PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptHHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
Sleep Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 December 16.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC3863761
NIHMSID: NIHMS531738

Iron deficiency anemia in infancy exerts long-term effects on the tibialis anterior motor activity during sleep in childhood

Abstract

Objectives

To explore the eventual connection between iron deficiency anemia (IDA) in infancy and altered leg movements during sleep in a 10-year follow-up study in children who did or did not have IDA in infancy.

Subjects and methods

Polysomnographic studies were performed in 32 10-year-old children (13 females and 19 males) who had IDA in infancy and 26 peers (10 females and 16 males) who were nonanemic controls. The time structure of their polysomnographically recorded leg movements (LM) was analyzed by means of an approach particularly able to consider their quantity, periodicity, and distribution during the night.

Results

All LM indexes and those related to periodic LM during sleep (PLMS) were slightly higher in the former IDA group than in the control group, but not always significant. The Periodicity index during NREM sleep was higher and was reflected by a small but significant increase in PLMS separated by 10–50 s intervals. PLMS index tended to be higher in former IDA children than in controls throughout the whole night.

Conclusion

The association between IDA in infancy, despite iron therapy, and PLMS in childhood could lead to new research in this area. Indeed, transient infantile IDA, a common nutritional problem among human infants, may turn out to be important for understanding the mechanisms of PLMS or restless legs syndrome, which are common in adulthood.

Keywords: Iron deficiency anemia in infancy, Periodic leg movements during sleep, Periodicity index, Non-REM sleep, Long-term effects of iron deficiency anemia in infancy, Children

1. Introduction

Iron deficiency (ID) is prevalent in human infants, especially in the late infancy/toddler period (between the ages of six and 24 months) [15]. There is mounting evidence that ID affects motor activity, not only in animal studies [612], but also in humans. In the adult human, maximal physical performance, submaximal endurance, and work productivity have been shown to be reduced in iron deficiency anemia (IDA) and, in some cases, in ID without anemia [1317]. The few published studies of ID and motor activity in school-aged children report decreased activity while awake [18]. In the human infant Lozoff et al. [19] found decreased activity in a study of play behavior and mother-infant interaction. In another study of 12- to 18-month-old undernourished infants with IDA, Harahap et al. [20] found reduced Bayley motor scores and motor activity scores, in comparison to controls, before iron therapy. Following iron treatment, the activity of IDA infants increased to a larger extent than controls. Using actigraphs, in a laboratory setting, Angulo-Kinzler et al. [21] found decreased frequency of motor activity at 12 and 18 months in infants who were anemic (IDA) at six months and received iron treatment beginning at that age. These results were limited to a short-term period of observation (activity during the waking time immediately preceding and following an afternoon nap in the laboratory). Using 24-h acti-graphic recordings in the home, Angulo-Kinzler et al. [22] reported that six-month-old infants with IDA showed an overall increase in motor activity compared to controls, with actigraphs worn on the ankle; differences were no longer observed at 12 and 18 months of age after effective iron supplementation treatment. We hypothesized that increased leg activity during the period of IDA might indicate a shared underlying mechanism with restless legs syndrome (RLS). RLS is a sensorimotor dysfunction where ID is suspected to play an important pathogenic role. ID might trigger RLS symptoms and might worsen a pre-existing RLS form, and iron supplementation can ameliorate them [23], but its relation to IDA in infancy has not been investigated.

We had a chance to explore a connection between IDA in infancy and altered leg movements in a 10-year follow-up performed at INTA, University of Chile, within an ongoing collaborative research project with University of Michigan. We studied motor activity during sleep as recorded from the tibialis anterior muscles of 10-year-old children who did or did not have IDA in infancy. We focused on the so-called periodic leg movements during sleep (PLMS), a typical sleep-related motor phenomenon usually found in more than 80% of affected individuals with RLS [24]. We analyzed PLMS by means of a new approach that has been shown to be particularly able to describe the periodicity of this motor pattern [25].

2. Subjects and methods

2.1. Subjects

The children in this study are participants in ongoing research in Chile on the behavioral, developmental, neurosensory, cognitive, and sleep–wake patterns effects of IDA in infancy. Detailed descriptions of the population and findings during infancy have been published elsewhere [26,27]. In brief, study participants were healthy full-term infants (birth weights ≥3.0 kg, no perinatal complications, and no acute or chronic illnesses). Infants were assessed for IDA at 6, 12, and 18 months. Anemia was defined as venous hemoglobin ≤100 g/L at six months and <110 g/L at 12 and 18 months (for details see Lozoff et al. [26]). ID was defined as two of three iron measures in the iron-deficient range (mean cell volume <70 fl, erythrocyte protoporphyrin >100 μg/dL red blood cells [1.77 μmol/L], serum ferritin <12 μg/L). For each IDA infant, an infant of the same age who was clearly nonanemic (venous hemoglobin ≥115 g/L) was randomly selected. They constituted the “control” group. Six-month-old infants were treated for one year with 15 mg/day of elemental iron as oral ferrous sulphate; infants identified at 12 or 18 months were treated with oral iron (30 mg/day) for a minimum of six months. Given that IDA in infancy was very common in the population at the time, infants from the control group underwent the same iron treatment in order to assure they did not become anemic with advancing age.

As part of the 10-year-old follow-up, we compared tibialis anterior activity patterns during sleep between 32 children (13 females and 19 males) who had IDA in infancy (former IDA) and 26 children (10 females and 16 males) who were nonanemic controls. All were healthy and not taking any medications at the time of polysomnographic recording. The sex distribution was not different between the two groups (Chi-square = 0.001, d.f. = 1, p = 0.963). Table 1 shows the background characteristics of the groups. There were no statistically significant differences in child or family factors, including attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) subtype (aggressive, impulsive, hyperactive) score and apnea/hypopnea index (AHI) in children, as well as the prevalence of a positive family history of RLS in one or both biologic parents.

Table 1
Background characteristics of the two groups of subjects studied.

The research protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of Michigan Medical Center, Ann Arbor, of INTA, University of Chile, Santiago, and of the Office of Protection from Research Risks, NIH. Parental signed informed consent and child assent was obtained.

2.2. Nocturnal polysomnography

Subjects were allowed to sleep until they spontaneously woke up in the morning. Lights-out time was based on individual habitual bed time and ranged between 9:30 and 11:30 p.m. The following signals were recorded during polysomnography: EEG (six channels, F4, C4, O2, F3, C3, O1, referred to the contralateral earlobe); electrooculogram (electrodes placed 1 cm above the right outer cantus and 1 cm below the left outer cantus and referred to A1), electromyogram (EMG) of the submentalis muscle, ECG (one derivation), body position, and temperature. EMG of the right and left tibialis anterior muscles (bipolar derivations with two electrodes placed 3 cm apart on the belly of the anterior tibialis muscle of each leg, impedance was kept less than 10 KX), according to the WASM-IRLSSG scoring criteria [28]. Sleep signals were sampled at 200 Hz and stored on hard disk in European data format (EDF, see Kemp et al. [29] for details) for further analysis. EMG signals, in particular, were digitally band-pass filtered at 10–100 Hz, with a notch filter at 50 Hz. The sleep respiratory pattern of each patient was also monitored using nasal pressure cannula and oral airflow thermistor, abdominal respiratory effort strain gauge, snoring by microphone, and by monitoring oxygen saturation (pulse-oxymetry). Subjects with an AHI ≥5 were not included. All sleep measures were acquired and processed without knowledge of whether a given child was former IDA or control.

2.3. Sleep scoring and detection of leg movements

Prior to any recording we verified that the EMG amplitude recorded from the two tibialis anterior muscles was below 2 μV at rest and exceeded 7–10 μV for small voluntary flexions of the foot. EMG amplitude at maximal deflection was also measured for the application of the WASM-IRLSSG scoring criteria [28]. In all subjects, sleep was subdivided into 30-s epochs, and sleep stages were scored according to the standard criteria by Rechtschaffen and Kales [30] using the sleep analysis software Hypnolab 1.2 (SWS Soft, Italy). Leg movements (LMs) during sleep were first detected by the same software that allows their computer-assisted detection. With this software, the detection is performed using a human-supervised automatic approach controlled by the scorer that uses the WASM-IRLSSG scoring criteria [28]. The performance of this system has been evaluated and validated [31]. In this study one scorer also visually edited the detections proposed by the automatic analysis before computing a final result. The total LM index was calculated as the total number of LMs per hour of sleep. The PLMS index was calculated as the number of LMs in a series of four or more, separated by more than 5 and less than 90 s, per hour of sleep [28].

2.4. Further analysis of leg movement architecture

In this analysis we used the newly described approach to study LM periodicity [25]. These parameters are in line with those proposed by the WASM-IRLSSG [28] and include, in particular, the Periodicity Index computed as the number of sequences of three inter-LM intervals 10 < i ≤ 90 s/total number of inter-LM intervals [25]. This index can vary between zero (absence of periodicity) to one (all intervals with length 10 < i ≤ 90 s). Periodicity index is independent of the absolute number of LMs recorded.

2.5. Statistical analysis

The comparison between the different sleep scoring and LM parameters obtained from children with or without IDA in infancy was carried out by means of the Student’s t-test for independent data sets after controlling for the normality of distribution of data. The comparison between groups in the number of PLMS during each hour of the night was performed using a zero-inflated Poisson regression model [32]; this approach was chosen due to the excess number of zero PLMS for individual hours of the night. Statistical analyses were conducted using SAS and STATISTICA data analysis software system version 6. (StatSoft Inc., 2001. www.statsoft.com).

3. Results

3.1. Sleep architecture

Table 2 reports the sleep architecture parameters in the two groups of children. Former IDA children showed a longer sleep latency and lower number of stage shifts and awakenings than controls. However, these differences did not pass the conservative Bonferroni correction.

Table 2
Comparison between sleep architecture parameters obtained in the two groups of children.

3.2. Leg motor activity

Table 3 reports the parameters derived from the analysis of the tibialis anterior EMG activity. All LM indexes and those related to PLMS were slightly higher in former IDA subjects than in controls. However, only the differences in total and NREM sleep PLMS index reached statistical significance. Former IDA subjects showed significantly longer LM than controls in both REM and NREM sleep; however, none of these differences remained significant after the Bonferroni correction. The total sleep Periodicity index and that calculated for NREM were significantly higher in former IDA children than in normal controls; also, these differences did not pass the conservative Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

Table 3
Comparison between the LM activity parameters obtained in the two groups of children.

3.3. Analysis of inter-LM intervals

Fig. 1 shows the distribution of inter-LM intervals in former IDA children and controls. Former IDA children showed higher values than normal controls for several interval classes depicted in this figure, mostly for those in the range between 10 and 50 s. Consistent with the results obtained for the Periodicity index (Table 3), statistically significant differences were found for some of these interval classes, as indicated by asterisks.

Fig. 1
Distribution histograms of inter-LM intervals during sleep of the two subject groups. All values are shown as mean and SEM (whiskers). Asterisks indicate the points for which a significant difference was found between the graphs at statistical analysis ...

3.4. Night distribution of LMs

Fig. 2 compares the distribution of the number of PLMS and isolated LM and total LM per hour of sleep (first eight hours shown) between former IDA and control children. PLMS tended to be higher in former IDA children than in controls throughout the whole night. The differences were statistically significant for the first, third, sixth, seventh, and eighth hour of recording and marginally significant for the fourth hour (p = 0.071). In general, the differences between the two groups seemed to be more evident during the last three hours of the night. Both groups showed a trend for the number of PLMS to decrease throughout the night. There were no group differences for the distribution of isolated LMs, which showed a similar number in all hours of the night.

Fig. 2
Comparison between the distribution of number of PLMS (top panel) and isolated LMs (bottom panel) per hour of sleep (first 8 h shown) between subject groups. All values are shown as mean and SEM (whiskers). Asterisks indicate the points for which a significant ...

4. Discussion

The main finding of this study is that 10-year-old children who experienced IDA in infancy showed a mild but significant increase of tibialis anterior EMG activity during sleep when compared to age-matched normal controls. The activity is characterized by a slightly but significantly higher periodicity due to a selective increase of muscle activations separated by an interval ranging approximately 10–50 s. Taken all together, these findings point to the possibility that long-term consequences of IDA in infancy, despite iron therapy, can be detected during sleep [27].

Since groups were similar in several child or family factors, including ADHD score and AHI in children, and the prevalence of a positive family history of RLS, differences between groups do not appear to be influenced by these potential confounding variables. Also, given that only three subjects were included in this study and in the infancy one [21], it is difficult to have a hypothesis regarding the fact that there were no differences in leg activity between groups at 1–1.5 years of age and the difference in PLMS index several years later. Further, the methodology used for leg movement detection in infancy (actigraphy) and childhood (polysomnography, tibialis anterior EMG) studies might help in understanding the absence in infancy and the presence of leg activity differences in childhood.

Considering that the diagnosis of definite RLS in children and adolescents could be preceded by many years of persistent sleep disturbances, periodic limb movement disorder (PLMD), or possible/probable RLS [33], a developmental approach deserves special attention. In this context, children with PLMD have been characterized as having important clinical and polysomnographic correlates, including sleep onset and maintenance problems, leg pain/discomfort at night, parasomnias, getting out of bed at night, and family history of RLS [34]. On PSG recordings they present more awakenings, stage 1 sleep, stage shifts, and spontaneous arousals. In our study, compared to controls, former IDA children show similar criteria prevalence for RLS [35], but have longer sleep latency in PSG recordings that could relate to sleep onset problems seen in PLMD children. However, they also have other characteristics that differ from the PLMD patterns. For instance, based on parental report, former IDA children were less frequently identified as restless sleepers (data not shown) and presented less awakenings and stage shifts throughout nighttime PSG recordings relative to controls.

In order to understand how transient infantile IDA might influence PLMS approximately 10 years later, it is important to consider the neurophysiology of PLMS. The link could be via dopamine; dopamine D3 subtype receptors are probably directly involved in the pathogenesis of PLMS and RLS symptoms [3638]. These receptors appear to be concentrated in the limbic system and in the superficial layers of the dorsal horns of the cervical and lumbar regions and intermedio-lateralis gray matter of the spinal cord [3942]. Spinal D3 subtype receptors receive dopaminergic projections from the A11 nuclei, located within the lateral hypothalamus, which are suspected to play a role in the pathogenesis of RLS and PLM and are closely related to the central circadian pacemaker of the suprachiasmatic nuclei [43]. In the spinal cord, dopaminergic actions from the A11 exert direct inhibitory actions on dorsal neurons and on the sympathetic preganglionic neurons in the intermediolateral nucleus. It is thought that A11 similarly limits activity in the raphe-spinal projections and that a compromise in A11 inhibitory function might be able to shift the balance of descending control of the sympathetic preganglionics neurons toward excitation [44]. This view is supported by the well-known and marked reduction of PLMS induced by dopamine-agonist therapy [45,46] (the first-choice treatment for RLS [4749]), which selectively reduces a sub-population of the whole LM activity during sleep, with inter-movement interval of 6–46 s and ranging 2–4 s in duration [50]. This category of LM is especially sensitive to dopaminergic substances targeting the D3 receptor subtype [51].

Even though it is not clearly established whether dopamine-antagonists induce PLMS, centrally active dopaminergic antagonists exacerbate existing PLMS and clinically worsen RLS, but this was not the case for a peripherally active dopamine-antagonist [52]. These findings emphasize the involvement of central nervous system dopaminergic function (presumably at the subcortical levels of the brain) in the pathophysiology of PLMS.

Neurotransmitter systems are maturing during key periods of high risk for ID, and iron is essential for a number of enzymes involved in neurotransmitter synthesis, including tryptophan hydroxylase (serotonin) and tyrosine hydroxylase (norepinephrine and dopamine) [53]. Relevant to the issue of long-lasting effects are findings in rodent studies showing that only some of the alterations in dopamine, serotonin, and noradrenergic transporters and receptors were reversible by iron supplementation at weaning [12,54,55]. Therefore, reduced iron in the brain might cause reduced dopaminergic function by decreasing the activity of tyrosine hydroxylase or the expression of transporters or receptors for dopamine [56]. The ID hypothesis for RLS is based on autopsy and MRI studies reporting lower iron levels in the substantia nigra of RLS patients [57,58], on the lower cerebral spinal fluid ferritin and iron concentrations found in RLS patients with normal serum iron stores [59,60], and on the impaired capacity to store iron in the brain of patients with early onset RLS compared to patients with late-onset RLS [59]. Finally, iron therapy has been demonstrated to be effective on RLS as well as on PLMS in adult patients [23,61] and to be associated with clinical improvement in children with PLMS [62,63].

Our findings show that transient IDA in infancy is associated with a neurophysiological “scar,” as shown by our detailed analysis of PLMS. The changes may be connected to a subtle but polysomnographically detectable dopaminergic dysfunction, even at 10 years of age. This is a period of life when clinically relevant RLS is uncommon [64] and PLMS are not usually observed in normal conditions [65]. PLMS do not seem to show particular periodicity, even in children of this age with clinical conditions characterized by increased leg motor activity during sleep [66,67]. The statistically significant difference observed in this study for PLMS index and periodicity index cannot be translated into a clinical significance at this stage and only the long-term follow-up of these children will be able to provide information on its eventual predictive value of an eventual clinical consequence. There are no population-based or longitudinal data that assess what proportion of individuals with PLMS eventually exhibit some or all of the diagnostic criteria for RLS. However, it is now accepted that PLMS are an endophenotype of RLS [68]. Asymptomatic PLMS are often a precursor to RLS [33,69], and they are more common in relatives of RLS patients [70,71] and more prevalent and abundant in ethnic groups that have the highest frequencies of RLS/PLMS risk alleles [72]. The possibility that a history of a transient ID might induce a late-onset RLS, even after the re-establishment of normal values of ferritin, recalls to some extent the symptomatic RLS form associated with pregnancy. Pregnancy is a strong risk factor for both ID and a transient form of RLS, which usually disappears after delivery; however, women who experience RLS symptoms during pregnancy are more likely to develop a chronic form of RLS later in their life compared to women who do not [73,74]. Moreover, there is a positive correlation between the number of pregnancies and the risk to be affected by RLS [75].

It is not possible to know, at this stage, if IDA in infancy is a risk factor for the development of RLS later in life. However, our findings warrant continued follow-up. If this sleep motor phenomenon is further enhanced with advancing age, transient infantile IDA, a common nutritional problem among human infants [15], may turn out to be important for understanding the mechanisms of PLMS and, maybe, of RLS, a common sleep disturbance of adulthood [64,76].

Acknowledgments

We would like to express our gratitude to the children and parents whose ongoing participation made this follow-up possible. We also thank all technicians who contributed to nighttime recordings during the course of this study and drivers for providing careful transportation services to children and parents. Statement of financial support: This study received grants from the National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA (R01 HD33487) and the Chilean Agency for Funding in Science and Technology (CONICYT, Fondecyt 1070668).

The content of this article is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development or the National Institutes of Health.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest The ICMJE Uniform Disclosure Form for Potential Conflicts of Interest associated with this article can be viewed by clicking on the following link: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2012.05.011.

References

[1] Darnton-Hill I, Mora JO, Weinstein H, Wilbur S, Nalubola PR. Iron and folate fortification in the Americas to prevent and control micronutrient malnutrition: An analysis. Nutr Rev. 1999;57(1):25–31. [PubMed]
[2] DeMaeyer E, Adiels-Tegman M. The prevalence of anaemia in the world. World Health Stat Q. 1985;38(3):302–16. [PubMed]
[3] Fiorentino RF, Guirriec RM. Prevalence of nutritional anemia in infancy and childhood with emphasis on developing countries. In: Stekel A, editor. Iron nutrition in infancy and childhood. Raven Press; New York: 1984. pp. 61–74.
[4] Freire WB. Strategies of the Pan American Health Organization/World Health Organization for the control of iron deficiency in Latin America. Nutr Rev. 1997;55(6):183–8. [PubMed]
[5] Stoltzfus R. Defining iron-deficiency anemia in public health terms: A time for reflection. J Nutr. 2001;131(2S-2):565S–7S. [PubMed]
[6] Hunt JR, Zito CA, Erjavec J, Johnson LK. Severe or marginal iron deficiency affects spontaneous physical activity in rats. Am J Clin Nutr. 1994;59(2):413–8. [PubMed]
[7] Felt BT, Lozoff B. Brain iron and behavior of rats are not normalized by treatment of iron deficiency anemia during early development. J Nutr. 1996;126(3):693–701. [PubMed]
[8] Glover J, Jacobs A. Activity pattern of iron-deficient rats. Br Med J. 1972;2(5814):627–8. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[9] Munro N. A three year study of iron deficiency and behavior in rhesus monkeys. Int J Biosoc Res. 1987;9:35–62.
[10] Pinero D, Jones B, Beard J. Variations in dietary iron alter behavior in developing rats. J Nutr. 2001 Feb;131(2):311–8. [PubMed]
[11] Youdim MB, Yehuda S, Ben-Uriah Y. Iron deficiency-induced circadian rhythm reversal of dopaminergic-mediated behaviours and thermoregulation in rats. Eur J Pharmacol. 1981;74(4):295–301. [PubMed]
[12] Felt BT, Beard JL, Schallert T, Shao J, Aldridge JW, Connor JR, et al. Persistent neurochemical and behavioral abnormalities in adulthood despite early iron supplementation for perinatal iron deficiency anemia in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2006;171(2):261–70. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[13] Dallman PR. Iron deficiency: Distinguishing the effects of anemia from muscle iron deficiency on work performance. In: Saltman P, Hegenauer J, editors. The biochemistry and physiology of iron. Elsevier North Holland; Amsterdam: 1982. pp. 509–23.
[14] Edgerton VR, Gardner GW, Ohira Y, Gunawardena KA, Senewiratne B. Iron-deficiency anaemia and its effect on worker productivity and activity patterns. Br Med J. 1979;2(6204):1546–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[15] Lozoff B, Brittenham GM. Behavioral aspects of iron deficiency. Prog Hematol. 1986;14:23–53. [PubMed]
[16] Stoltzfus RJ. Iron-deficiency anemia: Reexamining the nature and magnitude of the public health problem. Summary: Implications for research and programs. J Nutr. 2001;131(2S–2):697S–700S. discussion S–1S. [PubMed]
[17] Zhu YI, Haas JD. Altered metabolic response of iron-depleted nonanemic women during a 15-km time trial. J Appl Physiol. 1998;84(5):1768–75. [PubMed]
[18] Bhatia D, Seshadri S. Anemia, undernutrition and physical work capacity of young boys. Indian Pediatr. 1987;24(2):133–9. [PubMed]
[19] Lozoff B, Klein NK, Nelson EC, McClish DK, Manuel M, Chacon ME. Behavior of infants with iron-deficiency anemia. Child Dev. 1998;69(1):24–36. [PubMed]
[20] Harahap H, Jahari AB, Husaini MA, Saco-Pollitt C, Pollitt E. Effects of an energy and micronutrient supplement on iron deficiency anemia, physical activity and motor and mental development in undernourished children in Indonesia. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2000;54(Suppl. 2):S114–9. [PubMed]
[21] Angulo-Kinzler RM, Peirano P, Lin E, Garrido M, Lozoff B. Spontaneous motor activity in human infants with iron-deficiency anemia. Early Hum Dev. 2002;66(2):67–79. [PubMed]
[22] Angulo-Kinzler RM, Peirano P, Lin E, Algarin C, Garrido M, Lozoff B. Twenty-four-hour motor activity in human infants with and without iron deficiency anemia. Early Hum Dev. 2002;70(1–2):85–101. [PubMed]
[23] Allen RP, Earley CJ. The role of iron in restless legs syndrome. Mov Disord. 2007;22(Suppl. 18):S440–8. [PubMed]
[24] American Academy of Sleep Medicine International Classification of Sleep Disorders. American Academy of Sleep Medicine; Westchester, Illinois: 2005.
[25] Ferri R, Zucconi M, Manconi M, Plazzi G, Bruni O, Ferini-Strambi L. New approaches to the study of periodic leg movements during sleep in restless legs syndrome. Sleep. 2006;29(6):759–69. [PubMed]
[26] Lozoff B, De Andraca I, Castillo M, Smith JB, Walter T, Pino P. Behavioral and developmental effects of preventing iron-deficiency anemia in healthy full-term infants. Pediatrics. 2003;112(4):846–54. [PubMed]
[27] Peirano PD, Algarin CR, Chamorro RA, Reyes SC, Duran SA, Garrido MI, et al. Sleep alterations and iron deficiency anemia in infancy. Sleep Med. 2010;11(7):637–42. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[28] Zucconi M, Ferri R, Allen R, Baier PC, Bruni O, Chokroverty S, et al. The official World Association of Sleep Medicine (WASM) standards for recording and scoring periodic leg movements in sleep (PLMS) and wakefulness (PLMW) developed in collaboration with a task force from the International Restless Legs Syndrome Study Group (IRLSSG) Sleep Med. 2006;7(2):175–83. [PubMed]
[29] Kemp B, Varri A, Rosa AC, Nielsen KD, Gade J. A simple format for exchange of digitized polygraphic recordings. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1992;82(5):391–3. [PubMed]
[30] Rechtschaffen A, Kales A. In: A manual of standardized terminology, techniques and scoring system for sleep stages of human subjects. Rechtschaffen A, Kales A, editors. BI/BR; Los Angeles, CA: 1968. [PubMed]
[31] Ferri R, Zucconi M, Manconi M, Bruni O, Miano S, Plazzi G, et al. Computer-assisted detection of nocturnal leg motor activity in patients with restless legs syndrome and periodic leg movements during sleep. Sleep. 2005;28(8):998–1004. [PubMed]
[32] Lambert D. Zero-inflated Poisson regression, with application to defects in manufacturing. Technometrics. 1992;34:1–14.
[33] Picchietti DL, Stevens HE. Early manifestations of restless legs syndrome in childhood and adolescence. Sleep Med. 2008;9(7):770–81. [PubMed]
[34] Gingras JL, Gaultney JF, Picchietti DL. Pediatric periodic limb movement disorder: Sleep symptom and polysomnographic correlates compared to obstructive sleep apnea. J Clin Sleep Med. 2011;7(6):603–609A. [PubMed]
[35] Allen RP, Picchietti D, Hening WA, Trenkwalder C, Walters AS, Montplaisi J. Restless legs syndrome: Diagnostic criteria, special considerations, and epidemiology. A report from the restless legs syndrome diagnosis and epidemiology workshop at the National Institutes of Health. Sleep Med. 2003;4(2):101–19. [PubMed]
[36] Allen RP, Earley CJ. Validation of the Johns Hopkins restless legs severity scale. Sleep Med. 2001;2(3):239–42. [PubMed]
[37] Garcia-Borreguero D, Larrosa O, de la Llave Y, Granizo JJ, Allen R. Correlation between rating scales and sleep laboratory measurements in restless legs syndrome. Sleep Med. 2004;5(6):561–5. [PubMed]
[38] Montplaisir J, Lorrain D, Godbout R. Restless legs syndrome and periodic leg movements in sleep: The primary role of dopaminergic mechanism. Eur Neurol. 1991;31(1):41–3. [PubMed]
[39] Austin J. Schizophrenia: An update and review. J Genet Couns. 2005;14(5):329–40. [PubMed]
[40] Bordet R. Central dopamine receptors: General considerations (Part 1) Rev Neurol (Paris) 2004;160(8–9):862–70. [PubMed]
[41] Levant B, McCarson KE. D(3) dopamine receptors in rat spinal cord: Implications for sensory and motor function. Neurosci Lett. 2001;303(1):9–12. [PubMed]
[42] Nieoullon A, Amalric M. Dopaminergic receptors: Structural features and functional implications. Rev Neurol (Paris) 2002;158(Spec no. 1):S59–68. [PubMed]
[43] Ondo WG, He Y, Rajasekaran S, Le WD. Clinical correlates of 6-hydroxydopamine injections into A11 dopaminergic neurons in rats: A possible model for restless legs syndrome. Mov Disord. 2000;15(1):154–8. [PubMed]
[44] Clemens S, Rye D, Hochman S. Restless legs syndrome: Revisiting the dopamine hypothesis from the spinal cord perspective. Neurology. 2006;67(1):125–30. [PubMed]
[45] Manconi M, Ferri R, Zucconi M, Oldani A, Fantini ML, Castronovo V, et al. First night efficacy of pramipexole in restless legs syndrome and periodic leg movements. Sleep Med. 2007;8(5):491–7. [PubMed]
[46] Saletu M, Anderer P, Saletu B, Hauer C, Mandl M, Semler B, et al. Sleep laboratory studies in periodic limb movement disorder (PLMD) patients as compared with normals and acute effects of ropinirole. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2001;16(2):177–87. [PubMed]
[47] Trenkwalder C, Garcia-Borreguero D, Montagna P, Lainey E, de Weerd AW, Tidswell P, et al. Ropinirole in the treatment of restless legs syndrome: Results from the TREAT RLS 1 study, a 12 week, randomised, placebo controlled study in 10 European countries. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2004;75(1):92–7. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[48] Trenkwalder C, Hundemer HP, Lledo A, Swieca J, Polo O, Wetter TC, et al. Efficacy of pergolide in treatment of restless legs syndrome: The PEARLS Study. Neurology. 2004;62(8):1391–7. [PubMed]
[49] Walters AS, Ondo WG, Dreykluft T, Grunstein R, Lee D, Sethi K. Ropinirole is effective in the treatment of restless legs syndrome. TREAT RLS 2: A 12-week, double-blind, randomized, parallel-group, placebo-controlled study. Mov Disord. 2004;19(12):1414–23. [PubMed]
[50] Manconi M, Ferri R, Feroah TR, Zucconi M, Ferini-Strambi L. Defining the boundaries of the response of sleep leg movements to a single dose of dopamine agonist. Sleep. 2008;31(9):1229–37. [PubMed]
[51] Manconi M, Ferri R, Zucconi M, Clemens S, Giarolli L, Bottasini V, et al. Preferential D2 or preferential D3 dopamine agonists in restless legs syndrome. Neurology. 2011;77(2):110–7. [PubMed]
[52] Wetter TC, Stiasny K, Winkelmann J, Buhlinger A, Brandenburg U, Penzel T, et al. A randomized controlled study of pergolide in patients with restless legs syndrome. Neurology. 1999;52(5):944–50. [PubMed]
[53] Wigglesworth JM, Baum H. Iron dependent enzymes in the brain. In: Youdim MBH, editor. Brain iron: Neurochemical and behavioural aspects. Taylor and Francis; New York: 1988. pp. 25–66.
[54] Beard JL, Felt B, Schallert T, Burhans M, Connor JR, Georgieff MK. Moderate iron deficiency in infancy: Biology and behavior in young rats. Behav Brain Res. 2006;170(2):224–32. [PubMed]
[55] Burhans MS, Dailey C, Beard Z, Wiesinger J, Murray-Kolb L, Jones BC, et al. Iron deficiency: Differential effects on monoamine transporters. Nutr Neurosci. 2005;8(1):31–8. [PubMed]
[56] Beard JL, Connor JR. Iron status and neural functioning. Annu Rev Nutr. 2003;23:41–58. [PubMed]
[57] Allen RP, Barker PB, Wehrl F, Song HK, Earley CJ. MRI measurement of brain iron in patients with restless legs syndrome. Neurology. 2001;56(2):263–5. [PubMed]
[58] Connor JR, Wang XS, Patton SM, Menzies SL, Troncoso JC, Earley CJ, et al. Decreased transferrin receptor expression by neuromelanin cells in restless legs syndrome. Neurology. 2004;62(9):1563–7. [PubMed]
[59] Clardy SL, Earley CJ, Allen RP, Beard JL, Connor JR. Ferritin subunits in CSF are decreased in restless legs syndrome. J Lab Clin Med. 2006;147(2):67–73. [PubMed]
[60] Mizuno S, Mihara T, Miyaoka T, Inagaki T, Horiguchi J. CSF iron, ferritin and transferrin levels in restless legs syndrome. J Sleep Res. 2005;14(1):43–7. [PubMed]
[61] Earley CJ, Heckler D, Allen RP. The treatment of restless legs syndrome with intravenous iron dextran. Sleep Med. 2004;5(3):231–5. [PubMed]
[62] Simakajornboon N, Gozal D, Vlasic V, Mack C, Sharon D, McGinley BM. Periodic limb movements in sleep and iron status in children. Sleep. 2003;26(6):735–8. [PubMed]
[63] Simakajornboon N, Kheirandish-Gozal L, Gozal D. Diagnosis and management of restless legs syndrome in children. Sleep Med Rev. 2009;13(2):149–56. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[64] Allen RP, Walters AS, Montplaisir J, Hening W, Myers A, Bell TJ, et al. Restless legs syndrome prevalence and impact: REST general population study. Arch Intern Med. 2005;165(11):1286–92. [PubMed]
[65] Pennestri MH, Whittom S, Adam B, Petit D, Carrier J, Montplaisir J. PLMS and PLMW in healthy subjects as a function of age: Prevalence and interval distribution. Sleep. 2006;29(9):1183–7. [PubMed]
[66] Ferri R, Franceschini C, Zucconi M, Drago V, Manconi M, Vandi S, et al. Sleep polygraphic study of children and adolescents with narcolepsy/cataplexy. Dev Neuropsychol. 2009;34(5):523–38. [PubMed]
[67] Ferri R, Manconi M, Lanuzza B, Cosentino FI, Bruni O, Ferini-Strambi L, et al. Age-related changes in periodic leg movements during sleep in patients with restless legs syndrome. Sleep Med. 2008;9(7):790–8. [PubMed]
[68] Walters AS, Picchietti D, Hening W, Lazzarini A. Variable expressivity in familial restless legs syndrome. Arch Neurol. 1990;47(11):1219–20. [PubMed]
[69] Winkelman JW. Periodic limb movements in sleep – Endophenotype for restless legs syndrome? N Engl J Med. 2007;357(7):703–5. [PubMed]
[70] Birinyi PV, Allen RP, Hening W, Washburn T, Lesage S, Earley CJ. Undiagnosed individuals with first-degree relatives with restless legs syndrome have increased periodic limb movements. Sleep Med. 2006;7(6):480–5. [PubMed]
[71] Bonati MT, Ferini-Strambi L, Aridon P, Oldani A, Zucconi M, Casari G. Autosomal dominant restless legs syndrome maps on chromosome 14q. Brain. 2003;126(Pt 6):1485–92. [PubMed]
[72] O’Brien LM, Holbrook CR, Faye Jones V, Gozal D. Ethnic difference in periodic limb movements in children. Sleep Med. 2007;8(3):240–6. [PubMed]
[73] Cesnik E, Casetta I, Turri M, Govoni V, Granieri E, Strambi LF, et al. Transient RLS during pregnancy is a risk factor for the chronic idiopathic form. Neurology. 2010;75(23):2117–20. [PubMed]
[74] Manconi M, Govoni V, De Vito A, Economou NT, Cesnik E, Casetta I, et al. Restless legs syndrome and pregnancy. Neurology. 2004;63(6):1065–9. [PubMed]
[75] Berger K, Luedemann J, Trenkwalder C, John U, Kessler C. Sex and the risk of restless legs syndrome in the general population. Arch Intern Med. 2004;164(2):196–202. [PubMed]
[76] Hening W, Walters AS, Allen RP, Montplaisir J, Myers A, Ferini-Strambi L. Impact, diagnosis and treatment of restless legs syndrome (RLS) in a primary care population: The REST (RLS epidemiology, symptoms, and treatment) primary care study. Sleep Med. 2004;5(3):237–46. [PubMed]