PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptHHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
J Atten Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 August 19.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC3746768
NIHMSID: NIHMS499448

Anterior Cingulate Volumetric Alterations in Treatment-Naïve Adults With ADHD

A Pilot Study
Nikos Makris, M.D., Ph.D., Larry J. Seidman, Ph.D., Eve M. Valera, Ph.D., Joseph Biederman, M.D., Michael C. Monuteaux, Sc.D., David N. Kennedy, Ph.D., Verne S. Caviness, Jr., M.D., D.Phil., George Bush, M.D., M.M.Sc., Katherine Crum, Ariel B. Brown, Ph.D., and Stephen V. Faraone, Ph.D.

Abstract

Objective

We sought to examine preliminary results of brain alterations in anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) in treatment-naïve adults with ADHD. The ACC is a central brain node for the integration of cognitive control and allocation of attention, affect and drive. Thus its anatomical alteration may give rise to impulsivity, hyperactivity and inattention, which are cardinal behavioral manifestations of ADHD.

Method

Segmentation and parcellation of the ACC was performed on controls (n = 22), treated (n = 13) and treatment-naïve adults with ADHD (n = 13).

Results

There was a 21% volume reduction in the left ACC of the treatment-naïve group relative to the control group. Also, there was a 23% volume reduction in the right ACC of the treated group relative to the control group.

Conclusion

These results raise the possibility that in ADHD there are volumetric deficits persistent into adulthood, that are independent of medical treatment.

Keywords: ADHD, treatment-naïve ADHD, ACC, volumetry, morphometry, MRI

Introduction

ADHD is estimated to affect 5% to 10% of children and 4% of adults (Kessler & Merikangas, 2004). Emerging evidence shows that like its pediatric counterpart ADHD in adults is a highly morbid disorder (Biederman et al., 2006). Given its severe morbidity, understanding the neural basis of ADHD is of high clinical, scientific, and public health importance.

Convergent data from neuroimaging, neuropsychological, genetics, and neurochemical studies have implicated dysfunction of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and parietal cortex (together, the cingulo-frontal-parietal [CFP] cognitive-attention network), along with striatum and cerebellum, as contributing to the pathophysiology of ADHD (Bush et al., 2008). The ACC findings are particularly consistent, with many imaging studies reporting ACC hypofunction (Bush et al., 1999; Bush, Valera, & Seidman, 2005; Durston et al., 2003; Ernst et al., 2003; Konrad, Neufang, Hanisch, Fink, & Herpertz-Dahlmann, 2006; Pliszka et al., 2006; Rubia et al., 1999; Tamm, Menon, Ringel, & Reiss, 2004; Zametkin et al., 1990) or structural abnormalities (Makris, Biederman et al., 2007; Makris, Buka et al., 2008; Seidman et al., 2006; Shaw et al., 2006). The ACC plays key roles in cognition, attention, target detection, motor control (response selection and inhibition), error detection and feedback-based decision-making. Our own prior structural work has shown the ACC to have the largest volume reduction in adults with ADHD (13.9%) of any of the structures we investigated. However, because this literature is based on samples of largely treated subjects, uncertainties remain as to whether these findings reflect the neural underpinning of the disorder or the complications of its treatment. Thus, in this study, we focus in on the ACC in never-treated adults with ADHD.

To-date there is only one study showing volumetric decrease on the right ACC in treatment-naïve children with ADHD relative to treated children with ADHD and controls (Pliszka et al., 2006). However, there is currently no study to our knowledge regarding the nature of cerebral alterations in treatment-naïve adults with ADHD. This paucity of information represents a serious void in our knowledge base on ADHD calling for studies specifically addressing treatment-naïve subjects with ADHD.

The principal focus of this study was to delineate the nature of ACC alterations in treatment-naïve adults with ADHD. To this end, we hypothesized volumetric MRI reductions in ACC in treatment-naïve adults with ADHD compared to community controls using T1-weighted MRI. This is, to our knowledge, the first study reporting volumetric alterations in treatment-naïve adults with ADHD.

Method

Methods are described in detail in Biederman et al. (2008) and Seidman et al. (2006). This study consists of a subset of ADHD subjects from the previous reports and a larger study of persons with ADHD varying on a number of dimensions, including use of medications. Briefly, males and females between the ages of 18 and 59 were eligible for the study. Treatment-naïve and treated subjects with ADHD as well as control adults were group matched to be comparable on age, sex, handedness, and education (Table 1). Exclusion criteria were deafness, blindness, psychosis, neurological disorder, sensorimotor handicaps, inadequate command of the English language, or an estimated Full Scale intelligence quotient (IQ) estimate <80 as measured by Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised (WAIS-R; Wechsler, 1981). We recruited ADHD subjects from referrals to psychiatric clinics at the Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH) and advertisements in the greater Boston area, and control subjects through advertisements in the same geographical areas. After complete description of the study to the participants, written informed consent was obtained, and all participants received an honorarium for participating. The study was approved by the MGH Human Subjects Institutional Review Board (IRB) committee.

Table 1
Demographic Characteristics

Clinical Assessment Measures

ADHD adults were only included if they met full criteria for current ADHD according to the DSM-IV, with childhood onset and persistence into adulthood. We conducted direct interviews with all participants. Trained lay interviewers, blind to ascertainment status, interviewed all adults with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (First, Spizer, Gibbon, & Willians, 1997) supplemented with modules from the Kiddie SADS-E (Orvashcel, 1994) to cover ADHD and other childhood disorders.

Magnetic Resonance Imaging

Whole brain Magnetic Resonance (MR) images were collected on a Siemens Sonata 1.5 T scanner at the MGH Martinos Center (Charlestown, MA). A sagittal localizer scan was performed for placement of slices, followed by a coronal T2-weighted sequence to rule out unexpected neuropathology. Two sagittal 3DMP-RAGE (T1-weighted, nonselective, inversion prepared, spoiled gradient, echo pulse, no distortion correction) sequences were collected and used for morphometric analyses conducted at the MGH Center for Morphometric Analysis (CMA). The volumetric T1-weighted images, which were used for the analysis were as follows: TR = 2730 ms, TE = 3.39 ms, T1 = 1,000 ms, flip angle = 7°, bandwidth = 190 Hz/pixel, FOV = 256 × 256 mm2, sampling matrix = 256 × 192 pixels, 128 contiguous 1.33 mm slices, averages = 2.

Image Preprocessing: Standard Orientation and Segmentation

The images were resampled into a standard coordinate system based on the bicommissural line (anterior commissure–posterior commissure) and the interhemispheric fissure (Filipek, Kennedy, & Caviness, 1988; Filipek, Richelme, Kennedy, & Caviness, 1994; Talairach, & Tournoux, 1988). Given this coordinate system, coronal slices were defined perpendicular to the bicommissural line and aligned with the interhemispheric fissure. This positional normalization procedure allowed the reconstruction of a new set of coronal images at the slice thickness of the original acquisition (1.33 mm). The images were not rescaled.

MRI Based Segmentation and Parcellation of Cerebral Cortex

Neuroanatomic segmentation was performed using semiautomated intensity contour algorithms for external border definition and signal intensity histogram distributions for delineation of gray–white borders (Filipek et al., 1994). This technique allows for border definition as the midpoint between the peaks of the bimodal distribution for any given structure and its surrounding tissue. Segmentation was performed on coronal images and divided the brain into gray matter and white matter regions. The cerebrum was segmented into its principal gray matter and white matter structures and total cerebral white matter. Specifically, the cortical ribbon was defined by two outlines, one external outline between the sub-arachnoid CSF and the cerebral cortex, and the other between the cerebral cortex and the underlying cerebral white matter (Filipek et al., 1994). The total number of voxels in each brain region determined its volume.

Cortical Parcellation

At the neocortex, which was defined by the aforementioned gray–white matter segmentation procedure, we parcellated the anterior cingulate gyrus (ACC or CGa) parcellation unit (PU) bilaterally (Caviness, Meyer, Makris, & Kennedy, 1996). This was based on the configuration of a specified set of cerebral landmarks, precisely the cingulate sulcus (anterior and superior border of ACC), the callosal sulcus (inferior border of ACC) and the plane passing though the dorsocaudal endpoint of the precentral sulcus (i.e., plane K; posterior border of ACC), and addresses interindividual topographic variability by preserving the morphological and topographic uniqueness of the individual brain. This method is explained extensively in Caviness et al. (1996). Following cortical parcellation, volumes were calculated for each PU by calculating the volume of the PU on each slice and then summing all slices on which the ACC appeared. We selected a priori the ACC as the PU to investigate based on our previous work (Figure 1, Seidman et al., 2006).

Figure 1
The anterior cingulate gyrus (ACC) is shown on a 3-D reconstruction of the medial aspect of the left hemisphere

Statistical Analysis

First, we compared the three groups on demographic, psychiatric, and neurocognitive factors, using the Chi square test and one-way ANOVAs for categorical and dimensional variables, respectively.

We tested the hypothesis that treatment-naïve and treated adults with ADHD would have morphometric abnormalities in the ACC relative to the controls using regression models. In each model, we estimate the volumetric outcome as a function of group status. We present the mean volumes within each group. All models were estimated using linear regression, and all tests were two-tailed. The alpha level was set at 0.05.

Results

The treatment-naïve group showed significant volume decrease in the left anterior cingulate gyrus as shown in Table 2, with a large effect size (d = .71, 21% smaller). On the other hand, the treated group showed significant volumetric decrease in the right anterior cingulate gyrus with a large effect size (d = .85, 23% smaller; Table 2). There were no significant group differences in whole brain or total cerebral volume or in the right ACC (Table 1).

Table 2
Adjusted mean MRI volumes for the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)

Discussion

This pilot analysis showed a volumetric decrease in the cortex of the left ACC in treatment-naïve adults with ADHD. This finding suggests that volumetric abnormalities in the ACC in adults with ADHD represent alterations reflecting the neural underpinnings of the disorder.

Consistent with our results are the results from a recent study by Pliszka et al. (2006) that showed an alteration in the right ACC of treatment-naïve children with ADHD. Although these two studies are not directly comparable given the determining influence developmental forces may have on brain structure as a function of time, it is interesting to note that both studies have identified a change related to the ACC.

The anterior cingulate gyrus is an important regulator of other cortical and subcortical brain regions, and its disconnection seems to be consistent with the symptoms encountered in ADHD. The ACC is connected with other cortical areas ipsilaterally and contralaterally as well as with subcortical structures. This mesh of connections converging to and diverging from the neuronal bodies of the ACC constitutes a central networking node within the overall connectional map of the brain critical for the interface of drive, emotion, cognition and motor function (Paus, 2001) as well as for the modulation of cognitive control (Cohen, Botvinick, & Carter, 2000). An abnormality of ACC could produce derangement of attention and executive neural networks in adults with ADHD. Importantly, a deficit of the ACC network may cause breakdown in modulation of cognitive control and allocation of attention, which may result in impulsivity, hyperactivity, and inattention.

The finding that in adults with ADHD the left ACC is abnormal volumetrically is critical to elucidate a concept that has recently been raised in ADHD clinical research. A study by Shaw et al. (2007) reported that children with ADHD show a “delay” in maturation of brain structures as compared to controls (Shaw et al., 2007). It is unclear from this study, however, whether the brain abnormalities will eventually “normalize” or not. In a previous study by Shaw et al. (2006), the authors showed that certain aspects of the morphometric profile of children with “better outcome ADHD” (such as the right inferior parietal lobule) tend to “normalize” as they progress into late adolescence (Shawet al., 2006). The results of our study, although preliminary, bear evidence that there are abnormalities in the anterior cingulate gyrus of ADHD participants that persist well into adulthood.

Our study showing abnormalities in the ACC in medication-naïve ADHD participants add to a very limited literature on this topic. Although the stimulant drugs have been proven to be relatively safe and highly effective for the treatment of ADHD, there is very limited and equivocal information on how they affect brain structure. Castellanos et al. (2002) found that medication-naïve children and adolescents with ADHD showed smaller white matter volumes relative to both ADHD children and adolescents with previous history of stimulant treatment and control participants (Castellanos et al., 2002). In contrast, a recent study by our group showed that white matter volume was increased in adults with ADHD relative to controls (Seidman et al., 2006). However, the two studies investigated different samples of individuals with ADHD, that is, Seidman et al. (2006) studied adults, whereas Castellanos et al. (2002) studied children and adolescents. Although Seidman et al. (2006) studied adults with ADHD, they did not investigate treatment effects in ADHD.

Regarding the effect of treatment on gray matter in ADHD, a recent study by Pliszka et al. (2006) showed bilateral volumetric decreases in the caudate nucleus in treatment-naïve and treated ADHD children relative to controls suggesting that medication may not have an effect on this subcortical structure in children with ADHD (Pliszka et al., 2006). However, they did show volumetric decrease on the right ACC in treatment-naïve children with ADHD relative to treated children with ADHD and controls. The treated children instead, did not show significant differences from controls suggesting a possible protective effect of medication on this cortical structure. In contrast, our preliminary data showing a significant volumetric decrease of the right anterior cingulate gyrus in the group of treated adults with ADHD would suggest that medication may not have a protective effect on this structure. More work in needed to be done to confirm these findings and clarify this important issue.

How a volumetric decrease of the left anterior cingulate gyrus in treatment-naïve adults with ADHD can be reconciled with the observation by Pliszka et al. (2006) of a volumetric decrease on the right ACC in treatment-naïve children with ADHD is a notion that needs further clarification. In a previous volumetric study of our group (Seidman et al., 2006) we documented statistically significant volume differences in treated adults with ADHD, in total anterior cingulate gyrus (13.9%) and right anterior cingulate gyrus (13.2%). Although it did not reach statistical significance, the left anterior cingulate gyrus was also much smaller in the adult ADHD group (14.8%). Thus a decrease in size left anterior cingulate gyrus would have been expected to be present in treatment-naïve adults with ADHD. A smaller total anterior cingulate gyrus may not be surprising in treatment-naïve children with ADHD as well. A developmental shift of laterality from childhood to adulthood would be also an interesting possibility, however, to the best of our knowledge there is currently no information to explain this scenario. More work is needed to confirm these findings and clarify this important issue.

Our findings must be interpreted in light of some limitations. One is that ADHD is clinically heterogeneous. We cannot test hypotheses about heterogeneity without markedly increasing the sample size of the study. Another issue relates to the limitations of the naturalistic treatment history used in our study. A naturalistic study design has some limitations inherent to it. Specifically, adults with ADHD will have been treated with several medications at the time of study entry, which could impact gray and white matter biology in different ways. However, the alternate study design, a randomized, controlled trial, has important limitations as well. A controlled trial would only be able to evaluate the effect of medications over a relatively short duration because attrition rates would increase to unacceptable levels, especially in a placebo group. Thus, we considered the naturalistic design to be the more efficient and feasible design to address our main aims at this point in time. Furthermore, given the absence of studies on treatment-naïve adults with ADHD and the small number of participants in the present study we are considering this result as preliminary until replicated in larger, better powered studies.

Conclusion

In this pilot study, we found preliminary evidence suggesting that in treatment-naïve adults with ADHD the left ACC is altered volumetrically, showing a decrease in its volume as compared with healthy controls. If confirmed in larger studies, these findings suggest that ADHD is associated with persistent brain abnormalities localized at the left ACC, a region central to cognitive control, drive and affect modulation that appear to be independent of medication treatments.

Acknowledgments

This research was primarily supported by a grant from the NIMH MH 62152 (LJS). Preparation of this article was also supported in part by grants from: The National Association for Research in Schizophrenia and Depression (NARSAD) and the National Institutes of Health National Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine NCAM (NM); the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression Distinguished Investigator Award (JB), Janssen Pharmaceuticals and the Johnson and Johnson Center for the Study of Psychopathology (JB); the Fairway Trust (DK); National Research Service Award (NIMH F32 MH065040-01A1), Peter Livings-ton Fellowship through the Harvard Medical School Department of Psychiatry, and the Clinical Research Training Program Fellowship in Biological and Social Psychiatry MH-16259 (EMV); The National Center for Research Resources (P41RR14075); the March of Dimes Foundation (LJS), and the Mental Illness and Neuroscience Discovery (MIND) Institute (LJS).

Biographies

• 

Nikos Makris, MD, PhD, is Associate Professor of Psychiatry and Neurology at Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School. He is the Director of the Center for Morphometric Analysis and the MGH Morphometric Analysis Center.

• 

Larry J. Seidman, PhD, is a Professor of Psychology in the Harvard Medical School Departments of Psychiatry at Massachusetts General Hospital and the Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center.

• 

Eve M. Valera, PhD is an Assistant Professor of Psychology in the Department of Psychiatry at Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School. Within the Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging at MGH, she directs The Cerebellar Psychiatric Research Laboratory, which is dedicated to understanding the role of the cerebellum and associated cortico-cerebellar circuits in psychiatric illness. Additional work in her lab employs magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) in conjunction with measures of cognitive functioning to assess the effects of physical abuse resulting in brain injury in battered women.

• 

Joseph Biederman, MD is the Chief of the Clinical and Research Programs in Pediatric Psychopharmacology and ADHD at the Massachusetts General Hospital. He is the author and coauthor of more than 650 scientific papers.

• 

Michael C. Monuteaux, ScD, is Assistant Professor of Psychiatry at Harvard Medical School and the Assistant Director of Research at the Clinical and Research Programs in Pediatric Psychiatry and Adult ADHD at Massachusetts General Hospital. He is a psychiatric epidemiologist interested in studying the risk factors for aggression in children and the course and outcomes of disruptive behavior disorders.

• 

David N. Kennedy, PhD, is currently Professor of Psychiatry at UMass Medical School and Director of the Neuroinformatics Program in Psychiatry at UMass Medical School.

• 

Verne S. Caviness Jr., MD, DPHL, is the Giovanni Armenise Professor of Neurology at the Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School. He served as Chief of the Division of Child Neurology from 1982 – 2007. He was a founding director of the Center for Morphometric Analysis with investigative work both in magnetic resonance imaging and the cell biology of forebrain development.

• 

George Bush, MD, MMSc, Associate Professor of Psychiatry at Harvard Medical School and Massachusetts General Hospital, serves as the Director of the MGH Cingulate Cortex Research Laboratory, Director of Neuroimaging Research at the Benson-Henry Institute for Mind Body Medicine at MGH, and Assistant Director of the MGH Psychiatric Neuroimaging Research Program. His research combines functional neuroimaging and intracranial recordings to study how cingulate cortex contributes to normal cognitive, decision-making, reward, motor, and emotional processing. This knowledge is then applied to elucidating the pathophysiology of neuropsychiatric disorders, including attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.

• 

Katherine Crum is currently a PhD candidate and a Teaching Fellow at Boston Univeristy.

• 

Ariel B. Brown, PhD, is currently a Postdoctoral Fellow at the Pediatric Psychopharmacology Clinic and the Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging at Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School.

• 

Stephen V. Faraone, PhD, a clinical psychologist, is Professor of Psychiatry and of Neuroscience & Physiology at SUNY Upstate Medical University. He researches a wide range of issues in the area of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, including genetics, neurobiology, diagnosis and treatment.

Contributor Information

Nikos Makris, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston University School of Medicine.

Larry J. Seidman, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School.

Eve M. Valera, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School.

Joseph Biederman, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School.

Michael C. Monuteaux, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School.

David N. Kennedy, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School.

Verne S. Caviness, Jr., Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School.

George Bush, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School.

Katherine Crum, Boston University.

Ariel B. Brown, Boston University School of Medicine, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School.

Stephen V. Faraone, SUNY Upstate Medical University.

References

  • Biederman J, Faraone SV, Spencer TJ, Mick E, Monuteaux MC, Aleardi M. Functional impairments in adults with self-reports of diagnosed ADHD: A controlled study of 1001 adults in the community. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2006;67:524–540. [PubMed]
  • Biederman J, Makris N, Valera EM, Monuteaux MC, Goldstein JM, Buka S, et al. Towards further understanding of the co-morbidity between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and bipolar disorder: a MRI study of brain volumes. Psychological Medicine. 2008;38:1045–1056. [PubMed]
  • Bush G, Frazier JA, Rauch SL, Seidman LJ, Whalen PJ, Jenike MA, et al. Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by fMRI and the counting stroop. Biological Psychiatry. 1999;45:1542–1552. [PubMed]
  • Bush G, Spencer TJ, Holmes J, Shin LM, Valera EM, Seidman LJ, et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of methylphenidate and placebo in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder during the multi-source interference task. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2008;65:102–114. [PubMed]
  • Bush G, Valera EM, Seidman LJ. Functional neuroimaging of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A review and suggested future directions. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:1273–1284. [PubMed]
  • Castellanos FX, Lee PP, Sharp W, Jeffries NO, Greenstein DK, Clasen LS, et al. Developmental trajectories of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescents with attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2002;288:1740–1748. [PubMed]
  • Caviness VS, Jr, Meyer JW, Makris N, Kennedy DN. MRI-based topographic parcellation of the human neocortex: An anatomically specified method with estimate of reliability. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1996;8:566–587. [PubMed]
  • Cohen JD, Botvinick M, Carter CS. Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex: Who’s in control? Nature Neuroscience. 2000;3:421–423. [PubMed]
  • Durston S, Tottenham NT, Thomas KM, Davidson MC, Eigsti IM, Yang Y, et al. Differential patterns of striatal activation in young children with and without ADHD. Biological Psychiatry. 2003;53:871–878. [PubMed]
  • Ernst M, Kimes AS, London ED, Matochik JA, Eldreth D, Tata S, et al. Neural substrates of decision making in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:1061–1070. [PubMed]
  • Filipek PA, Kennedy DN, Caviness VS., Jr A method of morphometric analysis of the human brain based upon magnetic resonance imaging. Annals of Neurology. 1988;24:356.
  • Filipek PA, Richelme C, Kennedy DN, Caviness VS., Jr The young adult human brain: An MRI-based morphometric analysis. Cerebral Cortex. 1994;4:344–360. [PubMed]
  • First M, Spizer R, Gibbon M, Willians J. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1997.
  • Kessler RC, Merikangas KR. The national comorbidity survey replication (NCS-R): Background and aims. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 2004;13:60–68. [PubMed]
  • Konrad K, Neufang S, Hanisch C, Fink GR, Herpertz-Dahlmann B. Dysfunctional attentional networks in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Evidence from an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;59:643–651. [PubMed]
  • Makris N, Biederman J, Valera EM, Bush G, Kaiser J, Kennedy DN, et al. Cortical thinning of the attention and executive function networks in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17:1364–1375. [PubMed]
  • Makris N, Buka SL, Biederman J, Papadimitriou GM, Hodge SM, Valera EM, et al. Attention and executive systems abnormalities in adults with childhood ADHD: A DT-MRI study of connections. Cerebral Cortex. 2008;18:1210–1220. [PubMed]
  • Orvashcel H. Schedule for affective disorder and schizophrenia for school-age children epidemiologic version. 5. Fort Lauderdal, FL: Nova Southeastern University, Center for Psychological Studies; 1994.
  • Paus T. Primate anterior cingulate cortex: Where motor control, drive and cognition interface. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2001;2:417–424. [PubMed]
  • Pliszka SR, Glahn DC, Semrud-Clikeman M, Franklin C, Perez R, 3rd, Xiong J, et al. Neuroimaging of inhibitory control areas in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder who were treatment naive or in long-term treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163:1052–1060. [PubMed]
  • Rubia K, Overmeyer S, Taylor E, Brammer M, Williams SC, Simmons A, et al. Hypofrontality in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during higher-order motor control: A study with functional MRI. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156:891–896. [PubMed]
  • Seidman LJ, Valera EM, Makris N, Monuteaux MC, Boriel DL, Kelkar K, et al. Dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex volumetric abnormalities in adults with attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder identified by magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;60:1071–1080. [PubMed]
  • Shaw P, Eckstrand K, Sharp W, Blumenthal J, Lerch JP, Greenstein D, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2007;104:19649–19654. [PubMed]
  • Shaw P, Lerch J, Greenstein D, Sharp W, Clasen L, Evans A, et al. Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and clinical outcome in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:540–549. [PubMed]
  • Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. New York, NY: Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc; 1988.
  • Tamm L, Menon V, Ringel J, Reiss AL. Event-related fMRI evidence of frontotemporal involvement in aberrant response inhibition and task switching in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2004;43:1430–1440. [PubMed]
  • Wechsler D. The psychometric tradition: Developing the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 1981;10:82–85.
  • Zametkin AJ, Nordahl TE, Gross M, King AC, Semple WE, Rumsey J, et al. Cerebral glucose metabolism in adults with hyperactivity of childhood onset. New England Journal of Medicine. 1990;323:1361–1366. [PubMed]