PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptHHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
Neurotoxicology. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 May 1.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC4875812
NIHMSID: NIHMS771161

Developmental Stress and Lead (Pb): Effects of Maternal Separation and/or Pb on Corticosterone, Monoamines, and Blood Pb in Rats

Abstract

The level of lead (Pb) exposure in children has decreased dramatically since restrictions on its use were implemented. However, even with restrictions, children are exposed to Pb and still present with cognitive and behavioral deficits. One prominent aspect of the exposome of these children is that many come from low social economic status (SES) conditions, and low SES is associated with stress. In order to compare the combined effects of early stress and Pb, Sprague-Dawley rats were exposed to vehicle or Pb either alone or in combination with maternal separation stress during brain development (i.e., postnatal day (P)4 to P11, 19, or P28). Maternally separated/isolated pups had lower body and thymus weights during exposure and had increased levels of blood Pb compared with vehicle controls. Isolation, but not Pb, affected the response to an acute stressor (standing in shallow water) when assessed on P19 and P29, but not earlier on P11. Interactions of Pb and isolation were found on monoamines in the neostriatum, hippocampus, and hypothalamus on turnover but not on levels, and most changes were on dopamine turnover. Isolation had greater short-term effects than Pb. Interactions were dependent on age, sex, and acute stress.

Keywords: Lead (Pb), maternal separation, acute shallow water stress, corticosterone, dopamine, serotonin, development, rat

1. Introduction

Lead (Pb) continues to be an environmental health risk despite restrictions on its use. Children are especially susceptible to Pb exposure (Lidsky and Schneider, 2003), and this exposure is associated with altered cardiovascular function (Gump et al., 2005), encephalopathy (Patel and Athawale, 2009), genotoxicity (Mendez-Gomez et al., 2008), and cognitive and behavioral deficits, the latter including but not limited to decreased IQ (Chiodo et al., 2007;Needleman et al., 1979;Surkan et al., 2007;Wang et al., 1989;Winneke et al., 1985), altered language function (Yuan et al., 2006), social problems (Chiodo et al., 2007;Burns et al., 1999), and increased juvenile adjudication (Dietrich et al., 2001). Even levels of Pb below the Centers for Disease Control’s 10 μg/dL level of concern (CDC, 2005) result in neuropsychological and IQ reductions (Neal and Guilarte, 2010;Bellinger, 2011). Because of the persistence of Pb in the environment, it continues to be found in children, albeit at low levels (Bellinger, 2008;Lanphear et al., 2000). However, these levels have not been modeled often in animals and represent a gap in understanding how such levels affect brain development.

Pb exposure also has effects on stress mechanisms (Gump et al., 2005;Gump et al., 2008). Interactions between Pb and stress have been reported in children (Tong et al., 2000) and in animals (Weston et al., 2014;Cory-Slechta et al., 2013b;Cory-Slechta et al., 2010;Cory-Slechta et al., 2012;Cory-Slechta et al., 2009;Cory-Slechta et al., 2013a;Rossi-George et al., 2009;Rossi-George et al., 2011;Virgolini et al., 2006;Virgolini et al., 2008b;Virgolini et al., 2008a;Cory-Slechta et al., 2004). The latter are from a laboratory primarily using a paradigm in which female Long-Evans rats are given Pb (50 or 150 ppm in drinking water) for 2 months prior to mating, throughout gestation, and up to weaning or through adulthood. Restraint stress in this model is to the dam and consists of three 45 min episodes given on two days of gestation: embryonic (E) days 15 and 16 (where evidence of mating is counted as E0) and may be characterized as an acute stress paradigm. This model, and its variations, show a number of interactions in which the effects of Pb are increased by the prenatal stress exposure. However, this model does not address the effects of chronic developmental stress.

Chronic stress can cause neurotoxicity (McEwen and Stellar, 1993;McEwen, 1998) and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis dysfunction (Lupien et al., 1999;McEwen et al., 1992). In young children and rodents, the HPA axis passes through a stage of reduced responsiveness to stress (stress hyporesponsive period; SHRP). The SHRP is hypothesized to be a neuroprotective mechanism to prevent excitotoxicity when glucocorticoid receptors are developing (De Kloet et al., 1988;Sapolsky and Meaney, 1986). Stressors that exceed the buffering capacity of the SHRP result in adverse consequences (Anisman et al., 1998;Gos et al., 2008;Gruss et al., 2008).

A number of stressors may interfere with child developmental markers, one of which is low socioeconomic status (SES). Children in such settings face a variety of difficulties that may include impoverishment, family disruption, overcrowding, poor diet, threats, violence, and other challenges that result in elevated cortisol levels and other markers of stress (Gump et al., 2005;Lupien et al., 2000;Lupien et al., 2001). These conditions can co-occur when children live in older housing that tend to have higher levels of Pb paint, dust, and or soil contamination (Goyer, 1996;Jacobs et al., 2002;Lanphear et al., 1998;Levin et al., 2008;Meyer et al., 2003;Muntner et al., 2005;Schnaas et al., 2004).

In the present study, we tested the interaction of Pb exposure in combination with the chronic stressor of maternal separation (pup isolation; ISO). We hypothesized that the combination would be more detrimental than either factor alone. ISO has been used in many studies. It consists of isolating pups from the dam and littermates during a period that spans the SHRP (Kosten and Kehoe, 2005). The ISO model alters stress markers (McCormick et al., 1998), brain monoamines (Kehoe et al., 1998;Kosten et al., 2004), fear conditioning, and DNA methylation (Kao et al., 2012). Here, offspring were separated on P4 for 4 h/day until the day prior to assessment on P11, P19, or P29. These days were chosen to match a previous experiment of similar design but using barren-cage stress instead of maternal separation (Graham et al., 2011). The P4–28 period of brain development in rats is approximately analogous to late gestation to early childhood in humans (Bayer et al., 1993;Clancy et al., 2007a;Clancy et al., 2007b). In combination with ISO, pups were treated every other day with 1 or 10 mg/kg of Pb acetate by gavage from P4–10, P4–18, or P4–28. The day after the last treatment, pups were challenged with an acute stressor or left undisturbed. Pb doses used here were designed to produce blood Pb (BPb) concentrations similar to those observed in humans (Lanphear et al., 2000;Bellinger, 2008). The effects of chronic stress and Pb were assessed on brain monoamines, organ weights associated with stress and immunity, and corticosterone. The ultimate purpose was to develop a combination model suitable for studies on long-term behavioral effects and test whether maternal separation induces effects similar to or different from barren cage rearing as used in our previous experiment (Graham et al., 2011).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1 Animals

Male and nulliparous female Sprague-Dawley Crl:CD (IGS) rats (strain 001, Charles River Laboratories, Raleigh, NC) were acclimated for at least 1–4 weeks in the vivarium prior to breeding. The vivarium is accredited by the Association for the Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care, and care was provided in accordance with the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. Animals were maintained on a 14:10 h light:dark cycle (lights on at 600 h) with controlled temperature (19 ± 1 °C) and humidity (50% ± 10%). To control exposure to other metals in the diet, animals were maintained on an NIH-07 diet throughout the experiment in which metal content was certified by assay to be below preset values (PMI Corp., Lab Diet Auto 5018). Food and water were given ad libitum. For breeding, a male and female were cohabitated until the presence of a sperm plug was detected (E0). On E1, females were transferred to polycarbonate cages (46 × 24 × 20 cm) containing woodchip bedding and housed singly. Day of birth was designated P0, and on P1 litters were culled to 12 pups (6 males and 6 females) using a random number table. If litters had less than 12 pups on P1, 1 or 2 pups from another litter born on the same day were added to attain a uniform litter size of 12; this occurred in 17 litters (17.7%) of the 96 litters in the experiment. Of the 17 litters that had pups added, 13 received 1 pup (13.5%) and 4 received 2 pups (4.2%). The experiment was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Personnel conducting the study were blind to the group membership of the pups.

2.2 Pb Exposure and Rearing Conditions

The experiment had five independent variables: 2 rearing conditions, 3 exposure groups, 3 test ages, 2 levels of the acute stressor, and 2 sexes. The rearing conditions were maternal separation or no maternal separation. The three exposure groups were 0, 1, or 10 mg/kg Pb acetate. The three ages were P11, 19, or 29. The four levels of the acute stressor were no Shallow Water Stress (SWS) and 0, 30, and 60 min after SWS. The two sexes were males and females. Hence, the study design was a 2 × 3 × 3 × 4 × 2 design, the experiment had 144 cells, hence 96 litters × 12 offspring per litter = 1152 pups; 1152 pups divided by 144 cells resulted in 8 males and 8 females in each cell of the study stratified by litter. The dependent variables were body and organ weights, monoamines, and corticosterone.

Pb administration and ISO began on P4. Pb was given in a vehicle of 0.01 M anhydrous sodium acetate; VEH). Two male and female pairs per litter were gavaged with 0, 1, or 10 mg/kg Pb acetate (1Pb and 10Pb, respectively) in a volume of 3 mL/kg VEH as in our earlier experiment, and in that experiment we also showed that gavaging per se did not increase plasma corticosterone (Graham et al., 2011). Rats were gavaged every other day from P4 until P10, 18, or 28. Offspring assessed on P29 had their dams removed from the cage on P28 at which time the weanlings were housed by sex, 3 rats/cage (1 from each exposure group).

From P4 until assessment, ISO offspring were removed from their dam and isolated individually in a quiet room for 4 h in a small clean paper container with lid (14 cm height × 13 cm diameter at the bottom and 16 cm diameter at the top). Control animals remained with their dam throughout this period (Standard rearing: STD).

On P11, P19, or P29 separate subsets within each group were exposed to an acute stressor (shallow water stressor, SWS) or left undisturbed. The SWS animals were placed for 30 min in clean cages (28 × 16 × 12 cm polycarbonate cages) filled with room temperature water to a depth of 2 cm for P11, 3 cm for P19, and 4 cm for P29 animals. During the middle of the light cycle (approximately 1000–1400 h), one pup from each condition at each age was decapitated 0, 30, or 60 min after removal from SWS or directly upon removal from their home cage (baseline). Trunk blood was collected in 12 × 75 mm polyethylene tubes containing 0.05 mL of 2% EDTA for corticosterone assay. An additional blood sample was taken from animals on P29 for BPb analysis. Monoamine determinations were from rats not subjected to SWS. The neostriatum, hypothalamus, and hippocampus were dissected over ice using a brain block (Zivic-Miller, Pittsburgh, PA) as described (Grace et al., 2010). Spleen, thymus, and adrenals were removed and weighed. Body weights were obtained before euthanasia. Samples were stored at −80 °C until assayed.

2.3 Blood Pb

BPb was measured by anodonic stripping voltammetry using an ESA Lead Analyzer 3010B (Chelmsford, MA). Whole blood (100 μL) was mixed with Metexchange® reagent, and the integration set point was −480 mV. The limit of detection was 1 μg/dL; readings below this level were assigned this value (a total of 29 samples: 28 from the control group and 1 from the 1 mg/kg Pb group). Because of the small volume of blood in younger animals, corticosterone and BPb analyses were only performed on P29 rats that did not undergo SWS. Values are reported in μg/dL.

2.4 Corticosterone

Blood was centrifuged at 4 °C for 25 min at 1300 RCF. Plasma was diluted 3:1 in buffer and assayed for corticosterone using a commercially available EIA kit (Immunodiagnostic Systems Inc., Fountain Hills, AZ). The limit of detection was 0.55 ng/mL; samples that fell below this (two samples) were assigned this value. Each age × sex × Pb exposure × ISO × time after stress group had sample sizes of N = 8. Values are reported in ng/mL.

2.5 Monoamines

Monoamines were assessed by high performance liquid chromatography with electrochemical detection (HPLC-ECD). Frozen tissues were weighed, thawed, and sonicated in appropriate volumes of 0.1 N perchloric acid (Fisher Scientific, Pittsburgh, PA). Samples were centrifuged for 14 min at 13,000 RCF at 4 °C. The supernatant sample was transferred to a new vial for injection on a Supelco Supelcosil LC-18 column (150 × 4.6 mm, 3 μm; Sigma-Aldrich Co., St. Louis, MO). The HPLC system consisted of a Waters 717plus autosampler (Waters Corp., Milford, MA), ESA 584 pump (ESA, Inc., Chelmsford, MA), and ESA Coulochem III electrochemical detector. The potential settings were −150 mV for E1 and +250 mV for E2, with a guard cell potential of +350 mV. MD-TM mobile phase (ESA, Inc.) was used and consisted of 75 mM sodium dihydrogen phosphate (monohydrate), 1.7 mM 1-octanesulfonic acid sodium, 100 μL/L triethylamine, 25 μM EDTA, and 10% acetonitrile, with a final pH of 3.0. The pump flow rate was set at 0.7 mL/min, and the samples were run at 28 °C. Standards for dopamine (DA), 3, 4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC), homovanillic acid (HVA), norepinephrine (NE), serotonin (5-HT), and 5-hydroxyindolacetic acid (5-HIAA) (Sigma-Aldrich Co., St. Louis, MO) were prepared in 0.1 N perchloric acid. All monoamines were run on a single chromatogram. Each age × sex × Pb exposure × ISO group had a final sample size of N = 4–8. Values are reported in pg/mg tissue wet weight.

2.6 Statistical Analysis

Data were analyzed using mixed linear factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA; Proc Mixed, SAS v9.2, SAS Institute, Cary, NC) except for body weight. Factors in Mixed models were exposure (Pb dose), sex, developmental stressor (ISO vs STD housing), and time following the acute stressor (SWS). Each age was analyzed separately. In order to control for litter statistically, it was included in models as a randomized block factor. Significant interactions were further analyzed using slice-effect ANOVAs. Body weight was analyzed using general linear model ANOVA (Proc GLM, SAS) because before SWS pups were not subdivided and therefore sex was a repeated measure. Because there were only two levels of sex and developmental stressor, no a posteriori pairwise comparisons were need when these were significant. Where there were three groups, as for Pb, slice-effect ANOVAs were used with Pdiff used within Mixed model ANOVAs for pairwise comparisons. This is equivalent to an LSD and does not distort alpha when only 3 groups are compared. Mortality was analyzed using Fisher’s test for uncorrelated proportions. Significance was set at p ≤ 0.05. Data are presented as least square mean ± SEM for Mixed analyses and ordinary mean ± SEM for GLM data. Because data were analyzed by mixed model ANOVA, SEMs are used in figures for purposes of inference. Because of the complexity of the ANOVA models we show only those main effects and interactions that pertain to the objectives of the study, i.e., Pb, ISO, or their combination and any interactions of Pb, ISO, or their combination with sex that were present. Other effects, such as main effects of sex, time, or interactions of secondary factors (such as time × sex, for example) are generally not presented graphically.

3. Results

3.1 Mortality

Twenty out of the 96 litters in the study had at least one death. This resulted in a total of 29 deaths out of 1152 pups (2.5%). There was no sex difference among the 29 pups that died, however, 55% of the deaths were in the 10Pb group. This resulted in some cells of the study having a total of only 6 or 7 females or males per cell rather than 8.

3.2 Body weight

Pup weight was recorded on P1, P4, and every other day until the day of tissue collection. Using the group that was tested on P29, the ISO groups had decreased body weight (main effect: F(1,1065)=6.67, p<0.01) compared with STD groups, regardless of Pb (Fig. 1A). Pb had no independent effect on pup weight or any interaction with ISO.

Fig. 1
Effects of ISO on Body and Thymus Weights

3.3 Adrenal weight

Adrenal weights were unaffected by Pb or ISO at P11, P19, or P29. However, at P29, there was a Pb × ISO interaction, F(2,73)=2.67, p<0.05, in which ISO Pb pups (1Pb and 10Pb) had lighter adrenal weights than ISO VEH animals (p<0.03; VEH: 24 ± 2 μg vs. 1Pb: 19 ± 2 μg and 10Pb: 18 ± 2 μg, respectively). Males had heavier adrenals than females at P29, (F(1,73)=4.16, p<0.05). When evaluated as a percentage of body weight, there were no effects of Pb, ISO (Table 1), or their interactions.

Table 1
Absolute organ weight ratios for adrenals and spleen and thymus weights (mg) as a function of rearing condition and age

3.4 Spleen weight

Spleen weights were decreased by ISO compared with STD at P11 (F(1,93)=4.15, p<0.05; Table 1), and female spleens were lighter than males (F(1,93)=16.86, p<0.0001). A significant sex × Pb × ISO interaction showed that 10Pb ISO males had lighter spleens than STD 10Pb males at this age (F(1,93)=2.57, p<0.02). At P19, a significant Pb × ISO interaction (F(2,74)=4.82, p<0.01) showed that among ISO pups, spleen weights in 10Pb animals were decreased compared with 1Pb, but did not differ compared with STD Pb animals (ISO 10Pb = 0.17 ± 0.03 mg vs. ISO 1Pb = 0.19 ± 0.03 mg vs. STD = 0.17 ± 0.03 mg). At P29, female spleens were lighter than males (F(1,25)=4.43, p<0.05), however there were no effects of Pb or ISO. As a percentage of body weight, the only effect was at P11 and was an ISO main effect (F(1,30)=7.62, p<0.01) in which spleen to body weight ratios of all ISO groups were smaller than in STD groups (Table 1).

3.5 Thymus weight

ISO pups had decreased thymus weights, regardless of age or Pb, compared with STD pups (P11: F(1,55)=6.67, p<0.01; P19: F(1,64)=31.03, p<0.0001; P29: F(1,73)=5.96, p<0.05), Table 1). When expressed as a percentage of body weight, the reduction in ISO animals remained significant at P11 and P19, but not at P29 (Fig. 1B).

3.6 Blood Pb (BPb)

Pb exposure increased BPb at P29 (F(2,72)=68.31, p<0.0001). Both the 1Pb and 10Pb animals had increased BPb levels compared with VEH animals (1Pb, p<0.05; 10Pb, p<0.0001, Fig. 2). BPb in the 10Pb group was significantly higher than in the 1Pb group (p<0.0001).

Fig. 2
Blood Pb Levels

ISO animals had increased BPb levels (F(1,72)=12.3, p<0.0008) as a function of Pb dose (ISO × Pb interaction. F(2,72)=7.49, p<0.001). As shown in Fig. 2, Pb-exposed ISO animals had increased BPb levels in the 10Pb group compared with 10Pb STD animals (p<0.0001).

3.7 Corticosterone

At P11, corticosterone was significantly altered by sex (F(1,308)=6.00, p<0.05; females had higher levels) and SWS (F(3,308)=9.17, p<0.0001). Corticosterone levels were highest immediately after SWS but were unaffected by ISO or Pb (Fig. 3A). At P19, corticosterone was affected by time after SWS (F(3,319)=52.40, p<0.0001) and there was an interaction of ISO and time after SWS (F(3,319)=4.77, p<0.01, Fig. 3B). ISO animals had increased levels of corticosterone compared with STD animals immediately after the SWS, but not at later intervals. There was no effect of Pb at this age. At P29, there was an effect of time after SWS (F(3,330)=180.80, p<0.0001) and an interaction of ISO with post-SWS time (F(3,330)=2.91, p<0.05, Fig. 3C). Basal levels were similar among groups, but ISO animals had a blunted response immediately after SWS at this age. Sex interacted with time after SWS (F(3,330)=4.60, p<0.01); females had higher corticosterone immediately after SWS than males. As with P11 and P19 animals, there was no effect of Pb.

Fig. 3
Plasma corticosterone before and after acute stress (SWS)

3.8 Monoamines

3.8.1 Neostriatum

At P11, neither Pb nor ISO had main effects on monoamine or metabolite concentrations; however, there were interactions. There was a P11 sex × Pb interaction for DOPAC/DA (F(2,59.1)=3.52, p<0.04). By slice ANOVA the effect was only in females (p<0.04), but there was a trend in males (p<0.10). The STD 10Pb females had increased turnover compared with STD VEH females and ISO 10Pb males had reduced turnover compared with STD VEH males (Fig. 4A). For HVA/DA, there was a sex × Pb (F(2,59.2)=4.14, p<0.03) and sex × Pb × ISO interaction at P11 (F(2,59.2)=3.58, p<0.04, Fig. 4B). STD 10Pb females had increased HVA/DA ratios compared with STD VEH females. In males, HVA/DA ratios were decreased in ISO 1Pb and 10Pb and in STD 10Pb compared with STD VEH males. There was a similar pattern in total metabolite turnover (DOPAC+HVA)/DA; there was a sex × Pb (F(2,59.1)=4.01, p<0.03) and sex × Pb × ISO interaction (F(2,59.1)=3.37, p<0.05, Fig. 4C) in which STD 10Pb females had higher turnover compared with STD VEH females. In males ISO VEH, 1Pb, and 10Pb and STD 10Pb males had decreased turnover compared with STD VEH males. The 5-HIAA/5-HT ratio was also decreased in all ISO groups compared with STD groups (310.0 ± 12.3 vs. STD: 373.0 ± 12.3, F(1,60)=13.09, p<0.01).

Fig. 4
Effects of ISO and Pb on P11 DA

At P19, neither Pb nor ISO had main effects on DA or NE. There was a sex × ISO interaction for HVA/DA (F(1,68.2)=3.88, p≤0.05). ISO males had reduced turnover compared with STD males regardless of Pb exposure (ISO males 35.0 ± 7.1 vs. STD males 58.0 ± 7.0). 5-HT was higher in females compared with males (F(1,68)=4.52, p<0.05) but there were no interactions with Pb or ISO. For NE there was an interaction of sex × ISO (F(1,29.3)=5.49, p<0.03); STD males had lower NE than STD females (females =102.6 ± 20.2 pg/mg vs. males = 54.9 ± 19.7 pg/mg) and NE was decreased in ISO females compared with STD females (ISO females 49.5 ± 20.1 vs. females =102.6 ± 20.2 pg/mg).

At P29, there were no main effects for DA, NE, or 5-HT. There was a sex × Pb × ISO interaction (F(2,68.2)=3.79, p<0.05) for 5-HIAA/5-HT ratios. ISO 1Pb males had a decreased ratio compared with ISO VEH males and STD 1Pb males (ISO 1Pb: 247.9 ± 27.6 vs. ISO VEH: 278.7 ± 27.6, p<0.05 and STD 1Pb: 356.5 ± 27.6, p<0.01).

3.8.2 Hypothalamus

At P11, there were no main effects of Pb or ISO on DA, NE, or 5-HT. DOPAC had an ISO × Pb interaction (F(2,60)=5.29, p<0.01): in ISO pups 10Pb animals had decreased levels compared with VEH animals (88.4 ± 20.4 pg/mg vs. 129.4 ± 20.4 pg/mg), but not 1Pb animals (103.1 ± 20.4 pg/mg). Pb × ISO interacted on HVA levels (F(2,54)=3.99, p<0.05), such that ISO 10Pb pups (142.8 ± 23.4 pg/mg) and ISO 1Pb (143.1 ± 23.8 pg/mg) pups had decreased levels compared with ISO VEH animals (187.3 ± 23.7 pg/mg), but there were no differences in STD animals. A sex × Pb interaction was found for 5-HIAA/5-HT (F(2,59.1)=5,85, p<0.01), such that 10Pb males had decreased ratios compared with 1Pb and VEH males (179.8 ± 21.0 vs. 228.3 ± 21.0, p<0.03 and 234.8 ± 21.5, p<0.05, respectively) with no effects in females.

At P19, there were no main effects on DA, NE, or 5-HT. There was a Pb × ISO interaction on DA (F(2,66.6)= 6.68, p<0.01). By slice ANOVA the effect was only significant for the STD condition (p<0.03), not for ISO. The STD 1Pb group had increased DA compared with the STD VEH group (STD VEH 205.9 ± 29.9 vs. STD 1Pb 299.5 ± 30.9). There was a Pb × ISO interaction on DOPAC/DA (F(2,66.3)=3.6, p<0.04) but further analyses failed to show any meaningful group differences. The DOPAC+HVA/DA ratio showed an ISO main effect (F(1,14)=5.6, p<0.04) and a Pb × ISO interaction (F(2,63.4)=4.57, p<0.02). ISO decreased the ratio compared with STD regardless of Pb exposure (75.5 ± 11.3 vs. 113.2 ± 11.2) and the interaction showed the ratio was decreased in 10Pb and 1Pb groups in relation to the STD VEH group (VEH: 135.3 ± 13.6 vs. 10Pb: 106.5 ± 13.6, p<0.04 and 1Pb: 97.7 ± 13.3, p<0.005, respectively).

At P29 there were no changes in hypothalamic DA or NE. 5-HT showed interactions of sex × ISO (F(1,57.4)=5.96, p<0.02) and sex × Pb (F(2,55.1)=3.82, p<0.03). In STD animals, females had decreased 5-HT compared with males (females: 423.4 ± 60.6 pg/mg vs. males: 534.9 ± 58.8, p<0.03), an effect not seen in ISO pups (females: 464.4 ± 55.8 pg/mg vs. males: 411.9 ± 52.9 pg/mg). The sex × Pb interaction showed that 10Pb females had decreased 5-HT compared with 10Pb males (females: 423.4 ± 60.6 pg/mg vs. males: 534.9 ± 58.8 pg/mg, p<0.03). 10Pb males also had increased 5-HT compared with males in the 1Pb and VEH groups (569.5 ± 50.6 pg/mg vs. 421.0 ± 50.6 pg/mg, p<0.01 and 429.5 ± 50.6 pg/mg, p<0.01, respectively).

3.8.3 Hippocampus

At P11, there were no main effects on DA, NE, or 5-HT. There was a main effect of Pb treatment on HVA (F(2,53.2)=4.04, p<0.03) in which 10Pb animals had increased HVA compared with 1Pb and VEH groups (146.7 ± 10.7 pg/mg vs. 114.3 ± 10.9 pg/mg, p<0.04 and 106.3 ± 10.7 pg/mg, p<0.01, respectively). A sex × ISO interaction (F(1,53.2)=6.26, p<0.02) showed that ISO females had increased HVA compared with STD females and ISO males (153.6 ± 12.8 pg/mg vs. 103.2 ± 11.8 pg/mg, p<0.05 and 110.8 ± 12.8 pg/mg, p<0.02, respectively). 5-HT and 5-HIAA were unaffected by ISO (Fig. 5A and B, respectively). 5-HIAA showed a sex main effect (F(1,59.1)=8.32, p<0.01) and a sex × Pb interaction (F(2,59.1)=3.20, p<0.05). Males had lower 5-HIAA levels compared with females. 5-HIAA levels in 10Pb males were lower than in 1Pb and VEH males (576.9 ± 42.4 pg/mg vs. 719.0 ± 43.6 pg/mg, p<0.004 and 673.2 ± 42.4 pg/mg, p<0.04, respectively). Turnover ratios were mostly unchanged, except the 5-HIAA/5-HT ratio for ISO on P11 (F(1,12)=5.12, p<0.05) where the ISO groups had lower turnover than STD groups (Fig. 5C).

Fig. 5
Effects of ISO on hippocampal 5-HT

At P19 there were no main effects on DA, NE, 5-HT, or 5-HIAA (Fig. 5A&B). For NE there was a sex × Pb interaction (F(2,65.7)=3.97, p<0.03). 10Pb females had increased NE compared with 1Pb and VEH females (441.7 ± 27.0 pg/mg vs. 376.2 ± 27.0 pg/mg, p<0.03 and 353.3 ± 26.4 pg/mg, p<0.004, respectively) and compared with 10Pb males (363.0 ± 27.7 pg/mg, p<0.05). The 5-HIAA/5-HT ratio was decreased in all P19 ISO groups compared with all P19 STD groups (F(1,13.8)=5.09, p<0.05: Fig. 5C).

At P29, there were no main effects on DA, NE, or 5-HT. There was a Pb × ISO interaction on 5-HT (F(2,54)=4.99, p≤0.01). In ISO animals, 1Pb pups had increased 5-HT compared with 10Pb and VEH pups (655.4 ± 86.1 pg/mg vs. 462.7 ± 87.5 pg/mg, p<0.01 and 511.5 ± 86.1 pg/mg, p<0.05, respectively). 5-HIAA showed a sex × ISO interaction (F(1,53.9)=4.17, p<0.05): ISO females had increased 5-HIAA compared with males (918.0 ± 76.0 pg/mg vs. 768.0 ± 76.0 pg/mg, p<0.02), with no difference among the STD groups. A Pb × ISO interaction was significant for 5-HIAA/5-HT (F(2,54.1)=3.48, p<0.04): STD 1Pb animals had lower ratios compared with other STD groups (168.7 ± 36.7 vs. SAL: 223.5 ± 36.7 and 10Pb: 237.5 ± 37.5). No difference was found for 5-HIAA/5-HT ratios between the STD and ISO groups at P29 (Fig. 5C).

4. Discussion

Pb continues to pose risks to children despite mandated decreases in exposure. In children, the primary risk is to neurocognitive and behavioral development. Pb exposure is not uniform and is higher in children living in low SES conditions. Children living in low SES settings often live in older neighborhoods where housing, soil, and plumbing may contain elevated levels of Pb. Low SES may also be used as a marker for conditions associated with a variety of stressors. Hence, Pb and stress may interact to increase the effect of each when they co-occur. Hence, our hypothesis was that developmental stress, as modeled by maternal separation, would interact with developmental Pb exposure to increase effects compared with either factor alone on organ weights, corticosterone, and brain monoamines.

One of the objectives of this experiment was to determine if maternal separation produced more or larger interactions with Pb than another developmental stressor that we had used previously, barren cage rearing. While these two developmental stressors are different we wanted to determine if one had a greater effect in combination with Pb on corticosterone, selected organ weights, and monoamines. Our intent was only to make this comparison in a global sense since it was impractical to use both stessors in a single experiment. Therefore, the comparison is a relative one focusing on areas of consistency or difference.

We previously tested the same doses of Pb in combination with barren-cage rearing in an experimental design identical to the present one (Graham et al., 2011). We hypothesized that maternal separation stress would induce greater interactions than Pb and barren-cage rearing. We found that neither barren-cage nor maternal separation stress interacted with Pb on adrenal or splenic weights, nor did they interact with Pb to affect thymic weight but both developmental stressors reduced thymic weights independently of Pb exposure. Barren-cage rearing reduced thymus weights only on P19 whereas maternal separation reduced thymus weight on P11 and P19 (see Fig. 1B).

For corticosterone, neither barren-cage rearing nor maternal separation interacted with Pb, but both induced changes independent of Pb. The pattern was somewhat different in response to the acute stressor inasmuch as barren cage reduced basal corticosterone on P11 (but not on P19 or P29) and reduced the rise in corticosterone 60 min after SWS. However, on P19, barren-cage induced a greater increase in corticosterone 0, 30, and 60 min after SWS. No changes were found on P29. Maternal separation had no effects on corticosterone at P11, increased the response to SWS at P19, and reduced the SWS increase in corticosterone at P29. Collectively, these data do not support the hypothesis that maternal separation was more stressful than barren-cage rearing on basal corticosterone or on corticosterone in response to an acute stress (SWS).

In terms of neurotransmitters, the models showed similarities and differences. Barren-cage increased hypothalamic NE (males) and 5-HT (males and females) at P19 but not at P11 or P29. Pb exposure increased hypothalamic NE and 5-HIAA in the 1Pb but not the 10Pb group at P29 in females. Maternal separation had no effects on hypothalamic monoamines, but reduced DA turnover in the striatum; 10Pb also reduced DA turnover in the striatum but did not interact with the stressor. In the hippocampus, barren-cage rearing interacted with 1Pb to increase 5-HT at P29 whereas 10Pb reduced 5-HT at P19 in females but did not interact with the stressor. Maternal separation reduced 5-HT utilization in the hippocampus at P11 and P19 but did not interact with Pb; Pb itself had no effect on 5-HT. Based on these outcomes, we conclude that there is no evidence that maternal separation is more stressful than barren-cage rearing on brain monoamines

Overall, our findings are in broad agreement with data reported by Cory-Slechta et al. in which Pb is given in drinking water during development at relatively high doses (see below) alone or in combination with two-days of prenatal restraint stress. Restraint in these experiments was given on gestational days (G)16 and G17 three times, 45 min each time, spaced 3 h apart (where G1 = embryonic day E0, hence G16–17 is the same as E15–16). In these studies a number of Pb and prenatal stress effects were reported on behavior, corticosterone, and monoamines, but relatively few stress × Pb interactions were found (Cory-Slechta et al., 2004;Virgolini et al., 2008a;Cory-Slechta et al., 2009;Rossi-George et al., 2009;Cory-Slechta et al., 2010;Rossi-George et al., 2011;Cory-Slechta et al., 2012;Cory-Slechta et al., 2013b;Cory-Slechta et al., 2013a;Weston et al., 2014). In the first study, interactions between prenatal restraint stress and monoamines were found in the high dose group Pb Group (150 ppm/day Pb) on frontal cortex DA utilization (DA/HVA but not DA/DOPAC) and in nucleus accumbens (increased DOPAC and HVA) and reduced 5-HT (Cory-Slechta et al., 2004). However, in a follow-up experiment using a similar design the effects were only in the frontal cortex in groups given prenatal and postnatal stress combined with Pb and were on NE rather than DA or 5-HT (Virgolini et al., 2008a). Effects of Pb and stress on monoamines were essentially absent in the next experiment of the same design with the same doses of Pb and the same prenatal stress paradigm (Cory-Slechta et al., 2009). In the next experiment, using 50 ppm/day Pb and the same stressor, there were also almost no effects on monoamines in the frontal cortex, striatum, or nucleus accumbens, but this time monoamines were measured in midbrain and hypothalamus and Pb × stress interactions were obtained. However, the interactions were not seen in the stressed-Pb group but in the Pb non-stressed and stressed-no Pb groups for 5-HT and 5-HT utilization; there were no effects on DA or NE (Cory-Slechta et al., 2010). In a further study using the same model with both 50 and 150 ppm/day Pb doses, a few interactions on brain monoamines were seen but not in frontal cortex. Three interactions were found in the nucleus accumbens and one in the striatum. Of those in the nucleus accumbens, DOPAC was reduced in the 50 ppm Pb-stress group but not in the 150 ppm Pb-stress group; DA utilization was increased in the no Pb-prenatal stress group; and NE was increased in the no Pb-prenatal stress group. The interaction in the striatum was in the 150 ppm Pb no stress group in which DOPAC was higher than the no Pb no stress control group (Cory-Slechta et al., 2012). In the next experiment using the same model but only the 50 ppm Pb dose, no interactions were found in the frontal cortex on monoamines, but reductions in the Pb-stress group were found in frontal cortex glutamate and GABA (not measured in previous studies) and in midbrain 5-HT and 5-HIAA. However, in all these latter instances the effects were hemisphere-specific, being found on only one side. Moreover, these effects were mostly in Pb groups, regardless of stress, or in stress groups regardless of Pb, very few were found in Pb-stress combination groups (Cory-Slechta et al., 2013a). In the most recent report using the same model and the 50 ppm Pb exposure with or without the same prenatal restraint stress, almost no effects were found on monoamines in the frontal cortex, nucleus accumbens, striatum, hypothalamus, or midbrain. In this experiment brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), NMDA-NR2A, and 5-HT transporter (SERT) were examined in frontal cortex, nucleus accumbens, hippocampus, and hypothalamus. There were no Pb-stress interactions on BDNF in any brain region, but there were Pb main effects (reduced BDNF in the frontal cortex) and Pb and stress main effects in the hippocampus. For NMDA-NR2A receptors, there were Pb main effects in frontal cortex and hypothalamus (reductions) but no interactions with stress. For SERT there were Pb-stress interactions in the frontal cortex and hippocampus. Pb without stress increased SERT in the frontal cortex. In the hippocampus, Pb and stress increased SERT relative to the stress no Pb and Pb no stress groups, although these groups had decreased SERT compared with control. Interestingly, all of the Pb-stress interactions in these studies were not sorted statistically by a posteriori methods, but shown as significant interactions with no pairwise group comparisons. Hence, while these experiments provide some limited evidence of interactions between prenatal stress and Pb, the effects are modest with limited consistency across experiments, not unlike what we report in this and our previous Pb-stress experiment. In the studies by Cory-Slechta et al. most of the interactions were found on behavior rather than on neurotransmitters. We have not assessed behavior using our Pb and two stress models.

Maternal separation in our ISO groups reduced body and organ weights and this has not been reported (Knuth and Etgen, 2005;Knuth and Etgen, 2007), but the effects found here replicate our findings using the Pb + barren cage combination (Graham et al., 2011). Chronic stress, whether in neonatal or adult rats is known to decrease thymus weight (Tuchscherer et al., 2002;Felszeghy et al., 2000;Bakker et al., 1995). Decreases in thymus weight suggests immune disruption but we did not measure other immune markers, but this should be done in future experiments.

BPb concentrations were dose-dependent. However, ISO 10Pb pups had higher BPb compared with STD 10Pb rats at P29. The fact that stress-induced heightened BPb above that seen in non-stressed Pb exposed rats has not been reported (Graham et al., 2011;Virgolini et al., 2008a;Virgolini et al., 2008b). If similar effects occur in children, this could be important in understanding why some children have higher BPb levels than others even of the same SES level. The mechanism of how ISO stress interacts to increase BPb is unknown but stress mobilizes energy reserves, such as glycogen and lipids to generate more glucose, and this may cause Pb stored in lipids to be released faster than in non-stressed Pb-exposed animals.

ISO animals showed similar levels of basal corticosterone, but altered responses to acute stress (SWS) compared with STD animals. ISO animals exhibited exaggerated responses to SWS at P19 but not at P29. The differential P19 responses were transient and disappeared 30 min after SWS exposure.

For neostriatum, DA turnover was decreased in ISO pups at P11, but not at later ages. STD females had increased NE compared with males at P19, a sexually dimorphic effect eliminated by ISO. A related effect was seen in the hypothalamus: ISO decreased male 5-HT levels.

ISO also affected hippocampal 5-HT turnover. ISO animals had decreased 5-HT turnover on P11 and P19, but not on P29. Chronic stress and elevated corticosterone are known to affect hippocampal development in other models (Ivy et al., 2010;Brunson et al., 2003;Gilles et al., 1996) and this may be why hippocampal 5-HT turnover was affected.

As noted, neurotransmitter effects of Pb differed from our previous study using barren cage rearing. One noteworthy difference was that ISO altered Pb levels whereas barren cage rearing did not. Since the aim of this experiment was to test for interactions of Pb and ISO, it is of interest that most of the ISO*Pb interactions were reductions in DA turnover. There were also ISO*Pb*sex interactions and most of these were in the neostriatum and hypothalamus and also involved reduced DA turnover preferentially in one sex, although in a few cases an increase was observed such as in the female STD-10Pb group. Hence, DA appears to be the most sensitive monoamine to Pb-stress interactions.

Limitations of the study include the use of only two doses of Pb. A greater range might have shown more effects. To model current levels of Pb found in children, we used doses of Pb lower than those used in many studies. While the use of lower doses has relevance to humans, detecting effects at these levels is more difficult. Interestingly, if one compares administered versus internal doses of Pb after developmental exposure in rats compared with non-human primates, species differences are apparent. While comparisons across studies cannot be precise, as a first approximation, we compared data from two laboratories that have published extensively on the developmental effects of Pb exposure. In rats we focused on studies from the laboratory of Cory-Slechta (Cory-Slechta et al., 2004;Virgolini et al., 2008a;Rossi-George et al., 2011;Cory-Slechta et al., 2009;Rossi-George et al., 2009;Cory-Slechta et al., 2010;Rossi-George et al., 2011;Cory-Slechta et al., 2012;Cory-Slechta et al., 2013b;Cory-Slechta et al., 2013a;Weston et al., 2014) and for non-human primates on studies from the laboratory of Rice (Rice et al., 1979;Rice, 1984;Rice and Gilbert, 1985;Truelove et al., 1985;Rice, 1985;Gilbert and Rice, 1987;Rice, 1988b;Rice and Karpinski, 1988;Rice, 1988a;Reuhl et al., 1989;Rice and Gilbert, 1990b;Rice and Gilbert, 1990a;Rice, 1990;Rice and Willes, 1979;Rice, 1992c;Rice, 1992a;Rice, 1992b;Rice, 1992a;Foster et al., 1993;Rice, 1993;Foster et al., 1996b;Foster et al., 1996a;Rice, 1997;Rice, 1998;Rice, 1996b;Rice, 1996a). Rice expresses doses as μg/kg/day given once orally per day in monkey infant formula. Cory-Slechta expresses doses in rats as ppm/day in drinking water. In order to compare these it is first necessary to estimate how much water rats drank/day. Using water consumption data from the literature, adult SD rats (males or females) consume 80–100 mL/day (Slone et al., 2012;McGivern et al., 1996). Cory-Slectha gives 50 or 150 ppm/day in drinking water which is the same as 50 and 150 μg/mL/day. Using the lower end of the drinking volume range of 80 mL/day, 50 μg/mL means an adult rat consumes 50 × 80 or 4000 μg/day. Assuming a body weight of 320 g, the resulting dose would be 4000 × 0.32 = 1280 μg/kg/day. Similarly, the higher dose would be 150 μg/mL/day × 80 mL/day which is 150 × 80 or 12000 μg/day; again using a body weight of 320 g, this amounts to 12000 × 0.32 = 3840 μg/kg as the dose. We can then compare what the doses given to monkeys and rats produce in terms of BPb concentrations. Cory-Slechta (Cory-Slechta et al., 2009) reports that the 50 ppm/day (i.e., 1280 μg/kg/day) dose results in BPb = 12–20 μg/dL and the higher dose of 150 ppm/day (i.e., 3840 μg/kg/day) results in BPb = 25–36 μg/dL. Rice reports four doses (50, 100, 500, and 2000 μg/kg/day). The 50 μg/kg/day dose results in BPb = ~10 μg/dL; the 100 μg/kg/day dose results in BPb = ~13 μg/dL; 500 μg/kg/day dose results in BPb = ~19 μg/dL; and 2000 μg/kg/day dose results in BPb = ~26 μg/dL (with a sharp rise in BPb when the animals get older). In the present study if we express our doses of 1 and 10 mg/kg in micrograms, we gave 1000 and 10000 μg/kg/every other day which averages out to 500 and 5000 μg/kg/day by gavage. These doses resulted in BPb values in the unstressed group of 3 and 10 μg/dL, respectively, values in the range of BPb levels found in Cory-Slechta’s studies. Comparing the rat and monkey doses relative to BPb concentrations shows that rats must be given 1–2 orders of magnitude higher doses to obtain BPb levels comparable with non-human primates. While only approximate, this comparison suggests that rats are less sensitive than non-human primates based on administered dose. There are also differences in internal dose in terms of effect, but this is difficult to compare because the outcomes measured in non-human primates and rats are very different. Nevertheless, it may be that had we given doses that produce BPb concentrations similar to the higher end of Cory-Slechta’s range of 12–36 μg/dL, perhaps we would have seen larger effects. That rats are less sensitive to neurotoxins than primates is not unique to Pb. For example, rats are at least an order of magnitude less sensitive to the prenatal effects of ethanol on brain development compared with primates (Gohlke et al., 2008), suggesting that this may be a more general phenomenon.

We hypothesized that Pb and maternal separation stress would exacerbate the effects of Pb on corticosterone (basal and following acute stress (SWS)), monoamines, and organ weights. The data showed such effects, but there is no easily overriding pattern and the interactions were not large. Furthermore, the interactions were often with age, dose, and sex and more on dopaminergic markers of turnover. The data suggest that monoamine metabolism (turnover) may be more sensitive to Pb and ISO compared with levels of the neurotransmitter itself. Children exposed to Pb are often exposed to stressors, hence, interaction models may be useful for better understanding how Pb affects brain development but it is not yet clear what the optimal stress model is.

Highlights

  • Effects of neonatal Pb, maternal separation and the combination were evaluated
  • Pups were evaluated on postnatal day 11, 19, and 28
  • Early stress-Pb interactions were present but complex
  • Maternal separation, Pb, age, sex, and acute stress interacted on neurotransmitters
  • Maternal separation reduced body and thymus weight and corticosterone after shallow water stress

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by NIH grant ES015689 (MTW) and training grant T32 ES007051 (RMA-K, DLG, CEG, AAB, and TLS).

Footnotes

Conflict Statement

The authors declare no conflicts of interest

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • Anisman H, Zaharia MD, Meaney MJ, Merali Z. Do early-life events permanently alter behavioral and hormonal responses to stressors? Int J Dev Neurosci. 1998;16:149–164. [PubMed]
  • Bakker JM, Schmidt ED, Kroes H, Kavelaars A, Heijnen CJ, Tilders FJ, van Rees EP. Effects of short-term dexamethasone treatment during pregnancy on the development of the immune system and the hypothalamo-pituitary adrenal axis in the rat. J Neuroimmunol. 1995;63:183–191. [PubMed]
  • Bayer SA, Altman J, Russo RJ, Zhang X. Timetables of neurogenesis in the human brain based on experimentally determined patterns in the rat. Neurotoxicology. 1993;14:83–144. [PubMed]
  • Bellinger DC. Very low lead exposures and children’s neurodevelopment. Curr Opin Pediatr. 2008;20:172–177. [PubMed]
  • Bellinger DC. The protean toxicities of lead: new chapters in a familiar story. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2011;8:2593–2628. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Brunson KL, Chen Y, Avishai-Eliner S, Baram TZ. Stress and the developing hippocampus: a double-edged sword? Mol Neurobiol. 2003;27:121–136. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Burns JM, Baghurst PA, Sawyer MG, McMichael AJ, Tong SL. Lifetime low-level exposure to environmental lead and children’s emotional and behavioral development at ages 11–13 years. The Port Pirie Cohort Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1999;149:740–749. [PubMed]
  • CDC. Preventing lead poisoning in young children: A statement of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services; 2005. pp. 1–137.
  • Chiodo LM, Covington C, Sokol RJ, Hannigan JH, Jannise J, Ager J, Greenwald M, Delaney-Black V. Blood lead levels and specific attention effects in young children. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2007;29:538–546. [PubMed]
  • Clancy B, Finlay BL, Darlington RB, Anand KJ. Extrapolating brain development from experimental species to humans. Neurotoxicology. 2007a;28:931–937. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Clancy B, Kersh B, Hyde J, Darlington RB, Anand KJ, Finlay BL. Web-based method for translating neurodevelopment from laboratory species to humans. Neuroinformatics. 2007b;5:79–94. [PubMed]
  • Cory-Slechta DA, Merchant-Borna K, Allen JL, Liu S, Weston D, Conrad K. Variations in the nature of behavioral experience can differentially alter the consequences of developmental exposures to lead, prenatal stress, and the combination. Toxicol Sci. 2013a;131:194–205. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Cory-Slechta DA, Stern S, Weston D, Allen JL, Liu S. Enhanced learning deficits in female rats following lifetime pb exposure combined with prenatal stress. Toxicol Sci. 2010;117:427–438. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Cory-Slechta DA, Virgolini MB, Liu S, Weston D. Enhanced stimulus sequence-dependent repeated learning in male offspring after prenatal stress alone or in conjunction with lead exposure. Neurotoxicology. 2012;33:1188–1202. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Cory-Slechta DA, Virgolini MB, Rossi-George A, Weston D, Thiruchelvam M. Experimental manipulations blunt time-induced changes in brain monoamine levels and completely reverse stress, but not Pb+/− stress-related modifications to these trajectories. Behav Brain Res. 2009;205:76–87. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Cory-Slechta DA, Virgolini MB, Thiruchelvam M, Weston DD, Bauter MR. Maternal stress modulates the effects of developmental lead exposure. Environ Health Perspect. 2004;112:717–730. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Cory-Slechta DA, Weston D, Liu S, Allen JL. Brain hemispheric differences in the neurochemical effects of lead, prenatal stress, and the combination and their amelioration by behavioral experience. Toxicol Sci. 2013b;132:419–430. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • De Kloet ER, Rosenfeld P, Van Eekelen JA, Sutanto W, Levine S. Stress, glucocorticoids and development. Prog Brain Res. 1988;73:101–120. [PubMed]
  • Dietrich KN, Ris MD, Succop PA, Berger OG, Bornschein RL. Early exposure to lead and juvenile delinquency. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2001;23:511–518. [PubMed]
  • Felszeghy K, Bagdy G, Nyakas C. Blunted pituitary-adrenocortical stress response in adult rats following neonatal dexamethasone treatment. J Neuroendocrinol. 2000;12:1014–1021. [PubMed]
  • Foster WG, McMahon A, Rice DC. Sperm chromatin structure is altered in cynomolgus monkeys with environmentally relevant blood lead levels. Toxicol Ind Health. 1996a;12:723–735. [PubMed]
  • Foster WG, McMahon A, Rice DC. Subclinical changes in luteal function in cynomolgus monkeys with moderate blood lead levels. J Appl Toxicol. 1996b;16:159–163. [PubMed]
  • Foster WG, McMahon A, YoungLai EV, Hughes EG, Rice DC. Reproductive endocrine effects of chronic lead exposure in the male cynomolgus monkey. Reprod Toxicol. 1993;7:203–209. [PubMed]
  • Gilbert SG, Rice DC. Low-level lifetime lead exposure produces behavioral toxicity (spatial discrimination reversal) in adult monkeys. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 1987;91:484–490. [PubMed]
  • Gilles EE, Schultz L, Baram TZ. Abnormal corticosterone regulation in an immature rat model of continuous chronic stress. Pediatr Neurol. 1996;15:114–119. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Gohlke JM, Griffith WC, Faustman EM. Computational models of ethanol-induced neurodevelopmental toxicity across species: Implications for risk assessment. Birth Defects Res B Dev Reprod Toxicol. 2008;83:1–11. [PubMed]
  • Gos T, Bock J, Poeggel G, Braun K. Stress-induced synaptic changes in the rat anterior cingulate cortex are dependent on endocrine developmental time windows. Synapse. 2008;62:229–232. [PubMed]
  • Goyer RA. Results of lead research: prenatal exposure and neurological consequences. Environ Health Perspect. 1996;104:1050–1054. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Grace CE, Schaefer TL, Graham DL, Skelton MR, Williams MT, Vorhees CV. Effects of inhibiting neonatal methamphetamine-induced corticosterone release in rats by adrenal autotransplantation on later learning, memory, and plasma corticosterone levels. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2010;28:331–342. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Graham DL, Grace CE, Braun AA, Schaefer TL, Skelton MR, Tang PH, Vorhees CV, Williams MT. Effects of developmental stress and lead (Pb) on corticosterone after chronic and acute stress, brain monoamines, and blood Pb levels in rats. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2011;29:45–55. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Gruss M, Braun K, Frey JU, Korz V. Maternal separation during a specific postnatal time window prevents reinforcement of hippocampal long-term potentiation in adolescent rats. Neuroscience. 2008;152:1–7. [PubMed]
  • Gump BB, Stewart P, Reihman J, Lonky E, Darvill T, Matthews KA, Parsons PJ. Prenatal and early childhood blood lead levels and cardiovascular functioning in 9(1/2) year old children. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2005;27:655–665. [PubMed]
  • Gump BB, Stewart P, Reihman J, Lonky E, Darvill T, Parsons PJ, Granger DA. Low-level prenatal and postnatal blood lead exposure and adrenocortical responses to acute stress in children. Environ Health Perspect. 2008;116:249–255. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Ivy AS, Rex CS, Chen Y, Dube C, Maras PM, Grigoriadis DE, Gall CM, Lynch G, Baram TZ. Hippocampal dysfunction and cognitive impairments provoked by chronic early-life stress involve excessive activation of CRH receptors. J Neurosci. 2010;30:13005–13015. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Jacobs DE, Clickner RP, Zhou JY, Viet SM, Marker DA, Rogers JW, Zeldin DC, Broene P, Friedman W. The prevalence of lead-based paint hazards in U.S. housing. Environ Health Perspect. 2002;110:A599–A606. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Kao GS, Cheng LY, Chen LH, Tzeng WY, Cherng CG, Su CC, Wang CY, Yu L. Neonatal isolation decreases cued fear conditioning and frontal cortical histone 3 lysine 9 methylation in adult female rats. Eur J Pharmacol. 2012;697:65–72. [PubMed]
  • Kehoe P, Shoemaker WJ, Arons C, Triano L, Suresh G. Repeated isolation stress in the neonatal rat: relation to brain dopamine systems in the 10-day-old rat. Behav Neurosci. 1998;112:1466–1474. [PubMed]
  • Knuth ED, Etgen AM. Corticosterone secretion induced by chronic isolation in neonatal rats is sexually dimorphic and accompanied by elevated ACTH. Horm Behav. 2005;47:65–75. [PubMed]
  • Knuth ED, Etgen AM. Long-term behavioral consequences of brief, repeated neonatal isolation. Brain Res. 2007;1128:139–147. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Kosten TA, Kehoe P. Neonatal isolation is a relevant model for studying the contributions of early life stress to vulnerability to drug abuse: response to Marmendal et al. (2004) Dev Psychobiol. 2005;47:108–110. [PubMed]
  • Kosten TA, Zhang XY, Kehoe P. Infant rats with chronic neonatal isolation experience show decreased extracellular serotonin levels in ventral striatum at baseline and in response to cocaine. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 2004;152:19–24. [PubMed]
  • Lanphear BP, Byrd RS, Auinger P, Schaffer SJ. Community characteristics associated with elevated blood lead levels in children. Pediatrics. 1998;101:264–271. [PubMed]
  • Lanphear BP, Dietrich K, Auinger P, Cox C. Cognitive deficits associated with blood lead concentrations <10 microg/dL in US children and adolescents. Public Health Rep. 2000;115:521–529. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Levin R, Brown MJ, Kashtock ME, Jacobs DE, Whelan EA, Rodman J, Schock MR, Padilla A, Sinks T. Lead exposures in U.S. Children, 2008: implications for prevention. Environ Health Perspect. 2008;116:1285–1293. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Lidsky TI, Schneider JS. Lead neurotoxicity in children: basic mechanisms and clinical correlates. Brain. 2003;126:5–19. [PubMed]
  • Lupien SJ, King S, Meaney MJ, McEwen BS. Child’s stress hormone levels correlate with mother’s socioeconomic status and depressive state. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48:976–980. [PubMed]
  • Lupien SJ, King S, Meaney MJ, McEwen BS. Can poverty get under your skin? Basal cortisol levels and cognitive function in children from low and high socioeconomic status. Dev Psychopathol. 2001;13:653–676. [PubMed]
  • Lupien SJ, Nair NP, Briere S, Maheu F, Tu MT, Lemay M, McEwen BS, Meaney MJ. Increased cortisol levels and impaired cognition in human aging: implication for depression and dementia in later life. Rev Neurosci. 1999;10:117–139. [PubMed]
  • McCormick CM, Kehoe P, Kovacs S. Corticosterone release in response to repeated, short episodes of neonatal isolation: evidence of sensitization. Int J Dev Neurosci. 1998;16:175–185. [PubMed]
  • McEwen BS. Stress, adaptation, and disease. Allostasis and allostatic load. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1998;840:33–44. [PubMed]
  • McEwen BS, Angulo J, Cameron H, Chao HM, Daniels D, Gannon MN, Gould E, Mendelson S, Sakai R, Spencer R. Paradoxical effects of adrenal steroids on the brain: protection versus degeneration. Biol Psychiatry. 1992;31:177–199. [PubMed]
  • McEwen BS, Stellar E. Stress and the individual. Mechanisms leading to disease. Arch Intern Med. 1993;153:2093–2101. [PubMed]
  • McGivern RF, Henschel D, Hutcheson M, Pangburn T. Sex difference in daily water consumption of rats: effect of housing and hormones. Physiol Behav. 1996;59:653–658. [PubMed]
  • Mendez-Gomez J, Garcia-Vargas GG, Lopez-Carrillo L, Calderon-Aranda ES, Gomez A, Vera E, Valverde M, Cebrian ME, Rojas E. Genotoxic effects of environmental exposure to arsenic and lead on children in region Lagunera, Mexico. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1140:358–367. [PubMed]
  • Meyer PA, McGeehin MA, Falk H. A global approach to childhood lead poisoning prevention. Int J Hyg Environ Health. 2003;206:363–369. [PubMed]
  • Muntner P, Menke A, DeSalvo KB, Rabito FA, Batuman V. Continued decline in blood lead levels among adults in the United States: the National Health and Nutrition Examination Surveys. Arch Intern Med. 2005;165:2155–2161. [PubMed]
  • Neal AP, Guilarte TR. Molecular neurobiology of lead (Pb(2+)): effects on synaptic function. Mol Neurobiol. 2010;42:151–160. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Needleman HL, Gunnoe C, Leviton A, Reed R, Peresie H, Maher C, Barrett P. Deficits in psychologic and classroom performance of children with elevated dentine lead levels. N Engl J Med. 1979;300:689–695. [PubMed]
  • Patel A, Athawale A. Blood lead levels in children with encephalopathy. Indian Pediatr. 2009;46:845–848. [PubMed]
  • Reuhl KR, Rice DC, Gilbert SG, Mallett J. Effects of chronic developmental lead exposure on monkey neuroanatomy: visual system. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 1989;99:501–509. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC. Behavioral deficit (delayed matching to sample) in monkeys exposed from birth to low levels of lead. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 1984;75:337–345. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC. Chronic low-lead exposure from birth produces deficits in discrimination reversal in monkeys. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 1985;77:201–210. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC. Chronic low-level lead exposure in monkeys does not affect simple reaction time. Neurotoxicology. 1988a;9:105–107. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC. Schedule-controlled behavior in infant and juvenile monkeys exposed to lead from birth. Neurotoxicology. 1988b;9:75–87. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC. Lead-induced behavioral impairment on a spatial discrimination reversal task in monkeys exposed during different periods of development. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 1990;106:327–333. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC. Behavioral effects of lead in monkeys tested during infancy and adulthood. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1992a;14:235–245. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC. Effect of lead during different developmental periods in the monkey on concurrent discrimination performance. Neurotoxicology. 1992b;13:583–592. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC. Lead exposure during different developmental periods produces different effects on FI performance in monkeys tested as juveniles and adults. Neurotoxicology. 1992c;13:757–770. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC. Lead-induced changes in learning: evidence for behavioral mechanisms from experimental animal studies. Neurotoxicology. 1993;14:167–178. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC. Behavioral effects of lead: commonalities between experimental and epidemiologic data. Environ Health Perspect. 1996a;104(Suppl 2):337–351. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Rice DC. Effect of long-term lead exposure on hematology, blood biochemistry, and growth curves in monkeys. Neurotoxicology. 1996b;18:221–236. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC. Effects of lifetime lead exposure in monkeys on detection of pure tones. Fundam Appl Toxicol. 1997;36:112–118. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC. Effects of lifetime lead exposure on spatial and temporal visual function in monkeys. Neurotoxicology. 1998;19:893–902. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC, Gilbert SG. Low lead exposure from birth produces behavioral toxicity (DRL) in monkeys. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 1985;80:421–426. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC, Gilbert SG. Lack of sensitive period for lead-induced behavioral impairment on a spatial delayed alternation task in monkeys. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 1990a;103:364–373. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC, Gilbert SG. Sensitive periods for lead-induced behavioral impairment (nonspatial discrimination reversal) in monkeys. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 1990b;102:101–109. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC, Gilbert SG, Willes RF. Neonatal low-level lead exposure in monkeys: locomotor activity, schedule-controlled behavior, and the effects of amphetamine. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 1979;51:503–513. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC, Karpinski KF. Lifetime low-level lead exposure produces deficits in delayed alternation in adult monkeys. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1988;10:207–214. [PubMed]
  • Rice DC, Willes RF. Neonatal low-level lead exposure in monkeys (Macaca fascicularis): effect on two-choice non-spatial form discrimination. J Environ Pathol Toxicol. 1979;2:1195–1203. [PubMed]
  • Rossi-George A, Virgolini MB, Weston D, Cory-Slechta DA. Alterations in glucocorticoid negative feedback following maternal Pb, prenatal stress and the combination: a potential biological unifying mechanism for their corresponding disease profiles. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 2009;234:117–127. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Rossi-George A, Virgolini MB, Weston D, Thiruchelvam M, Cory-Slechta DA. Interactions of lifetime lead exposure and stress: behavioral, neurochemical and HPA axis effects. Neurotoxicology. 2011;32:83–99. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Sapolsky RM, Meaney MJ. Maturation of the adrenocortical stress response: Neuroendocrine control mechanisms and the stress hyporesponsive period. Brain Research Reviews. 1986;11:65–76. [PubMed]
  • Schnaas L, Rothenberg SJ, Flores MF, Martinez S, Hernandez C, Osorio E, Perroni E. Blood lead secular trend in a cohort of children in Mexico City (1987–2002) Environ Health Perspect. 2004;112:1110–1115. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Slone EJ, Teuschler LK, Aume LL, Narotsky MG, Simmons JE, Lordo RA, Rice GE. Estimation of individual rodent water consumption from group consumption data for gestation, lactation, and postweaning life stages using linear regression models. ILAR J. 2012;53:E99–112. [PubMed]
  • Surkan PJ, Zhang A, Trachtenberg F, Daniel DB, McKinlay S, Bellinger DC. Neuropsychological function in children with blood lead levels <10 microg/dL. Neurotoxicology. 2007;28:1170–1177. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Tong S, McMichael AJ, Baghurst PA. Interactions between environmental lead exposure and sociodemographic factors on cognitive development. Arch Environ Health. 2000;55:330–335. [PubMed]
  • Truelove JF, Gilbert SG, Rice DC. Effect of diet on blood lead concentration in the cynomolgus monkey. Fundam Appl Toxicol. 1985;5:588–596. [PubMed]
  • Tuchscherer M, Kanitz E, Otten W, Tuchscherer A. Effects of prenatal stress on cellular and humoral immune responses in neonatal pigs. Vet Immunol Immunopathol. 2002;86:195–203. [PubMed]
  • Virgolini MB, Bauter MR, Weston DD, Cory-Slechta DA. Permanent alterations in stress responsivity in female offspring subjected to combined maternal lead exposure and/or stress. Neurotoxicology. 2006;27:11–21. [PubMed]
  • Virgolini MB, Rossi-George A, Lisek R, Weston DD, Thiruchelvam M, Cory-Slechta DA. CNS effects of developmental Pb exposure are enhanced by combined maternal and offspring stress. Neurotoxicology. 2008a;29:812–827. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Virgolini MB, Rossi-George A, Weston D, Cory-Slechta DA. Influence of low level maternal Pb exposure and prenatal stress on offspring stress challenge responsivity. Neurotoxicology. 2008b;29:928–939. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Wang L, Xu SE, Zhang GD, Wang WY. Study of lead absorption and its effect on children’s development. Biomed Environ Sci. 1989;2:325–330. [PubMed]
  • Weston HI, Weston DD, Allen JL, Cory-Slechta DA. Sex-dependent impacts of low-level lead exposure and prenatal stress on impulsive choice behavior and associated biochemical and neurochemical manifestations. Neurotoxicology. 2014;44:169–183. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Winneke G, Beginn U, Ewert T, Havestadt C, Kraemer U, Krause C, Thron HL, Wagner HM. Comparing the effects of perinatal and later childhood lead exposure on neuropsychological outcome. Environ Res. 1985;38:155–167. [PubMed]
  • Yuan W, Holland SK, Cecil KM, Dietrich KN, Wessel SD, Altaye M, Hornung RW, Ris MD, Egelhoff JC, Lanphear BP. The impact of early childhood lead exposure on brain organization: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of language function. Pediatrics. 2006;118:971–977. [PubMed]