PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptHHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
Appetite. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 May 1.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC4799737
NIHMSID: NIHMS763202

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Infant Feeding Styles Questionnaire in Latino Families

Abstract

Background

Parent feeding practices affect risk of obesity in children. Latino children are at higher risk of obesity than the general population, yet valid measure of feeding practices, one of which is the Infant Feeding Styles Questionnaire (IFSQ), have not been formally validated in Spanish.

Objective

To validate the IFSQ among Latino families, we conducted confirmatory factor analysis of pressuring, restrictive, and responsive feeding constructs from the IFSQ.

Design/Methods

The IFSQ was administered at the 12-month visit in the Greenlight study, a multi-center cluster randomized trial to prevent obesity. Parents were included if they were of Latino origin (n=303) and completed an English or Spanish language modified IFSQ (without the indulgence construct). Scores from nine sub-constructs of the IFSQ were compared between English and Spanish language versions. We tested reliability with Cronbach’s alpha coefficients and performed confirmatory factor analysis to examine factor loadings and goodness of fit characteristics, modifying constructs to achieve best fit.

Results

Of 303 parents completing the IFSQ, 84% were born outside the US, and 74% completed the IFSQ in Spanish. Reliability coefficients ranged from 0.28–0.61 for the laissez-faire sub-constructs and from 0.58–0.83 for the pressuring, restrictive, and responsive sub-constructs. Results for all coefficients were similar between participants responding to an English and Spanish version of the IFSQ. Goodness of fit indices ranged from CFI 0.82–1 and RMSEA 0.00–0.31, and the model performed best in pressuring-soothing (CFI 1.0, RMSEA 0.00) and restrictive-amount (CFI 0.98, RMSEA 0.1) sub-constructs.

Conclusions

In a sample of Latino families, pressuring, restrictive, and responsive constructs performed well. The modified IFSQ in both English and Spanish-speaking Latino families may be used to assess parenting behaviors related to early obesity risk in this at-risk population.

Keywords: feeding styles, confirmatory factor analysis, infant feeding, Latino

Introduction

Compared with non-Hispanic infants, Hispanic infants have a higher prevalence of weight-for-recumbent length greater than the 95th percentile. Additionally, obesity risk factors in infancy, such as the quality of infant diet and behaviors related to feeding, differ by race/ethnicity.13 When these risk factors are adjusted for, racial/ethnic disparities in childhood obesity are attenuated or disappear.4 With an increasing focus on preventing obesity, it is important to understand modifiable risk factors for the development of obesity in Latinos, the largest minority population in the United States. One such risk factor is rapid infant weight gain, defined as upward crossing of weight percentiles in the first two years of life, which increases obesity risk in childhood and adulthood, increases cardiovascular, respiratory, and metabolic risk, and may contribute to increases in body fat percentage compared to infants who do not exhibit rapid infant weight gain.513

Although mechanisms that contribute to rapid infant weight gain and subsequent obesity remain unclear, decisions about the primary mode of feeding (i.e. breastfeeding or bottle feeding) and parental perception of infant hunger likely influence early growth trajectories,14,15 and parental feeding practices are a critical component to infant and child growth, and might help explain the intergenerational transmission of obesity.1618 Theoretical and experimental work to identify, describe, and validate parental feeding practices has led to the development of the Child Feeding Questionnaire (CFQ).19,20 The CFQ and other valid measures have demonstrated associations between feeding practices and child food intake and weight status, particularly with “restrictive” behaviors that prohibit and control access to certain foods.21 Older children appear to be acutely aware of previous food restrictions and when in a more permissive environment, may routinely request foods that are being restricted.22

The environment of feeding shaping parent-infant interaction likely differs significantly from later childhood by schedule, dietary content, perception of hunger and satiety cues, and the development of desires and demands as children grow. Consideration of the development of feeding practices may be particularly important in early life, as parents habituate feeding practices, which may or may not be dependent on infant hunger and satiety cues.23,24 Some aspects of parental feeding practices are likely established in the first months to year of life and may have influences on weight gain in the first year of life or later childhood outcomes.2527 For example, highly controlling feeding practices at one year of life are predictive of difficulty with internal cues related to eating behaviors later in life.25

Early and reliable identification and modification of parental feeding practices, and parental feeding styles underlying these practices, could contribute to obesity prevention within a critical period during which behavior modification might be more amenable.28,29 Studying these behaviors in diverse populations is challenging, as much of the initial experimental work and validation occurred among mostly non-Hispanic white, middle- and upper-income families. A few studies have examined low-income African American mothers30 and Latina mothers,31 with data suggesting Latinos may be more likely to pressure their infants to finish feeds and less likely to be responsive to feeding cues.31 As cultural influences differ and play variable roles in parental feeding styles and practices, more routine study of these beliefs and behaviors in Latino populations requires a valid and reliable measure.

Development of the Infant Feeding Styles Questionnaire (IFSQ)32 was in part a result of the search for a valid and reliable measure of parent feeding beliefs and practices in infancy and early childhood. The IFSQ was initially developed through formative ethnographic research33,34 and then assessed for construct validity in two samples of African-American mothers attending WIC clinics. The IFSQ assesses beliefs and practices within five parental feeding styles: “laissez-faire” (with sub-constructs of diet quality and attention); “pressuring” (with sub-constructs of pressuring to feed, pressuring with cereal, and pressuring as soothing); “restrictive” (with sub-constructs of diet quality and amount); “responsive” (with sub-constructs of satiety and attention); and “indulgence” (with sub-constructs permissive, coaxing, soothing, and pampering) (Table 1). Indulging and uninvolved feeding styles have been associated with unhealthy nutrient intake in low-income children,35 and restrictive and pressuring feeding practices were more likely seen in a group of low-income Hispanic infants at high risk for obesity.36 In a sample of low-income African-American mothers, restrictive feeding style was associated with larger infant size but better infant nutrition, while pressuring style was associated with smaller infant size and more age-inappropriate feeding.37

Table 1
Infant Feeding Style Questionnaire (IFSQ) Structure and Content32

The IFSQ has been used in multiple settings36,3841 and has the advantage of assessing parental beliefs and practices, yet whether these constructs are adequately represented or can be reliably measured in English- and in Spanish-speaking Latino families remains unknown. Given potential misinterpretation of questions related to feeding42 and the documented importance of culture-specific influences on feeding styles among African-Americans,33,43,44 we aimed to validate model fit of the IFSQ in a large population of low-income, English- and Spanish-speaking Latino families so that we may begin to fill a critical gap in knowledge of this understudied and rapidly growing segment of the US population.

Methods

Sample

The IFSQ was administered to caregivers of children enrolled in the Greenlight study, a cluster-randomized trial of an obesity prevention intervention focused on the first two years of life.45 The Greenlight study uses a literacy and numeracy-sensitive intervention based on social cognitive theory to target adult caregivers at their child’s preventive office visits in the first 2 years of life. Two university clinics were randomized to implement the obesity prevention intervention, which included a series of picture-based low-literacy toolkits to encourage recommended behaviors and a health-communication curriculum for the child’s health care provider. Two “active control” sites implemented The Injury Prevention Program (TIPP) curriculum designed by the American Academy of Pediatrics.46

The methods of the Greenlight study have been published previously.45 Briefly, caregivers were eligible to enroll if their infant was between 6 and 16 weeks old at the 2 month baseline visit, had a weight-for-length greater than the 3rd percentile based on WHO growth curves, was born at least 34 weeks gestational age and at least 1500 grams at birth, and did not have a known medical problem affecting growth (e.g. failure to thrive or a metabolic disorder). Caregivers were excluded if they did not speak English or Spanish, were less than 18 years old, had mental, neurological illness or poor vision, or planned to leave the clinic or move within the next 2 years. To focus on validation and model fit for the IFSQ among Latino participants, we limited our sample to caregivers who self identified as Hispanic/Latino. Written and verbal consent were obtained from caregivers according to the institutional review board policies at each of the four sites. The four institutional review boards approved the study, which was registered with the national Clinical Trials Registry (NCT01040897 at clinicaltrials.gov), and a data safety monitoring board monitored study progress.

Measures

Data collected at the 12-month visit included four of the five constructs of the IFSQ (“laissez-faire,” “pressuring,” “restrictive,” and “responsive”). The “indulgence” construct was not used in the Greenlight study due to time limitations. The survey was completed either in person with study personnel at the child’s 12 month visit (97%) or shortly after this visit, by phone (3%). The Spanish language version of the IFSQ was developed for the Latino Infant Nutrition Study, translated from English by a native Spanish speaker and then translated back to English, with discrepancies in the versions resolved by the bilingual research team.47 In brief, the complete IFSQ includes 39 items probing beliefs (scored on a 5-point scale: disagree, slightly disagree, neutral, slightly agree, agree) and 44 items probing behaviors (scored similarly: never, seldom, half of the time, most of the time, always). For the Greenlight study, we collected 51 items (23 probing beliefs and 28 probing behaviors), and the IFSQ was administered in either English or Spanish, based on the caregiver’s stated preference. Other measures included in this study included caregiver sociodemographics (age, sex, income, acculturation), self-reported caregiver anthropomorphic measures, and directly-measured child anthropomorphics. Maternal obesity was defined as body mass index >30 kg/m2 as defined by World Health Organization (WHO) standards. Acculturation was measured with the Short Acculturation Scale for Hispanics (SASH), with low acculturation considered to be a score <3.48

Analysis

Initially, we examined sociodemographic and anthropometric variables of our sample and calculated mean scores, standard deviations, and distributions of each item within the sub-constructs of the four feeding styles and calculated item reliabilities in the overall sample and in the subsets completing the IFSQ in English and in Spanish with Cronbach’s alpha coefficients to establish whether further validation was warranted. Based on these results, we performed confirmatory factor analysis for nine sub-constructs of the IFSQ across the four feeding styles. We examined factor loadings and model fit, using this information to iteratively modify models that had poor or marginal fit by eliminating items with non-significant or low factor loadings and/or including covariance between similarly worded items to improve model fit. All modifications were theoretically, not empirically, driven, and overall, few modifications were made. We focused on the nine sub-constructs based on previous work with the IFSQ19,20 showing that the sub-constructs measure different aspects of infant feeding styles and are differentially associated with child feeding and weight outcomes. We also tested whether second order CFA models, including all the items associated with each feeding style and the covariances between constructs, had better fit in this sample. These models either had poor fit, as was the case with pressuring, or did not converge due to empirical under-identification, occurring when the covariance matrix between the sub-constructs was equal to zero.

A weighted least squares estimator, with items considered categorical, in Mplus version 5 (Muthen and Muthen, Los Angeles, CA) examined model fit for each of the nine sub-constructs, producing several measures of model goodness-of fit: chi-square; root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA); and comparative fit index (CFI). The chi-square test evaluates absolute fit of the model to the data matrix, with larger and statistically significant values indicating poor fit. The RMSEA and CFI, measures of comparative fit, assess the model against a model of “reasonable” fit to the data. Values <0.06 (RMSEA) and >0.95 (CFI) are generally suggested as cutoffs for good model fit, although there are no clear standards for their interpretation.49 Additionally, we calculated Schwartz’s Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC) according to Raftery.50 This version of the BIC allows comparison of models estimated with weighted least squares estimators. Negative change in BIC between models indicates the preferred model. When combined with the BIC, used in this way as a measure of incremental fit changes between models, the chi-square, RMSEA, and CFI provide a comprehensive evaluation of model fit.

Results

From a total sample of 865 participants enrolled in the Greenlight study, 430 (49.7%) identified as Latino; of these, 303 (70%) had a majority of items collected from the IFSQ. Characteristics including acculturation, language of administration, household income, infant’s sex, and infant’s weight-for-recumbent length did not differ significantly between those with complete and incomplete IFSQ responses. Sample demographics and anthropometrics are displayed in Table 2. The majority of caregivers completing the IFSQ were mothers (94%), born outside of the United States (84%), and spoke Spanish as their primary language (74%). Just over half of mothers were born in Mexico, 43% had less than a high school education, 76% were unemployed, and income was below $20,000 per year in 64% of households. Mean acculturation scores were low, defined as a mean of ≤ 2.99, according to scoring of the validated SASH.48 The mean pre-pregnancy body mass index (BMI) of mothers was 28 kg/m2 (SD 6), however nearly one-third of mothers were obese as defined by the WHO (BMI> 30 kg/m2). Mean birth weight of children in the sample was 3.3kg (SD 0.5kg), which represents the 50th percentile of the WHO growth standard of the population. At the 12 month visit, weight-for-length z-score (WFLz) and BMI z score were each 0.43.

Table 2
Characteristics of the Study Population

Respondents tended to score higher in restrictive and responsive sub-constructs than laissez-faire and pressuring sub-constructs, and overall internal reliability between the pressuring, restrictive, and responsive sub-constructs ranged from 0.63 to 0.83. When examined by language of IFSQ administration, reliability for Spanish language respondents, comprising the majority of the sample, closely resembled overall reliability (Table 3).

Table 3
IFSQ Score Characteristics and Reliabilities

Confirmatory factor analysis results for the models of three constructs (pressuring, restrictive, and responsive) and selected modifications including stepwise elimination of items with lower factor loadings and/or addition of error covariance are shown in Tables 4 (overall sample) and 5 (Spanish language sample). The laissez-faire construct displayed poor fit with low Cronbach’s alphas (0.44 overall, 0.48 for Spanish-speaking, and 0.55 for English speaking samples) and so CFA was not performed. The individual items and their factor loadings are shown in the Appendix.

Table 4
Goodness of Fit Indices For Feeding Style Constructs, Total Sample (N=303)

Full models of three constructs of the IFSQ (pressuring, restrictive, and responsive) fit well. The pressuring to finish sub-construct had a large and statistically-significant chi-square (150.7, p<0.001), although the chi-square is highly dependent on sample size and assumptions of distribution, and in large sample sizes like ours, small discrepancies between the observed and predicted matrices will result in a significant chi-square.51 We improved the chi-square and other model fit indices by dropping one item (PR5) and adding covariance to include three similarly-worded items (“try to get child to finish breastmilk or formula” and “try to get child to eat even if not hungry” and “if child seems full, encourage to finish anyway”). This modification achieved a better fit, shown by reduced chi-square (53.7, p<0.001), a CFI of 0.95, and a negative BIC (−9.15). Similarly, the pressuring with cereal sub-construct was modified by eliminating one item (PR11), which also achieved a high CFI (0.98) and a significant change in BIC from 118 to 0.08. Initial, full models of pressuring soothing, restrictive amounts, and responsive to satiety all fit very well, with small chi-square values (0.7–16.4), low RMSEAs (0.0–0.09) and high CFIs (0.99–1.00). When goodness of fit indices were examined in the subset of the population completing Spanish language surveys (n=215), these showed very similar model fit in all sub-constructs with identical modifications (Table 5).

Table 5
Goodness of Fit Indices For Feeding Style Constructs, Spanish-Speaking (N=215)

After initial confirmatory factor analysis showed poor model fit for the laissez-faire construct in this population, this construct was reexamined with exploratory factor analysis. Initial eigenvalues suggested a two-factor model, which was examined for reliability with the Cronbach’s alpha. Despite analyzing as a two-factor model, and dropping items that might have improved the scale’s reliability, Cronbach’s alpha values remained low (0.26–0.41), and we concluded that the laissez-faire construct of feeding beliefs and behaviors is not reliable in this sample of Latino families.

Discussion

We completed confirmatory factor analysis of the IFSQ in a large sample of mostly Spanish-speaking Latino families, and showed that overall model fit was appropriate for three of four IFSQ constructs collected as a part of the Greenlight study: pressuring; restrictive feeding; and responsive feeding. The fourth IFSQ sub-construct, laissez-faire, did not fit these data. This is the first formal validation of the IFSQ in a Latino population and the first use of the measure with Spanish-speaking participants. The IFSQ has been confirmed to fit well in 150 low-income African American mothers whose children had a mean age 10 months32 yet there was previously no indication that a tool used earlier in life would be useful in a Latino population that is at potentially higher risk for obesity.

Validation of the IFSQ among Latino caregivers allows expanded use of this tool for Latino population, which is both the most rapidly expanding and highest risk community in the US. Although restrictive infant-feeding practices (i.e. strictly limiting types and amounts of intake) have been most consistently associated with increased child BMI,21 it is unclear what cultural influences might underlie this association, and so measuring reliable constructs in diverse cultures is important when measuring risk for obesity in early life.52 Constructs measured in the IFSQ have been examined in other Latino populations. In one population of Hispanic, non-US born, less educated families, restrictive and pressuring feeding constructs from the IFSQ were highly prevalent.36 Among other studies involving parenting style and feeding, Hispanic participants were more likely to be indulgent (i.e. less demanding and more responsive on a two-dimension framework).16 Our sample scored highest on the responsive construct (mean 3.97), potentially demonstrating similarities with this previous sample of Hispanic families.

The laissez-faire feeding style construct did not achieve adequate model fit, and when reexamined with exploratory factor analysis, did not appear to form an underlying latent construct in this population of Latino families. It is possible that, among Latinos, culturally-specific wording may not adequately describe behaviors thought to be “laissez-faire,” or that differences in wording that are sensitive to translation may have altered the construct’s reliability, as the measured Cronbach’s alpha values differed significantly between language administration. Additionally, the fact that many families were breastfeeding and not formula-feeding could mean that behaviors like bottle-propping, which is measured within the laissez-faire construct, were not relevant. Others have described challenges with reliability of the laissez-faire construct in Latino families.47 We suggest that this construct be measured in additional Latino populations, perhaps targeting families choosing to feed with a bottle, to capture beliefs and practices related to bottle-feeding.

Our analysis is limited by the fact that the IFSQ was administered without the indulgence construct, and only once in the Greenlight Study, at the infant’s 12-month visit, and recall of several behaviors or beliefs more pertinent to early infancy may not represent current feeding behaviors at one year of life. Additionally, the survey was part of a trial to prevent obesity, which may have resulted in changes to responses at the intervention sites, although few specific components of the IFSQ were addressed by the intervention. Although our sample of Latino participants was generally of low acculturation, there are likely differences in cultural beliefs and behaviors between first and subsequent Latino generations living in the United States, and we did not separately analyze our data by generation. There were not sufficient numbers of English speaking Latino participants to analyze the IFSQ within this subpopulation alone, and although there appear to be no substantive differences between the language of administration and the overall model fit, we did not perform invariance testing to confirm this. Finally, the pressuring as soothing sub-construct may have shown evidence of overfit and should be examined closely in other samples.

The literature suggests that maternal control over feeding appears to have a substantial influence on infant weight gain, and can redirect weight gain patterns, theoretically by either encouraging more intake in smaller infants, or restricting intake in larger infants.27 As more specific constructs of feeding behaviors and beliefs are measured in more populations and over time, an improving landscape of modifiable behaviors should emerge. Longitudinal assessment of these behaviors, along with weight measurements, is necessary, as there is currently a paucity of longitudinal data.37,53 If high levels of external control of feeding behaviors (including restriction) early in life are responsible for poor regulation of intake later,20 then perhaps restrictive behaviors can be discouraged to prevent later problems with feeding behaviors. Now that we have demonstrated good model fit with confirmatory factory analysis for these three feeding styles, differences in weight-for-length and BMI should be examined by feeding style scores to attempt to identify where feeding behaviors might be modified to decrease obesity incidence in Latino families that may already be at greater risk for obesity.

Acknowledgments

All phases of this study were supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver Institute for Child Health and Development, NICHD (R01 HD049794), with supplemental funding from CDC and OBSSR (Grant #R01HD059794-04S1, R01HD059794-04S2). Parts of the study were supported the National Institutes of Health’s National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences through its Clinical and Translational Science Awards Program (CTSA), grants # 1UL1RR029893, UL1TR000445 and UL1RR025747. Dr. Wood is supported by University of North Carolina Primary Care Research Fellowship (HRSA 5T32HP014001). Funding for the development of the Spanish IFSQ was provided by the Center for Excellence in Children's Nutrition sponsored by Mead Johnson and Company and The University of North Carolina's Program on Ethnicity, Culture and Health Outcomes (ECHO). Additional support was provided by the Carolina Population Center (R24 HD050924). The team would like to acknowledge Sophie Ravanbakht for her assistance with IFSQ administration and translation.

Abbreviations

IFSQ
Infant Feeding Styles Questionnaire
CFI
Comparative Fit Index
RMSEA
Root Mean Square Error of Approximation
CDC
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
CFQ
Child Feeding Questionnaire
WIC
Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children
WHO
World Health Organization
SASH
Short Acculturation Scale for Hispanics
BIC
Bayesian Information Criteria

Appendix. IFSQ Items and Factor Loadings (Total Sample)

FactorItemFactor
Loading
Pressuring
Finishing

Behavior Items
PR1Try to get (child) to finish his/her food1
PR2If (child) seems full, encourage to finish anyway1.01
PR3Try to get (child) to finish breastmilk or formula0.66
PR4Try to get (child) to eat even if not hungry0.34
PR5Insist re-try new food refused at same meal0.77
PR6Praise after each bite to encourage finish food0.97
Belief Items
PR7Important for toddler finish all food on his/her plate0.61
PR8Important for infant finish all milk in his/her bottle0.84
Cereal

Behavior Items
PR11Give/gave (child) cereal in the bottle1
Belief Items
PR12Cereal in bottle helps infant sleep thru the night1.44
PR13Putting cereal in bottle good b/c helps infant feel full1.14
PR14An infant <6 mo needs more than formula or breastmilk to be full1.27
PR15An infant <6 mo needs more than formula or breastmilk to sleep through the night0.81
Soothing

Behavior Items
PR16When (child) cries, immediately feed him/her1
Belief Items
PR17Best way to make infant stop crying is to feed1.41
PR18Best way to make toddler stop crying is to feed1.27
PR19When infant cries, usually means s/he needs to be fed1.09

Restrictive
Amount

Behavior Items
RS1I carefully control how much (child) eats1
RS2I am very careful not to feed (child) too much1.05
Belief Items
RS3Important parent has rules re: how much toddler eats1.33
RS4Important parent decides how much infant should eat1.23
Diet Quality

Behavior Items
RS5I let (child) eat fast food1
RS6I let (child) eat junk food1.11
Belief Items
RS7A toddler should never eat fast food1.29
RS8An infant should never eat fast food1.01
RS9A toddler should never eat sugary food like cookies0.63
RS10A toddler should never eat junk food like chips0.42
RS11A toddler should only eat healthy food0.39

Responsive
Satiety

Behavior Items
RP1(Child) lets me know when s/he is full1
RP2(Child) lets me knows when s/he is hungry6.36
RP3I let (child) decide how much to eat1.86
RP4I pay attention when (child) seems to be telling me that s/he is full or hungry7.39
RP5I allow (child) to eat when s/he is hungry7.89
Belief Items
RP6Child knows when s/he is full6.33
RP7Child knows when hungry, needs to eat6.65
Attention

Behavior Items
RP8Talk to (child) to encourage to drink formula/breastmilk1
RP9Talk to (child) to encourage him/her to eat2.13
RP10Show (child) how to eat by taking a bite or pretending to1.88
RP11I will retry new foods if they are rejected at first1.75
Belief Items
RP12Important to help or encourage a toddler to eat1.35
*Factor loadings are not standardized

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

1. Thompson AL, Bentley ME. The critical period of infant feeding for the development of early disparities in obesity. Social Science & Medicine. 2013;97:288–296. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
2. Perrin EM, Rothman RL, Sanders LM, et al. Racial and Ethnic Differences Associated With Feeding- and Activity-Related Behaviors in Infants. Pediatrics. 2014 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
3. Wen X, Kong KL, Eiden RD, Sharma NN, Xie C. Sociodemographic differences and infant dietary patterns. Pediatrics. 2014;134(5):e1387–e1398. [PubMed]
4. Taveras EM, Gillman MW, Kleinman KP, Rich-Edwards JW, Rifas-Shiman SL. Reducing racial/ethnic disparities in childhood obesity: the role of early life risk factors. JAMA pediatrics. 2013;167(8):731–738. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
5. Eid EE. Follow-up study of physical growth of children who had excessive weight gain in first six months of life. British medical journal. 1970;2(5701):74–76. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
6. Ong KK, Loos RJ. Rapid infancy weight gain and subsequent obesity: systematic reviews and hopeful suggestions. Acta paediatrica (Oslo, Norway : 1992) 2006;95(8):904–908. [PubMed]
7. Stettler N, Kumanyika SK, Katz SH, Zemel BS, Stallings VA. Rapid weight gain during infancy and obesity in young adulthood in a cohort of African Americans. The American journal of clinical nutrition. 2003;77(6):1374–1378. [PubMed]
8. Dennison BA, Edmunds LS, Stratton HH, Pruzek RM. Rapid infant weight gain predicts childhood overweight. Obesity (Silver Spring Md.) 2006;14(3):491–499. [PubMed]
9. Belfort MB, Rifas-Shiman SL, Rich-Edwards J, Kleinman KP, Gillman MW. Size at birth, infant growth, and blood pressure at three years of age. The Journal of pediatrics. 2007;151(6):670–674. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
10. Ben-Shlomo Y, McCarthy A, Hughes R, Tilling K, Davies D, Smith GD. Immediate postnatal growth is associated with blood pressure in young adulthood: the Barry Caerphilly Growth Study. Hypertension. 2008;52(4):638–644. [PubMed]
11. Skilton MR, Marks GB, Ayer JG, et al. Weight gain in infancy and vascular risk factors in later childhood. Pediatrics. 2013;131(6):e1821–e1828. [PubMed]
12. Sonnenschein-van der Voort AMM, Howe LD, Granell R, et al.
13. Leunissen RW, Kerkhof GF, Stijnen T, Hokken-Koelega A. Timing and tempo of first-year rapid growth in relation to cardiovascular and metabolic risk profile in early adulthood. JAMA : the journal of the American Medical Association. 2009;301(21):2234–2242. [PubMed]
14. Dewey KG, Heinig MJ, Nommsen LA, Peerson JM, Lonnerdal B. Growth of breast-fed and formula-fed infants from 0 to 18 months: the DARLING Study. Pediatrics. 1992;89(6 Pt 1):1035–1041. [PubMed]
15. Burton P, Wells JC, Kennedy K, Nicholl R, Khakoo A, Fewtrell MS. Association between infant correlates of impulsivity--surgency (extraversion)--and early infant growth. Appetite. 2011;57(2):504–509. [PubMed]
16. Hughes SO, Power TG, Fisher JO, Mueller S, Nicklas TA. Revisiting a neglected construct: parenting styles in a child-feeding context. Appetite. 2005;44(1):83–92. [PubMed]
17. Wardle J, Sanderson S, Guthrie CA, Rapoport L, Plomin R. Parental feeding style and the intergenerational transmission of obesity risk. Obesity Research. 2002;10(6):453–462. [PubMed]
18. Baughcum AE, Powers SW, Johnson SB, et al. Maternal feeding practices and beliefs and their relationships to overweight in early childhood. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2001;22(6):391–408. [PubMed]
19. Birch LL, Fisher JO, Grimm-Thomas K, Markey CN, Sawyer R, Johnson SL. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Child Feeding Questionnaire: a measure of parental attitudes, beliefs and practices about child feeding and obesity proneness. Appetite. 2001;36(3):201–210. [PubMed]
20. Johnson SL, Birch LL. PARENTS AND CHILDRENS ADIPOSITY AND EATING STYLE. Pediatrics. 1994;94(5):653–661. [PubMed]
21. Faith MS, Scanlon KS, Birch LL, Francis LA, Sherry B. Parent-child feeding strategies and their relationships to child eating and weight status. Obesity Research. 2004;12(11):1711–1722. [PubMed]
22. Fisher JO, Birch LL. Restricting access to palatable foods affects children's behavioral response, food selection, and intake. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 1999;69(6):1264–1272. [PubMed]
23. Hodges EA, Hughes SO, Hopkinson J, Fisher JO. Maternal decisions about the initiation and termination of infant feeding. Appetite. 2008;50(2–3):333–339. [PubMed]
24. Wasser H, Bentley M, Borja J, et al. Infants perceived as "fussy" are more likely to receive complementary foods before 4 months. Pediatrics. 2011;127(2):229–237. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
25. Blissett J, Farrow C. Predictors of maternal control of feeding at 1 and 2 years of age. International Journal of Obesity. 2007;31(10):1520–1526. [PubMed]
26. Duke RE, Bryson S, Hammer LD, Agras WS. The relationship between parental factors at infancy and parent-reported control over children's eating at age 7. Appetite. 2004;43(3):247–252. [PubMed]
27. Farrow C, Blissett J. Does maternal control during feeding moderate early infant weight gain? Pediatrics. 2006;118(2):E293–E298. [PubMed]
28. Gillman MW, Ludwig DS. How early should obesity prevention start? The New England journal of medicine. 2013;369(23):2173–2175. [PubMed]
29. Paul IM, Bartok CJ, Downs DS, Stifter CA, Ventura AK, Birch LL. Opportunities for the primary prevention of obesity during infancy. Advances in pediatrics. 2009;56:107–133. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
30. Powers SW, Chamberlin LA, van Schaick KB, Sherman SN, Whitaker RC. Maternal feeding strategies, child eating behaviors, and child BMI in low-income African-American preschoolers. Obesity. 2006;14(11):2026–2033. [PubMed]
31. Gross RS, Fierman AH, Mendelsohn AL, et al. Maternal perceptions of infant hunger, satiety, and pressuring feeding styles in an urban Latina WIC population. Academic pediatrics. 2010;10(1):29–35. [PubMed]
32. Thompson AL, Mendez MA, Borja JB, Adair LS, Zimmer CR, Bentley ME. Development and validation of the Infant Feeding Style Questionnaire. Appetite. 2009;53(2):210–221. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
33. Bentley M, Gavin L, Black MM, Teti L. Infant feeding practices of low-income, African-American, adolescent mothers: an ecological, multigenerational perspective. Social Science & Medicine. 1999;49(8):1085–1100. [PubMed]
34. Sacco LM, Bentley ME, Carby-Shields K, Borja JB, Goldman BD. Assessment of infant feeding styles among low-income African-American mothers: Comparing reported and observed behaviors. Appetite. 2007;49(1):131–140. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
35. Hoerr SL, Hughes SO, Fisher JO, Nicklas TA, Liu Y, Shewchuk RM. Associations among parental feeding styles and children's food intake in families with limited incomes. The international journal of behavioral nutrition and physical activity. 2009;6:55. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
36. Gross RS, Mendelsohn AL, Fierman AH, Hauser NR, Messito MJ. Maternal Infant Feeding Behaviors and Disparities in Early Child Obesity. Childhood Obesity. 2014;10(2):145–152. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
37. Thompson AL, Adair LS, Bentley ME. Pressuring and Restrictive Feeding Styles Influence Infant Feeding and Size among a Low-Income African-American Sample. Obesity. 2013;21(3):562–571. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
38. Stifter CA, Anzman-Frasca S, Birch LL, Voegtline K. Parent use of food to soothe infant/toddler distress and child weight status. An exploratory study. Appetite. 2011;57(3):693–699. [PubMed]
39. Paul IM, Williams JS, Anzman-Frasca S, et al. The Intervention Nurses Start Infants Growing on Healthy Trajectories (INSIGHT) study. Bmc Pediatrics. 2014:14. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
40. Sanders LM, Perrin EM, Yin HS, Bronaugh A, Rothman RL. Greenlight Study T. "Greenlight Study": A Controlled Trial of Low-Literacy, Early Childhood Obesity Prevention. Pediatrics. 2014;133(6):E1724–E1737. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
41. Taveras EM, Blackburn K, Gillman MW, et al. First Steps for Mommy and Me: A Pilot Intervention to Improve Nutrition and Physical Activity Behaviors of Postpartum Mothers and Their Infants. Maternal and Child Health Journal. 2011;15(8):1217–1227. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
42. Jain A, Sherman SN, Chamberlin LA, Whitaker RC. Mothers misunderstand questions on a feeding questionnaire. Appetite. 2004;42(3):249–254. [PubMed]
43. Bronner YL, Gross SM, Caulfield L, et al. Early introduction of solid foods among urban African-American participants in WIC. Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 1999;99(4):457–461. [PubMed]
44. Corbett KS. Explaining infant feeding style of low-income black women. Journal of pediatric nursing. 2000;15(2):73–81. [PubMed]
45. Sanders LM, Perrin EM, Yin HS, Bronaugh A, Rothman RL. "Greenlight study": a controlled trial of low-literacy, early childhood obesity prevention. Pediatrics. 2014;133(6):e1724–e1737. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
46. Krassner L. TIPP usage. Pediatrics. 1984;74(5 Pt 2):976–980. [PubMed]
47. Toledo LPK, Stubbs E, et al. Salud Infantil: Understanding and Promoting the Nutritional Health of Latino Infants. Chapel Hill, NC: Carolina Population Center; 2009.
48. Marin Gea. Development of a Short Acculturation Scale for Hispanics. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences. 1987;9(2):183–205.
49. Hu Lt, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 1999;6(1):1–55.
50. Raftery A. Bayesian Model Selection in Social Research. Sociological Methodology. 1995;25:11–163.
51. Brown T. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research. 1. The Guilford Press; 2006.
52. Lumeng JC, Taveras EM, Birch L, Yanovski SZ. Prevention of Obesity in Infancy and Early Childhood: A National Institutes of Health Workshop. JAMA pediatrics. 2015 [PubMed]
53. DiSantis KI, Hodges EA, Johnson SL, Fisher JO. The role of responsive feeding in overweight during infancy and toddlerhood: a systematic review. International journal of obesity (2005) 2011;35(4):480–492. [PubMed]