chronic ablation is that interpretations may be confounded by compensatory effects. Decreased synaptic plasticity in a nestin-TK model is observed only transiently, presumably due to delayed compensatory plasticity from more mature neurons (Singer et al., 2011), though it is not clear whether this occurs in GFAP-TK mice or rats. Compensatory effects on behavior are also apparent in studies of hippocampal function, where lesions are thought to impact retrograde memory more than anterograde memory because other brain regions can be recruited for memory formation in the absence of a functional hippocampus (Maren et al., 1997; Moser and Moser, 1998; Wiltgen et al., 2006). Nevertheless, chronic ablation may be an appropriate model for long-term reductions in adult neurogenesis due to chronic stress, depression, and other neurological disorders.

We expect that the GFAP-TK rat will be valuable in several different types of studies where technical, historical, or biological concerns make rats more advantageous than mice. Head-mounted electrode arrays and miniature microscopes are better suited to the larger heads of rats than mice, and limited MRI resolution makes their larger brain better for imaging studies. GFAP-TK rats should therefore benefit studies of the role of adult neurogenesis in hippocampal and olfactory bulb circuitry and network properties. In addition, studies of the role of neurogenesis in several behavioral and disease models that have been developed in rats, eg, addiction and stroke models, should be able to move forward more quickly in GFAP-TK rats than in equivalent mice. Species differences in adult neurogenesis also suggest that additional investigation of neurogenesis function in rats will be of value. The maturation speed and survival rate of adult-born neurons is much greater in the rat than in the mouse (Snyder et al., 2009), suggesting that rats may show stronger neurogenesis-dependent behavior than mice. This enhanced neurogenesis in rats is seen in the neocortex, as well as the hippocampus (Snyder et al., 2009), suggesting that transgenic rats may also benefit the study of adult neurogenesis in so-called “non-neurogenic” regions such as the neocortex, striatum, hypothalamus, and circumventricular organs and allow it to move forward at a faster pace (Kokoeva et al., 2007; Cameron and Dayer, 2008; Migaud et al., 2010; Ohira et al., 2013; Furube et al., 2015; Inta et al., 2015). GFAP-TK rats should be useful for determining whether new neurons in these regions are generated from GFAP-expressing stem cells, like granule neurons, or from a separate precursor population as the current findings suggest is the case for some olfactory bulb interneuron populations. Finally, several studies point to species-specific responses of new neurons to experience; for example, adult-born hippocampal neurons in rats undergo dendritic plasticity (Tronel et al., 2010; Lemaire et al., 2012) and activity-dependent cell survival (Gould et al., 1999; Dupret et al., 2007; Epp et al., 2007) in response to spatial water maze training, both of which are less apparent in mice (Trinchero et al., 2015). Although there is clear evidence that adult neurogenesis is behaviorally significant in mice, species differences suggest that comparative studies will help to clarify the function of adult-born neurons by addressing their roles in interactions with the environment from multiple angles.

Synthesis

The decision was a result of the Reviewing Editor Margaret McCarthy and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus was reached. A fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision is listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Cheryl Sisk

The major concern that must be addressed is whether the valganciclovir treatment is killing astrocytes throughout the brain as well as in the periphery, specifically the gut. The authors do not provide much discussion about potential off-target effects of the GFAP-TK plus vGCV procedure. The GFAP promoter will target not only adult neural progenitors, but also almost all astrocytes, which tile the entire CNS, as well as many other cells that express GFAP throughout the body, in peripheral nerves, in the gut and in multiple other organs. Transgenic GFAP-TK mice given high does of GCV develop a fatal gut illness (Bush et al Cell 93:189 (1998). This effect can be avoided by titrating the GCV dose. Have the present authors looked for or noticed such an effect? The authors do provide a figure on GFAP staining of hippocampal astrocytes which suggests no effect of the vGCV on them, but the authors do not explain or discuss why they are not concerned that there will be effects on astrocytes during their chronic treatment regimens, and that effects on astrocytes might impact on functional outcomes. Astrocytes are thought to be largely non-proliferative in healthy CNS, but the authors do not explain this or cite evidence for it. We ask that you thoroughly address this issue or provide additional data that speaks to it.

The second major concern focuses on the timing and dose of administration and how specific regimes were decided on. It will be useful to those seeking to adapt this model to understand thoroughly all of the twist and turns taken by the authors in establishing this important model. The authors seem to have adopted on/off delivery regimens of vGCV without explaining why. How did they arrive at their delivery regimen that starts out at 5 of 7 days per week and then shifts to 2 of 7 days per week? Why did they do this? Were there problems with continual dosing? Given that the vGCV is in the food, changing the food seems more work than simply leaving it. Changing the food also seems like it might lead to inconsistent dosing.

A third important question - would it be a good idea to include a statement in the manuscript somewhere (or maybe as a footnote) that these rats will be made available to other researchers (as is indicated by the authors in the submission form?

Specific comments

1. Line 349: The authors should consider adding Bennett et al Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience 41: 337-47 2009 to the list of references supporting adult neurogenesis in "non-neurogenic" regions. This paper provides good evidence for neurogenic niches in the circumventricular organs. They might also add Migaud et al European Journal of Neuroscience 32:2042-52 2010, which identifies the third ventricular region of the hypothalamus as a neurogenic niche (as does the already cited Kokoeva paper).

2. Figure 1A. I am not seeing the arrowhead that points to the TK+ stellate astrocytes. It may be missing or too small or obscured.

3. Figure 2B. Please specify which time point the images are from. I could not find a specific reference to 2B in the text, so it's not clear whether this information is given in the results section.

4. Figure 4 caption, line 572. Do the authors mean to say that TK rats consumed less (not more) sucrose? I'm looking at the middle graph in Figure 4C. And does this sentence indeed refer to the middle graph? If so, then that should be specifically indicated, as it has been for the left and right graphs in the figure.

5. Statistical table: should the header for the right-most column be p value, not power? Maybe eNeuro instructs authors to call p values power; if so, that doesn't sound right to me.

6. Typo line 66: Herpes Simplex, not Simples

7. Line 119: delete "of" after "after"

8. Line 195: delete "and" after environment (or insert "are" are mice in line 194)

References

  • Abbott A (2004) Laboratory animals: the Renaissance rat. Nature 428:464–466. 10.1038/428464a [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • al-Shawi R, Burke J, Wallace H, Jones C, Harrison S, Buxton D, Maley S, Chandley A, Bishop JO (1991) The herpes simplex virus type 1 thymidine kinase is expressed in the testes of transgenic mice under the control of a cryptic promoter. Mol Cell Biol 11:4207–4216. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Amat JA, Ishiguro H, Nakamura K, Norton WT (1996) Phenotypic diversity and kinetics of proliferating microglia and astrocytes following cortical stab wounds. Glia 16:368–382. 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1136(199604)16:4&amp;lt;368::AID-GLIA9&amp;gt;3.0.CO;2-W [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Arruda-Carvalho M, Sakaguchi M, Akers KG, Josselyn SA, Frankland PW (2011) Posttraining ablation of adult-generated neurons degrades previously acquired memories. J Neurosci 31:15113–15127. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3432-11.2011 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Bodnoff SR, Suranyi-Cadotte B, Aitken DH, Quirion R, Meaney MJ (1988) The effects of chronic antidepressant treatment in an animal model of anxiety. Psychopharmacology 95:298–302. 10.1007/BF00181937 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Bodnoff SR, Suranyi-Cadotte B, Quirion R, Meaney MJ (1989) A comparison of the effects of diazepam versus several typical and atypical anti-depressant drugs in an animal model of anxiety. Psychopharmacology 97:277–279. 10.1007/BF00442264 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Borrelli E, Heyman R, Hsi M, Evans RM (1988) Targeting of an inducible toxic phenotype in animal cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 85:7572–7576. [PubMed]
  • Borrelli E, Heyman RA, Arias C, Sawchenko PE, Evans RM (1989) Transgenic mice with inducible dwarfism. Nature 339:538–541. 10.1038/339538a0 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Braun RE, Lo D, Pinkert CA, Widera G, Flavell RA, Palmiter RD, Brinster RL (1990) Infertility in male transgenic mice: disruption of sperm development by HSV-tk expression in postmeiotic germ cells. Biol Reprod 43:684–693. [PubMed]
  • Brown JP, Couillard-Després S, Cooper-Kuhn CM, Winkler J, Aigner L, Kuhn HG (2003) Transient expression of doublecortin during adult neurogenesis. J Comp Neurol 467:1–10. 10.1002/cne.10874 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Burghardt NS, Park EH, Hen R, Fenton AA (2012) Adult-born hippocampal neurons promote cognitive flexibility in mice. Hippocampus 22:1795–1808. 10.1002/hipo.22013 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Burns KA, Murphy B, Danzer SC, Kuan CY (2009) Developmental and post-injury cortical gliogenesis: a genetic fate-mapping study with Nestin-CreER mice. Glia 57:1115–1129. 10.1002/glia.20835 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Bush TG, Puvanachandra N, Horner CH, Polito A, Ostenfeld T, Svendsen CN, Mucke L, Johnson MH, Sofroniew MV (1999) Leukocyte infiltration, neuronal degeneration, and neurite outgrowth after ablation of scar-forming, reactive astrocytes in adult transgenic mice. Neuron 23:297–308. [PubMed]
  • Bush TG, Savidge TC, Freeman TC, Cox HJ, Campbell EA, Mucke L, Johnson MH, Sofroniew MV (1998) Fulminant jejuno-ileitis following ablation of enteric glia in adult transgenic mice. Cell 93:189–201. [PubMed]
  • Cameron HA, Dayer AG (2008) New interneurons in the adult neocortex: small, sparse, but significant? Biol Psychiatry 63:650–655. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.09.023 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Cameron HA, Glover LR (2015) Adult neurogenesis: beyond learning and memory. Annu Rev Psychol 66:53–81. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015006 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Cohen RM, Rezai-Zadeh K, Weitz TM, Rentsendorj A, Gate D, Spivak I, Bholat Y, Vasilevko V, Glabe CG, Breunig JJ, Rakic P, Davtyan H, Agadjanyan MG, Kepe V, Barrio JR, Bannykh S, Szekely CA, Pechnick RN, Town T (2013) A transgenic Alzheimer rat with plaques, tau pathology, behavioral impairment, oligomeric Aβ, and frank neuronal loss. J Neurosci 33:6245–6256. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3672-12.2013 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Cummings DM, Snyder JS, Brewer M, Cameron HA, Belluscio L (2014) Adult neurogenesis is necessary to refine and maintain circuit specificity. J Neurosci 34:13801–13810. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2463-14.2014 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Dayer AG, Ford AA, Cleaver KM, Yassaee M, Cameron HA (2003) Short-term and long-term survival of new neurons in the rat dentate gyrus. J Comp Neurol 460:563–572. 10.1002/cne.10675 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Delaney CL, Brenner M, Messing A (1996) Conditional ablation of cerebellar astrocytes in postnatal transgenic mice. J Neurosci 16:6908–6918. [PubMed]
  • Deng W, Saxe MD, Gallina IS, Gage FH (2009) Adult-born hippocampal dentate granule cells undergoing maturation modulate learning and memory in the brain. J Neurosci 29:13532–13542. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3362-09.2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Doetsch F, Caillé I, Lim DA, García-Verdugo JM, Alvarez-Buylla A (1999) Subventricular zone astrocytes are neural stem cells in the adult mammalian brain. Cell 97:703–716. [PubMed]
  • Drew LJ, Kheirbek MA, Luna VM, Denny CA, Cloidt MA, Wu MV, Jain S, Scharfman HE, Hen R (2016) Activation of local inhibitory circuits in the dentate gyrus by adult-born neurons. Hippocampus 26:763–778. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Drew MR, Denny CA, Hen R (2010) Arrest of adult hippocampal neurogenesis in mice impairs single- but not multiple-trial contextual fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci 124:446–454. 10.1037/a0020081 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Dupret D, Fabre A, Döbrössy MD, Panatier A, Rodríguez JJ, Lamarque S, Lemaire V, Oliet SHR, Piazza PV, Abrous DN (2007) Spatial learning depends on both the addition and removal of new hippocampal neurons. PLoS Biol 5:e214. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050214 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Dupret D, Montaron MF, Drapeau E, Aurousseau C, Le Moal M, Piazza PV, Abrous DN (2005) Methylazoxymethanol acetate does not fully block cell genesis in the young and aged dentate gyrus. Eur J Neurosci 22:778–783. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04262.x [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Elion GB, Furman PA, Fyfe JA, de Miranda P, Beauchamp L, Schaeffer HJ (1977) Selectivity of action of an antiherpetic agent, 9-(2-hydroxyethoxymethyl) guanine. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 74:5716–5720. 10.1073/pnas.74.12.5716 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Epp JR, Spritzer MD, Galea LAM (2007) Hippocampus-dependent learning promotes survival of new neurons in the dentate gyrus at a specific time during cell maturation. Neuroscience 149:273–285. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.07.046 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Faiz M, Sachewsky N, Gascón S, Bang KWA, Morshead CM, Nagy A (2015) Adult neural stem cells from the subventricular zone give rise to reactive astrocytes in the cortex after stroke. Cell Stem Cell 17:624–634. 10.1016/j.stem.2015.08.002 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Furman PA, McGuirt PV, Keller PM, Fyfe JA, Elion GB (1980) Inhibition by acyclovir of cell growth and DNA synthesis of cells biochemically transformed with herpesvirus genetic information. Virology 102:420–430. 10.1016/0042-6822(80)90109-9 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Furube E, Morita M, Miyata S (2015) Characterization of neural stem cells and their progeny in the sensory circumventricular organs of adult mouse. Cell Tissue Res 362:347–365. 10.1007/s00441-015-2201-0 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Garcia ADR, Doan NB, Imura T, Bush TG, Sofroniew MV (2004) GFAP-expressing progenitors are the principal source of constitutive neurogenesis in adult mouse forebrain. Nat Neurosci 7:1233–1241. 10.1038/nn1340 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Garthe A, Behr J, Kempermann G (2009) Adult-generated hippocampal neurons allow the flexible use of spatially precise learning strategies. PLoS One 4:e5464. 10.1371/journal.pone.0005464 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Gould E, Beylin A, Tanapat P, Reeves A, Shors TJ (1999) Learning enhances adult neurogenesis in the hippocampal formation. Nat Neurosci 2:260–265. 10.1038/6365 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Groves JO, Leslie I, Huang GJ, McHugh SB, Taylor A, Mott R, Munafò M, Bannerman DM, Flint J (2013) Ablating adult neurogenesis in the rat has no effect on spatial processing: evidence from a novel pharmacogenetic model. PLoS Genet 9:e1003718. 10.1371/journal.pgen.1003718 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Gu Y, Arruda-Carvalho M, Wang J, Janoschka SR, Josselyn SA, Frankland PW, Ge S (2012) Optical controlling reveals time-dependent roles for adult-born dentate granule cells. Nat Neurosci 15:1700–1706. 10.1038/nn.3260 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Heyman RA, Borrelli E, Lesley J, Anderson D, Richman DD, Baird SM, Hyman R, Evans RM (1989) Thymidine kinase obliteration: creation of transgenic mice with controlled immune deficiency. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 86:2698–2702. [PubMed]
  • Imayoshi I, Sakamoto M, Kageyama R (2011) Genetic methods to identify and manipulate newly born neurons in the adult brain. Front Neurosci 5:64. 10.3389/fnins.2011.00064 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Imayoshi I, Sakamoto M, Ohtsuka T, Takao K, Miyakawa T, Yamaguchi M, Mori K, Ikeda T, Itohara S, Kageyama R (2008) Roles of continuous neurogenesis in the structural and functional integrity of the adult forebrain. Nat Neurosci 11:1153–1161. 10.1038/nn.2185 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Inta D, Cameron HA, Gass P (2015) New neurons in the adult striatum: from rodents to humans. Trends Neurosci 38:517–523. 10.1016/j.tins.2015.07.005 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Jessberger S, Clark RE, Broadbent NJ, Clemenson GD, Consiglio A, Lie DC, Squire LR, Gage FH (2009) Dentate gyrus-specific knockdown of adult neurogenesis impairs spatial and object recognition memory in adult rats. Learn Mem 16:147–154. 10.1101/lm.1172609 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Kentros CG, Agnihotri NT, Streater S, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER (2004) Increased attention to spatial context increases both place field stability and spatial memory. Neuron 42:283–295. [PubMed]
  • Kitamura T, Saitoh Y, Takashima N, Murayama A, Niibori Y, Ageta H, Sekiguchi M, Sugiyama H, Inokuchi K (2009) Adult neurogenesis modulates the hippocampus-dependent period of associative fear memory. Cell 139:814–827. 10.1016/j.cell.2009.10.020 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Kokoeva MV, Yin H, Flier JS (2007) Evidence for constitutive neural cell proliferation in the adult murine hypothalamus. J Comp Neurol 505:209–220. 10.1002/cne.21492 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Lacefield CO, Itskov V, Reardon T, Hen R, Gordon JA (2012) Effects of adult-generated granule cells on coordinated network activity in the dentate gyrus. Hippocampus 22:106–116. 10.1002/hipo.20860 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Lemaire V, Tronel S, Montaron MF, Fabre A, Dugast E, Abrous DN (2012) Long-lasting plasticity of hippocampal adult-born neurons. J Neurosci 32:3101–3108. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4731-11.2012 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Lemasson M, Saghatelyan A, Olivo-Marin JC, Lledo PM (2005) Neonatal and adult neurogenesis provide two distinct populations of newborn neurons to the mouse olfactory bulb. J Neurosci 25:6816–6825. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1114-05.2005 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Lepousez G, Valley MT, Lledo PM (2013) The impact of adult neurogenesis on olfactory bulb circuits and computations. Annu Rev Physiol 75:339–363. 10.1146/annurev-physiol-030212-183731 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Lodge DJ, Grace AA (2011) Hippocampal dysregulation of dopamine system function and the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Trends Pharmacol Sci 32:507–513. 10.1016/j.tips.2011.05.001 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Madsen TM, Kristjansen PEG, Bolwig TG, Wörtwein G (2003) Arrested neuronal proliferation and impaired hippocampal function following fractionated brain irradiation in the adult rat. Neuroscience 119:635–642. 10.1016/S0306-4522(03)00199-4 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Maren S, Aharonov G, Fanselow MS (1997) Neurotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus and Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Behav Brain Res 88:261–274. [PubMed]
  • Migaud M, Batailler M, Segura S, Duittoz A, Franceschini I, Pillon D (2010) Emerging new sites for adult neurogenesis in the mammalian brain: a comparative study between the hypothalamus and the classical neurogenic zones. Eur J Neurosci 32:2042–2052. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07521.x [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Morshead CM, Garcia AD, Sofroniew MV, van Der Kooy D (2003) The ablation of glial fibrillary acidic protein-positive cells from the adult central nervous system results in the loss of forebrain neural stem cells but not retinal stem cells. Eur J Neurosci 18:76–84. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02727.x [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Moser EI, Kropff E, Moser MB (2008) Place cells, grid cells, and the brain's spatial representation system. Annu Rev Neurosci 31:69–89. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.061307.090723 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Moser MB, Moser EI (1998) Distributed encoding and retrieval of spatial memory in the hippocampus. J Neurosci 18:7535–7542. [PubMed]
  • Ohira K, Takeuchi R, Shoji H, Miyakawa T (2013) Fluoxetine-induced cortical adult neurogenesis. Neuropsychopharmacology 38:909–920. 10.1038/npp.2013.2 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Parent JM, Tada E, Fike JR, Lowenstein DH (1999) Inhibition of dentate granule cell neurogenesis with brain irradiation does not prevent seizure-induced mossy fiber synaptic reorganization in the rat. J Neurosci 19:4508–4519. [PubMed]
  • Petrik D, Yun S, Latchney SE, Kamrudin S, LeBlanc JA, Bibb JA, Eisch AJ (2013) Early postnatal in vivo gliogenesis from nestin-lineage progenitors requires cdk5. PLoS One 8:e72819. 10.1371/journal.pone.0072819 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Restivo L, Niibori Y, Mercaldo V, Josselyn SA, Frankland PW (2015) Development of Adult-Generated Cell Connectivity with Excitatory and Inhibitory Cell Populations in the Hippocampus. J Neurosci 35:10600–10612. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3238-14.2015 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Revest JM, Dupret D, Koehl M, Funk-Reiter C, Grosjean N, Piazza PV, Abrous DN (2009) Adult hippocampal neurogenesis is involved in anxiety-related behaviors. Mol Psychiatry 14:959–967. 10.1038/mp.2009.15 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Santarelli L, Saxe M, Gross C, Surget A, Battaglia F, Dulawa S, Weisstaub N, Lee J, Duman R, Arancio O, Belzung C, Hen R (2003) Requirement of hippocampal neurogenesis for the behavioral effects of antidepressants. Science 301:805–809. 10.1126/science.1083328 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Sauvageot CM, Stiles CD (2002) Molecular mechanisms controlling cortical gliogenesis. Curr Opin Neurobiol 12:244–249. [PubMed]
  • Saxe MD, Battaglia F, Wang JW, Malleret G, David DJ, Monckton JE, Garcia ADR, Sofroniew MV, Kandel ER, Santarelli L, Hen R, Drew MR (2006) Ablation of hippocampal neurogenesis impairs contextual fear conditioning and synaptic plasticity in the dentate gyrus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:17501–17506. 10.1073/pnas.0607207103 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Seo DO, Carillo MA, Chih-Hsiung Lim S, Tanaka KF, Drew MR (2015) Adult hippocampal neurogenesis modulates fear learning through associative and nonassociative mechanisms. J Neurosci 35:11330–11345. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0483-15.2015 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Seri B, García-Verdugo JM, McEwen BS, Alvarez-Buylla A (2001) Astrocytes give rise to new neurons in the adult mammalian hippocampus. J Neurosci 21:7153–7160. [PubMed]
  • Shors TJ, Miesegaes G, Beylin A, Zhao M, Rydel T, Gould E (2001) Neurogenesis in the adult is involved in the formation of trace memories. Nature 410:372–376. 10.1038/35066584 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Singer BH, Gamelli AE, Fuller CL, Temme SJ, Parent JM, Murphy GG (2011) Compensatory network changes in the dentate gyrus restore long-term potentiation following ablation of neurogenesis in young-adult mice. Proc Nat Acad Sci U S A 108:5437–5442. 10.1073/pnas.1015425108 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Singer BH, Jutkiewicz EM, Fuller CL, Lichtenwalner RJ, Zhang H, Velander AJ, Li X, Gnegy ME, Burant CF, Parent JM (2009) Conditional ablation and recovery of forebrain neurogenesis in the mouse. J Comp Neurol 514:567–582. 10.1002/cne.22052 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Snyder JS, Choe JS, Clifford MA, Jeurling SI, Hurley P, Brown A, Kamhi JF, Cameron HA (2009) Adult-born hippocampal neurons are more numerous, faster maturing, and more involved in behavior in rats than in mice. J Neurosci 29:14484–14495. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1768-09.2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Snyder JS, Hong NS, McDonald RJ, Wojtowicz JM (2005) A role for adult neurogenesis in spatial long-term memory. Neuroscience 130:843–852. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.10.009 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Snyder JS, Soumier A, Brewer M, Pickel J, Cameron HA (2011) Adult hippocampal neurogenesis buffers stress responses and depressive behaviour. Nature 476:458–461. 10.1038/nature10287 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Snyder JS, Soumier A, Cameron HA (2012) Stress can increase or decrease anxiety depending on the timing of the stressor. Figshare. . 10.6084/m9.figshare.89656 [Cross Ref]
  • Sweeney P, Yang Y (2015) An excitatory ventral hippocampus to lateral septum circuit that suppresses feeding. Nat Commun 6:10188. 10.1038/ncomms10188 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Tada E, Parent JM, Lowenstein DH, Fike JR (2000) X-irradiation causes a prolonged reduction in cell proliferation in the dentate gyrus of adult rats. Neuroscience 99:33–41. [PubMed]
  • Temprana SG, Mongiat LA, Yang SM, Trinchero MF, Alvarez DD, Kropff E, Giacomini D, Beltramone N, Lanuza GM, Schinder AF (2015) Delayed coupling to feedback inhibition during a critical period for the integration of adult-born granule cells. Neuron 85:116–130. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.11.023 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Toni N, Laplagne DA, Zhao C, Lombardi G, Ribak CE, Gage FH, Schinder AF (2008) Neurons born in the adult dentate gyrus form functional synapses with target cells. Nat Neurosci 11:901–907. 10.1038/nn.2156 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Tracy AL, Jarrard LE, Davidson TL (2001) The hippocampus and motivation revisited: appetite and activity. Behav Brain Res 127:13–23. [PubMed]
  • Trinchero MF, Koehl M, Bechakra M, Delage P, Charrier V, Grosjean N, Ladeveze E, Schinder AF, Abrous DN (2015) Effects of spaced learning in the water maze on development of dentate granule cells generated in adult mice. Hippocampus 25:1314–1326. 10.1002/hipo.22438 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Tronel S, Fabre A, Charrier V, Oliet SHR, Gage FH, Abrous DN (2010) Spatial learning sculpts the dendritic arbor of adult-born hippocampal neurons. Proc Nat Acad Sci U S A 107:7963–7968. 10.1073/pnas.0914613107 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Wiltgen BJ, Sanders MJ, Anagnostaras SG, Sage JR, Fanselow MS (2006) Context fear learning in the absence of the hippocampus. J Neurosci 26:5484–5491. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2685-05.2006 [PubMed] [Cross Ref]

Articles from eNeuro are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience