PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptHHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
Alzheimers Dement. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 7.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC3646265
NIHMSID: NIHMS466105

Some evolutionary perspectives on Alzheimer's disease pathogenesis and pathology

Abstract

There is increasing urgency to develop effective prevention and treatment for Alzheimer's disease (AD) as the aging population swells. Yet, our understanding remains limited for the elemental pathophysiological mechanisms of AD dementia that may be causal, compensatory, or epiphenomenal. To this end, we consider AD and why it exists from the perspectives of natural selection, adaptation, genetic drift, and other evolutionary forces. We discuss the connection between the apolipoprotein E (APOE) allele and AD, with special consideration to APOE ε4 as the ancestral allele. The phylogeny of AD-like changes across species is also examined, and pathology and treatment implications of AD are discussed from the perspective of evolutionary medicine. In particular, amyloid-β (Aβ) neuritic plaques and paired helical filament tau (PHFtau) neurofibrillary tangles have been traditionally viewed as injurious pathologies to be targeted, but may be preservative or restorative processes that mitigate harmful neurodegenerative processes or may be epiphenoma of the essential processes that cause neurodegeneration. Thus, we raise fundamental questions about current strategies for AD prevention and therapeutics.

Keywords: Alzheimer's disease, Dementia, Evolutionary medicine, Amyloid-β plaques, Neurofibrillary tangles, APOE, Comparative biology

1. Introduction

As baby boomers enter the vulnerable ages for Alzheimer's disease (AD), biomedical research is in a race against time to prevent, stabilize, or cure the disease. Implicitly or explicitly, rational therapeutic discovery relies on understanding the root cause(s) as well as intermediate and proximate pathophysiological processes of disease. Basic research on AD has historically focused on characterizing the signature pathological lesions, that is, amyloid-β (Aβ) neuritic plaques and paired helical filament tau (PHFtau) neurofibrillary tangles, and the precedent and consequent molecular, biochemical, and physical mechanisms that most likely link these disease lesions to the neurodegeneration and cognitive decline caused by AD.

From a therapeutic standpoint, a major challenge is to discern where one should intervene in the mechanistic chain of events of AD so as to achieve the best and least toxic effect. The body is an evolved, complex system that strives to maintain homeostasis and promote individual survival and genetic reproduction; thus, any particular stage of biochemical and pathological progression in a disease may be an adaptive homeostatic response to the upstream events that preceded it. Evolutionary biomedical approaches attempt to account for the initial abnormality or dysfunction before dealing with its downstream effects. Depending on its contextual function, intervening at a downstream stage could be helpful, unhelpful, or even harmful. Seeking evolution-based answers to questions about the cause of diseases like AD can provide important perspectives on downstream “pathological” processes that, in fact, may be preservative or restorative.

There has been relatively little attention to evolutionary aspects of AD. One hypothesis is that AD is simply an inevitable manifestation of senescence [1]. AD, with its neuropathological signatures, decline in function, and associated suffering, is considered a disease rather than a natural, “normal” phenomenon of aging. However, all body systems deteriorate with age; it would be surprising if the brain did not. The risk of developing AD may be as high as 50% by age 85 [2], and some have estimated that by age 130, everyone would have the “disease” [3].

Another major hypothesis that is not mutually exclusive involves “antagonistic pleiotropy,” in which genes that are beneficial in one respect are detrimental in another. It has been suggested that the robust neuroplasticity of the human brain, which is especially advantageous during developmental and reproductive years when learning and mental flexibility are at a premium, comes at a bioenergetic cost of higher risk of lesions in later life [4]. These perspectives are part of the more general evolutionary theories of biological aging. See Rose [5] for a full overview, and Martin [6] for a discussion specifically on brain aging.

2. Evolutionary perspectives on the genetics and origins of AD

2.1. APOE

AD exists in both familial early-onset forms, in which uncommon mutations in specific genes (i.e., APP, PSEN1, PSEN2) cause the disorder [7], and the much more common “sporadic” type, with a typically later onset, to which most of this review and commentary will be limited. A number of genes have been associated with sporadic AD such as A2M, FE65, GLUR5, and SORL1, among others [712]), but so far, the only unequivocal risk gene is apolipoprotein E (APOE) [13,14].

APOE resides on chromosome 19 and consists of four exons and three introns encoded in 3597 base pairs. There are three major alleles, APOE ε2, APOE ε3, and APOE ε4, which translate three isoforms of the protein: apoE-ε2, apoE-ε3, and apoE-ε4. Individuals heterozygous for the ε4 allele are at a 2- to 3-fold higher risk of developing AD, whereas homozygosity for ε4 confers a 10- to 30-fold risk [15,16]. In contrast, the presence of an ε2 allele may confer a 25% decreased risk of developing AD [17]. Thus, APOE ε4 has classically been seen as injurious; APOE ε2, as protective; and APOE ε3, as neutral.

The addition of a phylogenetic perspective, however, warrants a shift in the way the APOE alleles are conceptualized. Among all other primates besides humans, there is only one common APOE, which, like human ε4, has arginine codons at amino acid positions 112 and 158 (APOE ε3 has cysteine residues at position 112 and arginine at position 158, whereas APOE ε2 has cysteine at both positions) [18]. This suggests that APOE ε4 is the ancestral form of the gene [18,19], and APOE ε3 arose later with a single cytosine to thymidine substitution at position 112 after the human lineage diverged from that of chimpanzees and bonobos. Another such mutation at position 158 in the APOE ε3 allele later gave rise to the APOE ε2 form of the gene [18,20]. Rather than APOE ε4 “cropping up” at some point in our recent evolutionary history, it appears that such a gene was present in our nonhuman ancestors and has only recently been joined in the gene pool by competing functional variants [21] that may confer evolutionary advantages. In light of this, it may be more accurate to describe the ε4 allele as the “neutral” form because it predates the others, and to describe both the ε2 and ε3 alleles as protective, acting to mitigate the risk of AD, with the ε2 conferring more cognitive protection than ε3.

Such a distinction may affect how we approach therapeutic interventions. Should we be attempting to inhibit the effects of APOE ε4, mimic the effects of APOE ε2 or ε3, or both [19]? Rebeck et al. propose that the apoE-ε2 and -ε3 isoforms might simply perform the same lipid transport, antioxidant, anti-inflammatory, metabolic, and/or neuroprotective functions as those performed by apoE-ε4, only more efficiently [16].

2.2. Prevalence of protective alleles

Researchers studying the evolutionary origins of AD have considered the question of why the “new” alleles, and especially APOE ε3, have become so prevalent; approximately 95% of humans on average are estimated to carry at least one copy of the APOE ε3 allele, with 55% of humans homozygous for the allele (by contrast, about 28% of humans carry at least one APOE ε4 allele, with only 1%–2% homozygous for APOE ε4) [22]. It is possible that the APOE ε2 and APOE ε3 alleles, after arising in local populations, were subject to genetic drift such as a population bottleneck or founder effect. Not all genotypic or phenotypic traits necessarily have adaptive explanations, and genetic drift, as opposed to natural selection, accounts for a great deal of nonadaptive phenotypic variation [23]. According to Keller and Miller [24], drift almost inevitably results in genotypic uniformity, as the neutral allele either arbitrarily becomes fixated in the population or disappears entirely. Thus, if the spread of APOE ε3 is due to drift, it will eventually fixate in the genome over the course of many more generations (while APOE ε2 and ε4 would seem destined to be lost due to drift, given their relatively rarer distributions).

Alternatively (or additionally), the APOE ε2 and ε3 alleles may have been favored by natural selection, despite the fact that AD, with its postreproductive onset, would not seem to be strongly selected against. Researchers attempting to reconcile this conundrum have found evidence that the APOE ε3 allele confers benefits beyond AD protection. APOE ε4 carriers are at higher risk for atherosclerotic cardiovascular disease, not just AD [25]. Indeed, the gene APOE and the protein it encodes are involved in lipid absorption, immunity, and nerve growth [21]. Perhaps selection pressure on one of these effects, not neuroprotective factors, has allowed the APOE ε2 and APOE ε3 genes to flourish.

On the other hand, APOE ε4 carriers have been reported to have, on average, decreased levels of intellectual functioning throughout life and an increased likelihood of dropping out of high school relative to age-matched peers [26].It is unclear whether this is due to early, subclinical accumulations of AD neuropathology that are known to begin decades before the typical ages of sporadic AD [27], or due to other effects independent of AD pathology. Alternatively, APOE ε2 and APOE ε3 alleles might have other neurological benefits besides AD protection, including fewer tangles and plaques in young adults [28] and after head trauma [29].

Sapolsky and Finch discuss yet another way in which protective APOE alleles may be selected for: the human phenomenon of “grandmothering” [30]. Durable functioning in old age is selected for via kin selection [31]; that is, early humans who remained functionally able for longer were able to provide more care and food for their grandchildren, who in turn had a greater chance of survival and passing on those protective alleles to their own offspring. Although motherly care is the norm in all mammal species, “grandmothering” in nonhuman primates is almost entirely unheard of. As Sapolsky and Finch note, this fits with the observation that dementia in nonhuman primates occurs shortly after reproductive senescence, whereas dementia in humans tends to occur around 20 years after menopause, allowing more time for postreproductive females to contribute to the care of their grandchildren [30].

2.3. Recent origins of APOE ε3 and ε2

Given its presence in species as distantly related to humans as rabbits [32], the ancestral APOE gene (comparable with human APOE ε4) must have been present for millions of years in our nonhuman ancestors; yet the now-widespread protective alleles arose only recently, between 200,000 and 300,000 years ago [19], after the genus Homo diverged from Pan (chimpanzees and bonobos). The “grandmothering” theory ties in with evolutionary life history to provide a possible explanation for this.

As brain size in our ancestors increased relative to other apes, and bipedality resulted in smaller birth canals, babies had to be born relatively earlier in their development to allow passage of their larger heads during childbirth. This resulted in a longer period of helplessness and the need for more prolonged parental care, which in turn created a greater genetic opportunity for grandmothers to benefit by helping with childcare. Therefore, there would have been selection pressure for human grandmothers to stay functional after menopause. This may have led to the spread of the APOE ε3 allele, which was now beneficial but which would have conferred little advantage before the increase in brain size.

A related hypothesis posits a connection to our changing diet; Finch and Stanford suggest that at some point, our ancestors began eating more meat than earlier primates used to eat [33]. The shift in dietary emphasis meant higher cholesterol intake, allowing the APOE ε3 allele, with its lowered risk of atherosclerotic vascular disease, to confer a survival advantage to meat-eaters. AD protection would thus be incidental. Alternatively, it has been reported that the APOE ε4 allele is associated with a younger age of menopause [34] (although others have not found this [35]). If true, then the APOE ε3 or APOE ε2 alleles would provide an obvious procreative advantage. None of these theories are mutually incompatible with the others. The APOE ε2 allele later evolved from the APOE ε3 form and may have spread relative to the other alleles by any combination of the above hypotheses, including neuroprotective function, cardiovascular effects, or random genetic drift.

2.4. Conservation of the ε4 allele

Some researchers have attempted to explain why the APOE ε4 allele has not been eliminated from the gene pool altogether, in favor of APOE ε2 and APOE ε3, if these more protective forms confer a survival advantage. The simplest answer is the force of natural selection decreases as organisms age. However, the APOE ε2 and APOE ε3 alleles, having arisen recently in evolutionary history, seem to be overrepresented in the gene pool, suggesting possible selection pressure for them (see section 2.2). If this is the case, why does APOE ε4 persist? If a gene variant confers any net reproductive disadvantage, there is selection against it.

Some researchers have looked for potential counterbalancing benefits of the apoE-ε4 isoform. Various studies have discovered possible protection against liver damage in patients with the hepatitis C virus [36], reduced cardiovascular response to mental stress [37], and decreased chance of spontaneous abortion in fetuses [38]. Also, although ε2 carriers are at reduced risk for developing AD, they may be at higher risk for atherosclerotic disease, given the association of APOE ε2 homozygosity with type III hyperlipoproteinemia [39], giving the ε4 allele a relative advantage based on this disease association. These are all possible antagonistic pleiotropic mechanisms of maintaining harmful alleles.

Keller and Miller [24] believe that the APOE ε4 allele is indeed on its way to extinction, a process that began with the rise of the protective APOE ε3 allele. In their view, antagonistic pleiotropy is unlikely to result in generationally stable allelic variation; because it is highly improbable that multiple alleles would confer precisely the same reproductive value, one of the competing alleles is almost certain to eventually reach fixation. From this perspective, the only explanation for the continued existence of the ancestral allele is that we are viewing an “evolutionary snapshot” of a process that will, after many generations, drive APOE ε4 to extinction [24].

Reser [1] discusses the intriguing possibility that preclinical or prodromal AD itself is an adaptation, a kind of “rescue program” that allows the body to conserve resources in food-scarce environments. Many other body systems downregulate in response to low caloric intake, shunting precious calories from the least crucial areas to the most vital ones, and because the brain is a bioenergetically high-cost organ, it might be expected to do the same. Reser suggests that the parts of the brain involved in attending to and encoding new information are expendable in a natural environment once an animal reaches the age where it has learned all the skills it needs to survive. In such an environment, the loss of the least crucial brain areas in exchange for precious calories is a utilitarian trade. In modern human society, however, those brain regions (the hippocampus and higher-order association cortices) are involved in domains that we now highly value, such as higher-level executive functions, personality, working memory, and episodic memory. Also, contemporary civilization, public health initiatives, and medical advances, especially in infectious disease, cardiovascular disease, and cancer, have extended the life span far beyond what we would assume to be that of Homo in the wild, who generally would have died of predation, starvation, injury, or infectious disease. Therefore, clinical AD, in Reser's view, is “the unnatural progression of natural brain aging changes.” Circumstances notwithstanding, apoE influences the deposition, aggregation, activity, and neurotoxicity of Aβ, and APOE ε4 correlates with increased Aβ load in AD patients relative to the other alleles [16]. Next, we examine how Aβ and neurofibrillary tangles, the two signature pathologies of AD, fit into this integrated evolutionary picture and how evolutionarily informed research should approach them.

3. Evolutionary medicine and AD

3.1. AD pathology

One goal of evolutionary medicine is to determine the function of disease pathology, signs, and symptoms. It is important that adaptive defenses that the body has evolved in response to pathogens or adverse conditions are recognized as such [40]. Much of the recent and current drug therapy research for AD targets the processes of Aβ production, oligomerization, aggregation, and clearance. The rationale for this line of drug development derives from the predominant amyloid cascade hypothesis, which holds that Aβ, in either oligomeric or aggregated (plaque) forms, impairs neural transmission and is toxic to synapses and neurons, promoting their degeneration. Ultimately, this degeneration manifests in cognitive and functional decline [41]. Accordingly, Aβ lesions should be prevented from forming (e.g., with β-or γ-secretase inhibitors) or removed if present (e.g., with anti-Aβ immunotherapies); otherwise, their downstream effects will be harmful. However, despite more than two decades of research, uncertainties about the extent, mechanisms, and clinical relevance of Aβ in AD remain [42].

The proximate molecular and biochemical steps of processing amyloid precursor protein (APP) into Aβ and subsequent biophysical chemistry of Aβ oligomerization and aggregation have been studied in much detail [42]. Aβ production and plaque formation are most commonly viewed as abnormal biochemical and physical signs of disease, causing injury to healthy brain tissue. This notion is chiefly supported by the dominantly inherited early-onset AD caused by APP, presenilin 1 and presenilin 2 mutations, and the statistical associations between the presence of Aβ plaques and dementia in sporadic late-onset AD. Although the case for Aβ being at the core of the hereditary early-onset AD is compelling (but see Lee et al. [43,44]), the case for its role in late-onset AD is less so. Correlation does not necessarily signify causation, and it is possible that plaques are epiphenomena, reparative reactions, or compensatory phenomena stimulated by the same processes that cause neurodegeneration.

Neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs), composed of paired helical filament tau (PHFtau), are the other defining pathological lesions of AD. Tau is an important microtubule-associated protein that is abundant in neurons, promotes assembly of tubulins into microtubules, and stabilizes microtubules. Phosphorylation of tau by various kinases is an important posttranslational modification that serves to destabilize microtubules, allowing for growth and plastic remodeling. However, hyperphosphorylation of tau, presumably due to either hyperactive kinases or hypoactive phosphatases, is thought to promote the self-assembly of tau into straight and paired helical filaments and ultimately into NFTs [45]. The relationship between tau and Aβ remains unclear. Some have argued that Aβ is upstream to tau in the pathogenesis of AD based on genetic data related to APP and presenilin mutations [46], whereas pathological studies have indicated that in large population samples, tau lesions are observed earlier than Aβ lesions [27]. Neurofibrillary pathology is also more closely correlated with impaired cognition in AD than is Aβ [47]. Eckert et al. [48] propose a link between Aβ and tauopathy and describe a model in which both pathologies synergistically deregulate mitochondrial function, creating a “vicious cycle” that eventually leads to cell death.

3.2. An evolutionary perspective on plaques and tangles

A default evolutionary assumption is that plaques and tangles are biological “spandrels,” that is, by-products of the evolution of some other phenotypic trait, and are not adaptations in their own right (for a general discussion of spandrels, see Gould and Lewontin [23]). The term “spandrel” refers to the origination of the trait rather than its current function; that is, a trait that originally arose as a by-product may later, over the course of evolutionary time, come to serve an adaptive function via cooption [49]. Accordingly, Aβ plaques and PHFtau tangles might have initially arisen as spandrels, but may be currently harmful, neutral, or even beneficial.

The view of plaques and tangles as harmful spandrels, that is, injurious processes that humans happen to be susceptible to, has been implicit in the traditional view of AD pathology [50] (see Lee et al. [43,44]) for a brief summary of proposed toxic mechanisms). However, plaques and tangles may be merely epiphenomena, their presence signaling that neurodegeneration is taking place, rather than indicating a mechanistic role [51]. This possibility underscores the need for continued research into the chain of events that the brain undergoes in the degenerative dementia of AD, and aging in general.

If plaques and/or tangles are spandrels, then what processes might have given rise to them? Finch and Marchalonis posit that Aβ plaques are evolutionary holdovers of the ancient inflammatory immune system that preceded the more elaborate and effective immune system of jawed vertebrates [52]. Indeed, a recent report supports and extends this hypothesis in that Aβ was determined to have potent antimicrobial activity [50].

Additionally, and not mutually exclusive with Finch's hypothesis, it has been proposed that plaques, tangles, or both might be normal or even beneficial reactions to the primary process(es) that actually cause(s) the neurodegeneration [43,44,53]. In this case, plaques and tangles would be either spandrels, which later came to serve an adaptive purpose (known as “exaptation”), or naturally selected adaptations in their own right. In a body doing all it can to maintain homeostasis, it is conceivable that some of the hallmarks of AD pathology are evolved salutary processes working to protect the brain rather than damage it. Ideally, this possibility would be examined and incorporated into the assessment of new disease-modifying therapies, to ensure that treatments are not doing more harm than good [44]. Indeed, postmortem study of subjects from the clinical trial of AN1792, an experimental anti-Aβ agent, indicates that the immunotherapy was successful in disrupting Aβ plaques, but it was not effective at arresting the dementia or reducing other pathological features of the disease such as total and soluble Aβ concentrations, total tangles, or angiopathy [5456]. In addition, recent trials of semagacestat, an inhibitor of γ-secretase, an enzyme that cleaves APP to form Aβ, were prematurely stopped when an interim analysis found that AD subjects on active medication were deteriorating at a faster rate than those on placebo. There are many possible reasons for this, most likely related to the nonspecificity of this particular drug's γ-secretase inhibition. However, one possibility that will have to be considered is that production of Aβ may be a compensatory or reparative response in idiopathic AD and that its inhibition accelerates neurodegeneration.

Although Aβ neurotoxicity has been clearly demonstrated in vitro [57], a mechanistic role of Aβ in neurodegeneration has still not been unequivocally shown in vivo [58]. The data in transgenic mice that overexpress human APP are complex. Like humans with these mutations, mice produce both soluble oligomeric Aβ protofibrils as well as aggregated Aβ plaques. They exhibit evidence of synaptic injury as well as progressive behavioral deficits [59], but little evidence, if any, of the neuron loss per se of the type that is seen in human AD. Anti-Aβ immunotherapies in these mice produce reductions in Aβ abundance, clearance of Aβ plaques, decreased interaction of soluble Aβ oligomers with synaptic constituents, and reversal of behavioral deficits [60]. An interesting study that monitored the temporal profiles of levels of insoluble Aβ and soluble Aβ oligomers in the Tg2576 mouse described a transient lowering of soluble Aβ oligomers that occurs during an initial phase of fibrillar Aβ plaque formation and coincides with improved memory functioning back to normal for that brief time period [61]. This suggests that it is the oligomeric Aβ, and not the insoluble Aβ plaques, that is responsible for memory dysfunction in this mouse model.

Transgenic organisms can be instructive in modeling the biology of a particular gene and gene products, but they are not natural, and lack an evolutionary or adaptive context in which to interpret any but the most flagrant effects of the genetic manipulation. In humans, the toxicity of Aβ, in either fibrillary plaque or oligomeric forms, is not clear. Although generally lower in density than in those with AD, Aβ plaques can be found in the brains of almost all older humans. However, epidemiological studies with pathology have shown that high densities of Aβ plaques are not rare among elders with normal cognition. This shows that abundant Aβ plaques alone are not sufficient to cause dementia [6265]. Other aspects of Aβ plaques that support the argument against their being considered toxic are that their topographical distribution does not correlate well with that of NFTs, neuron or synaptic loss, or the neuropsychological profile of AD [66]. In fact, the density of plaques in the entorhinal cortex and hippocampal formation is among the lowest anywhere in the AD brain [66].

3.3. Potential benefits of Aβ and PHFtau

The cleavage of APP by β- and γ-secretases to form β may just be an evolutionary by-product, but there have also been attempts to find positive functions of β as well. Soscia et al. have shown that β functions as an antimicrobial peptide in vivo [50]. Kamenetz et al. found that neural activity increases β production, which in turn decreases excitatory neurotransmission [67]. This suggests a role for β (and thus APP) as a negative-feedback regulator of synaptic activity [58]. The effect of β on neurotransmission seems to be concentration-dependent; at picomolar levels of β1–42, synaptic transmission and memory are increased, whereas nanomolar concentrations show the opposite pattern [68,69]. Thus, β might have a normal, and perhaps even necessary, function at low levels, and may be disruptive only when its levels are increased due to mutations or other genetic vulnerabilities, or overstimulatory environmental factors. Yao and Papadopoulos suggest that APP and β have a function in controlling cholesterol transport and homeostasis, and that β overproduction leads to neuro-degeneration through a disruption of cholesterol trafficking [70]. APP knockout mice have been shown to display deficits in certain learning tasks, locomotor activity, and grip strength [71], indicating a role of APP in normal cognition. The cleavage of APP by β and γ-secretases to form β may actually be a crucial part of neuroplasticity that contributes to pruning of synapses that are no longer needed, counterbalancing the α-secretase pathway, which facilitates the creation of new connections [19]. Therefore, brain aging may also be an antagonistic pleiotropic effect of the synaptic pruning that is a normal part of brain development, a process in which APP plays a crucial role.

Regardless of whether monomeric β is functional or oligomeric β is harmful, the occurrence of plaques may be either functional, harmful, or both. Plaques have been hypothesized to acts as “sinks” to immobilize harmful free oligomeric β in the brain [43,72]. It is conceivable that β's tendency to aggregate, because of its fibril structure, is a design feature of the protein to ensure that neurons are not harmed by soluble oligomeric β, or to mitigate the effects of some harmful cellular process in AD [73]. The presence of APP and β deposits after brain trauma [74] suggests that these features can form in response to neuronal injury rather than, or in addition to, causing it.

An analogous account may be made for PHFtau and neurofibrillary pathology. Normal phosphorylation of tau and the resulting destabilization of microtubules are necessary for cytoskeletal rearrangement in the process of axonal and dendritic growth and plasticity [19]. Tau undergoes many other posttranslational modifications, including abnormal hyperphosphorylation, nitration, glycosylation, glycation, ubiquitination, cross-linking, oxidation, deamidation, and others (for review, see Meraz-Ríos et al. [75]). Each of these may promote or attenuate oligomerization and aggregation through various biophysical mechanisms. Like β, it is conceivable that hyperphosphorylated tau oligomers are the toxic species, or they may serve as the niduses for further filamentous aggregation. Tau oligomers have been reported to occur in vulnerable brain regions before observable NFTs in AD and tauopathies, and to occur in a time-dependent manner in tauopathy animal models in correlation with memory impairment [76]. Furthermore, although densities of tangles are highly correlated with neuron loss and cognitive decline in AD, it is also reported that most neurons that die in AD do not show tangles, indicating that tangles are neither sufficient nor necessary for cell death in AD [53,77]. Finally, like plaques, it may be that sequestration and aggregation of oligomerized PHFtau into NFTs is protective [53]. Indeed, some recent work has suggested a protective effect against activated caspase-induced apoptotic death in the neurons that bear NFTs in the rTg4510 mouse model of tauopathy [78,79]. Antagonistic pleiotropy related to tau's role in synaptic pruning may suggest a “trade-off” hypothesis of brain aging similar to that mentioned for APP; this process, which is necessary for early development, becomes harmful when the organism reaches advanced age.

3.4. AD pathologies in nonhuman species and their significance for understanding human AD

Humans are not the only species to show age-related cognitive impairment or even AD-like pathology. Comparing the disease in humans with similar phenomena in other animals, especially from a phylogenetic perspective, provides insights into the evolution and function of AD pathology.

All examined nonhuman primate species have been shown to display either NFTs, Aβ plaques, or both [20,80]. (It has recently been suggested that chimpanzees and other non-human primates might not show age-related cognitive impairment or neurodegeneration at all, and that the structure of the Aβ found in these animals' brains may be different than that found in humans [81,82]; however, it is worth noting that Sherwood et al.'s analysis [82] did not include the long-lived chimpanzee individuals in which brain aging would be expected to appear.) Ungulates, whales, bears, dogs, and cats also have been found to display AD-like pathology [20,83], and Aβ deposits have even been found in birds [84] and fish [51], the latter possibly suggesting an ancient origin for plaques. Age-related cognitive impairment has been observed in primates [20], rodents [85], dogs, and cats [86] (see Fig. 1). As in humans, AD-like pathologies are more likely to be found in older specimens, especially domesticated or captive individuals, whose life spans are likely to be longer than their wild counterparts. As more species are found to be susceptible to the same triad of AD-like features (plaques, tangles, and age-related cognitive impairment) that humans display, the distinction between these syndromes in other animals and AD in humans may become less useful [20].

Fig. 1
Plaques, tangles, and age-related cognitive impairment in other species. The triad of AD hallmarks (plaques, tangle, and age-related cognitive impairment) as found in other species, underscoring the need for more research. For specificity and ease of ...

Presence of Aβ deposits and PHFtau NFTs in other animals suggests homology, that is, that the vulnerability to these pathologies existed before our ancestral split with those species. The preservation of these pathologies over millions of years would be due to one of two factors: (a) some constraint on the strength of natural selection (e.g., their late-life manifestation may result in a lack of selection pressure) or (b) a possible beneficial function for these traits (e.g., amyloid aggregations might serve to prevent harm by oligomeric Aβ). More research into possible health-promoting or survival benefits of plaques and tangles may eventually provide an answer to the question of why they persist among different species.

While of increasing use in disease genetics [87], studies incorporating phylogenetic approaches to understanding AD pathology have been rare. A survey of other species, including nonmammals and especially long-lived ones like certain tortoises and elephants, would yield more clues to the universality of AD pathological lesions. Elephants may be of particular interest because they are renowned for their intelligence, memory, and complex social structure as well as longevity. Indeed, elephant grandmothers, great-aunts, and other postreproductive females play a role in caring for young [88]. Although these mammals are phylogenetically distant from humans, the age-related cognitive impairment and neuropathological changes (e.g., Aβ and PHFtau) in elephants, if they occur, might be expected to be similar to that of humans.

4. Conclusion and therapeutic implications

Viewing AD from an evolutionary perspective prompts a rethinking of the way we describe the relationship between the clinical dementia and the neuropathology by which we define the disease. By integrating the fields of phylogeny, life history theory, genetics, biochemistry, and evolutionary medicine, a unified theory of AD can be developed. The precursor of APOE ε4, universal in our ancestors, became a risk factor for dementia only after changing lifestyle and increased life span allowed alternative alleles (APOE ε3 and APOE ε2) to provide reproductive benefits to their bearers. Perhaps partly because of its improved metabolism of Aβ, APOE ε3 became ubiquitous, but APOE ε4 remains in the gene pool, disposing its bearers to increased rates of Aβ deposits, neurofibrillary tangles, and age-related cognitive decline. Future integrated research into the pathology of AD will reveal whether these disease hallmarks are neutral spandrels, beneficial adaptations, or perhaps an inevitable downside of our complex and highly plastic brains.

Given the complex biology of Aβ and PHFtau monomers, oligomers, and fibrillar aggregates, and their polymorphously deleterious but also potentially beneficial effects for the person, prudence may be advised in attacking these lesions directly. Community-based epidemiological studies of cognition and AD pathology as well as other clinical and preclinical studies provide compelling evidence that although plaques and tangles are commonly associated with AD-type dementia, these lesions are neither necessary nor sufficient for cognitive decline. In fact, some people appear resilient in their cognitive function, despite abundant plaque and tangle pathology. Conversely, others exhibit cognitive frailty, exhibiting severe dementia with only minimal or no pathology.

Orthodox thinking about AD views the neuropathology as the cause of dementia, and the differing levels of impairment among the subjects as being due to multifactorial contributions of Aβ and PHFtau pathology-induced injury such as inflammation and synaptodendritic degeneration. In this formulation, the resilient and cognitive frailty groups are exceptional, and the classification, nomenclature, and framework fit. However, if it turns out that Aβ plaques and/or PHFtau NFTs are correlated spandrels rather than mechanistic intermediaries of neurodegeneration, or perhaps even health-promoting adaptive responses to the as-yet-uncertain causes of idiopathic late-onset AD, then an alternative theoretical framework can be considered wherein plaques/tangles help to mitigate neurodegeneration and cognitive decline, and it is because of these disease features, not in spite of them, that brains remain resilient. Thus, it would be the so-called “resilient” and “cognitive frailty” groups that fit the expected pattern. Aβ plaques protect against dementia in the former and fail to “deploy” in the latter, allowing neuron damage to take place. In patients with both pathology and dementia, harmful processes eventually overwhelm the defensive plaque and/or tangle system and begin to cause cell death. We would then ask why these people get AD despite their defense mechanisms when the “resilient” people's defenses work so well. Why do the “frail” patients' defenses fail to work at all? Research designed to address these questions is pressing, especially as more drug therapies based on classical models of AD pathology, such as the Aβ-cascade hypothesis, enter clinical trials.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by the National Institute on Aging (AG010124) and the Marian S. Ware Charitable Giving Fund.

References

[1] Reser JE. Alzheimer's disease and natural cognitive aging may represent adaptive metabolism reduction programs. Behav Brain Funct. 2009;5:13. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[2] Evans DA, Funkenstein HH, Albert MS, Scherr PA, Cook NR, Chown MJ, Hebert LE, Hennekens CH, Taylor JO. Prevalence of Alzheimer's disease in a community population of older persons: higher than previously reported. JAMA. 1989;262:2551–6. [PubMed]
[3] Terry RD, Katzman R. Life span and synapses: will there be a primary senile dementia? Neurobiol Aging. 2001;22:347–8. [PubMed]
[4] Bufill E, Blesa R. Alzheimer's disease and brain evolution: is Alzheimer's disease an example of antagonistic pleiotropy? Rev Neurol. 2006;42:25–33. [PubMed]
[5] Rose MR. The evolutionary biology of aging. Oxford University Press; New York, NY: 1991.
[6] Martin GM. Gene action in the aging brain: an evolutionary biological perspective. Neurobiol Aging. 2002;23:647–54. [PubMed]
[7] Bertram L. Alzheimer's disease genetics current status and future perspectives. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2009;84:167–84. [PubMed]
[8] Avramopoulos D. Genetics of Alzheimer's disease: recent advances. Genome Med. 2009;1:34. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[9] Hollenbach E, Ackermann S, Hyman BT, Rebeck GW. Confirmation of an association between a polymorphism in exon 3 of the low-density lipoprotein receptor-related protein gene and Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1998;50:1905–7. [PubMed]
[10] Hu Q, Kukull WA, Bressler SL, Gray MD, Cam JA, Larson EB, Martin GM, Deeb SS. The human fe65 gene: genomic structure and an intronic biallelic polymorphism associated with sporadic dementia of the Alzheimer type. Hum Genet. 1998;103:295–303. [PubMed]
[11] Montoya SE, Aston CE, DeKosky ST, Kamboh MI, Lazo JS, Ferrell RE. Bleomycin hydrolase is associated with risk of sporadic Alzheimer's disease. Nat Genet. 1998;18:211–2. [PubMed]
[12] Rogaeva E, Meng Y, Lee JH, Gu Y, Kawarai T, Zou F, et al. The neuronal sortilin-related receptor sorl1 is genetically associated with Alzheimer disease. Nat Genet. 2007;39:168–77. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[13] Saunders AM, Strittmatter WJ, Schmechel D, George-Hyslop PHS, Pericak-Vance MA, Joo SH, et al. Association of apolipoprotein E allele epsilon 4 with late-onset familial and sporadic Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1993;43:1467–72. [PubMed]
[14] Corder EH, Saunders AM, Strittmatter WJ, Schmechel D, Gaskell PC, Small GW, Roses AD, Haines JL, Pericak-Vance MA. Gene dose of apolipoprotein e type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer's disease in late onset families. Science. 1993;261:921–3. [PubMed]
[15] Farrer LA, Cupples LA, Haines JL, Hyman BT, Kukull WA, Mayeux R, et al. Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the association between apolipoprotein e genotype and Alzheimer disease. JAMA. 1997;278:1349–56. [PubMed]
[16] Rebeck GW, Kindy M, LaDu MJ. Apolipoprotein E and Alzheimer's disease: the protective effects of apoe2 and e3. J Alzheimers Dis. 2002;4:145–54. [PubMed]
[17] Corder EH, Saunders AM, Risch NJ, Strittmatter WJ, Schmechel DE, Gaskell PC, Jr, et al. Protective effect of apolipoprotein E type 2 allele for late onset Alzheimer disease. Nat Genet. 1994;7:180–4. [PubMed]
[18] Hanlon CS, Rubinsztein DC. Arginine residues at codons 112 and 158 in the apolipoprotein E gene correspond to the ancestral state in humans. Atherosclerosis. 1995;112:85–90. [PubMed]
[19] Ashford JW. Apoe4: is it the absence of good or the presence of bad? J Alzheimer's Dis. 2002;4:141–3. [PubMed]
[20] Finch CE, Sapolsky RM. The evolution of Alzheimer disease, the reproductive schedule, and apoe isoforms. Neurobiol Aging. 1999;20:407–28. [PubMed]
[21] Fullerton SM, Clark AG, Weiss KM, Nickerson DA, Taylor SL, Stengard JH, et al. Apolipoprotein E variation at the sequence haplo-type level: implications for the origin and maintenance of a major human polymorphism. Am J Hum Genet. 2000;67:881–900. [PubMed]
[22] Hill JM, Bhattacharjee PS, Neumann DM. Apolipoprotein E alleles can contribute to the pathogenesis of numerous clinical conditions including hsv-1 corneal disease. Exp Eye Res. 2007;84:801–11. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[23] Gould SJ, Lewontin RC. The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1979;205:581–98. [PubMed]
[24] Keller MC, Miller G. Resolving the paradox of common, harmful, heritable metal disorders: which evolutionary model works best? Behav Brain Sci. 2006;29:385–452. [PubMed]
[25] Katzel LI, Fleg JL, Paidi M, Ragoobarsingh N, Goldberg AP. Apoe4 polymorphism increases the risk for exercise-induced silent myocardial ischemia in older men. Arterioscler Thromb. 1993;13:1495–500. [PubMed]
[26] Hubacek JA, Pitha J, Skodová Z, Adámková V, Lánská V, Poledne R. A possible role of apolipoprotein E polymorphism in predisposition to higher education. Neuropsychobiology. 2001;43:200–3. [PubMed]
[27] Braak H, Braak E. Frequency of stages of Alzheimer-related lesions in different age categories. Neurobiol Aging. 1997;18:351–7. [PubMed]
[28] Ghebremedhin E, Schultz C, Braak E, Braak H. High frequency of apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 allele in young individuals with very mild Alzheimer's disease-related neurofibrillary changes. Exp Neurol. 1998;153:152–5. [PubMed]
[29] Nicoll JA, Roberts GW, Graham DI. Apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 allele is associated with deposition of amyloid beta-protein following head injury. Nat Med. 1995;1:135–7. [PubMed]
[30] Sapolsky RM, Finch CE. Alzheimer's disease and some speculations about the evolution of its modifiers. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2000;924:99–103. [PubMed]
[31] Smith JM. Group selection and kin selection. Nature. 1964;201:1145–7.
[32] Mahley RW, Rall SC., Jr Is e4 the ancestral human apoee allele? Neurobiol Aging. 1999;20:429–30. [PubMed]
[33] Finch CE, Stanford CB. Meat adaptive genes and the evolution of slower aging in humans. Q Rev Biol. 2004;79:3–50. [PubMed]
[34] Koochmeshgi J, Hosseini-Mazinani SM, Seifati SM, Hosein-Pur-Nobari N, Teimoori-Toolabi L. Apolipoprotein E genotype and age at menopause. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1019:564–7. [PubMed]
[35] He L-N, Recker RR, Deng H-W, Dvornyk V. A polymorphism of apolipoprotein E (apoe) gene is associated with age at natural menopause in Caucasian females. Maturitas. 2009;62:37–41. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[36] Wozniak MA, Itzhaki RF, Faragher EB, James MW, Ryder SD, Irving WL. Apolipoprotein E-epsilon 4 protects against severe liver disease caused by hepatitis C virus. Hepatology. 2002;36:456–63. [PubMed]
[37] Ravaja N, Räikkönen K, Lyytinen H, Lehtimäki T, Keltikangas-Järvinen L. Apolipoprotein E phenotypes and cardiovascular responses to experimentally induced mental stress in adolescent boys. J Behav Med. 1997;20:571–87. [PubMed]
[38] Zetterberg H, Palmér M, Ricksten A, Poirier J, Palmqvist L, Rymo L, et al. Influence of the apolipoprotein E epsilon4 allele on human embryonic development. Neurosci Lett. 2002;324:189–92. [PubMed]
[39] Breslow JL, Zannis VI, SanGiacomo TR, Third JL, Tracy T, Glueck CJ. Studies of familial type III hyperlipoproteinemia using as a genetic marker the apoe phenotype e2/2. J Lipid Res. 1982;23:1224–35. [PubMed]
[40] Harris EE, Malyango AA. Evolutionary explanations in medical and health profession courses: are you answering your students' “Why” Questions? BMC Med Educ. 2005;5:16. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[41] Walsh DM, Selkoe DJ. A beta oligomers - a decade of discovery. J Neurochem. 2007;101:1172–84. [PubMed]
[42] Pimplikar SW. Reassessing the amyloid cascade hypothesis of Alzheimer's disease. Int J Biochem Cell Biol. 2009;41:1261–8. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[43] Lee H-G, Castellani RJ, Zhu X, Perry G, Smith MA. Amyloid-b in Alzheimer's disease: the horse or the cart? Pathogenic or protective? Int J Exp Pathol. 2005;86:133–8. [PubMed]
[44] Lee H-G, Casadesus G, Zhu X, Takeda A, Perry G, Smith MA. Challenging the amyloid cascade hypothesis: senile plaques and amyloid-β as protective adaptations to Alzheimer disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1019:1–4. [PubMed]
[45] Ballatore C, Lee VM, Trojanowski JQ. Tau-mediated neurodegeneration in Alzheimer's disease and related disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8:663–72. [PubMed]
[46] Hardy JA. The relationship between amyloid and tau. J Mol Neurosci. 2003;20:203–6. [PubMed]
[47] Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Arnold SE. Neurofibrillary tangles mediate the association of amyloid load with clinical Alzheimer disease and level of cognitive function. Arch Neurol. 2004;61:378–84. [PubMed]
[48] Eckert A, Schulz KL, Rhein V, Göotz J. Convergence of amyloid-β and tau pathologies on mitochondria in vivo. Mol Neurobiol. 2010;41:107–14. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[49] Gould E, McEwen BS, Tanapat P, Galea LA, Fuchs E. Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of the adult tree shrew is regulated by psychosocial stress and NMDA receptor activation. J Neurosci. 1997;17:2492–8. [PubMed]
[50] Soscia SJ, Kirby JE, Washicosky KJ, Tucker SM, Ingelsson M, Hyman BT, et al. The Alzheimer's disease-associated amyloid ß-protein is an antimicrobial peptide. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e9505. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[51] Maldonado TA, Jones RE, Norris DO. Distribution of beta-amyloid and amyloid precursor protein in the brain of spawning (senescent) salmon: a natural, brain-aging model. Brain Res. 2000;858:237–51. [PubMed]
[52] Finch CE, Marchalonis JJ. Evolutionary inflammatory perspectives on amyloid and features of Alzheimer disease. Neurobiol Aging. 1996;17:809–15. [PubMed]
[53] Lee H-G, Perry G, Moreira PI, Garrett MR, Liu Q, Zhu X, Takeda A, Nunomura A, Smith MA. Tau phosphorylation in Alzheimer's disease: pathogen or protector? Trends Mol Med. 2005;11:164–9. [PubMed]
[54] Kokjohn TA, Roher AE. Antibody responses, amyloid-beta peptide remnants and clinical effects of an-1792 immunization in patients with ad in an interrupted trial. CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets. 2009;82:88–97. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[55] Robinson SR, Bishop GM, Lee H-G, Münch G. Lessons from the an 1792 Alzheimer vaccine: lest we forget. Neurobiol Aging. 2004;25:609–15. [PubMed]
[56] Masliah E, Hansen L, Adame A, Crews L, Bard F, Lee C, et al. Abeta vaccination effects on plaque pathology in the absence of encephalitis in Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2005;64:129–31. [PubMed]
[57] Butterfield DA, Drake J, Pocernich C, Castegna A. Evidence of oxidative damage in Alzheimer's disease brain: central role for amyloid beta-peptide. Trends Mol Med. 2001;7:548–54. [PubMed]
[58] Kern A, Behl C. The unsolved relationship of brain aging and late-onset Alzheimer disease. Biochim Biophys Acta. 2009;1790:1124–32. [PubMed]
[59] Selkoe DJ. Soluble oligomers of the amyloid β-protein impair synaptic plasticity and behavior. Behav Brain Res. 2008;192:106–13. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[60] Röskam S, Neff F, Schwarting R, Bacher M, Dodel R. APP transgenic mice: the effect of active and passive immunotherapy in cognitive tasks. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34:487–99. [PubMed]
[61] Lesné S, Kotilinek L, Ashe KH. Plaque-bearing mice with reduced levels of oligomeric amyloid-β assemblies have intact memory function. Neuroscience. 2008;151:745–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[62] Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Arvanitakis Z, Kelly JF, Aggarwal NT, Shah RC, Wilson RS. Neuropathology of older persons without cognitive impairment from two community-based studies. Neurology. 2006;66:1837–44. [PubMed]
[63] White L. Brain lesions at autopsy in older Japanese-American men as related to cognitive impairment and dementia in the final years of life: a summary report from the Honolulu-Asia aging study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2009;18:713–25. [PubMed]
[64] O'Brien RJ, Resnick SM, Zonderman AB, Ferrucci L, Crain BJ, Pletnikova O, et al. Neuropathologic studies of the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA) J Alzheimers Dis. 2009;18:665–75. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[65] Iacono D, Markesbery WR, Gross M, Pletnikova O, Rudow G, Zandi P, Troncoso JC. The nun study: clinically silent ad, neuronal hypertrophy, and linguistic skills in early life. Neurology. 2009;73:665–73. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[66] Arnold SE, Hyman BT, Flory J, Damasio AR, Van Hoesen GW. The topographical and neuroanatomical distribution of neurofibrillary tangles and neuritic plaques in the cerebral cortex of patients with Alzheimer's disease. Cereb Cortex. 1991;1:103–16. [PubMed]
[67] Kamenetz F, Tomita T, Hsieh H, Seabrook GR, Borchelt D, Iwatsubo T, Sisodia S, Malinow R. APP processing and synaptic function. Neuron. 2003;37:925–37. [PubMed]
[68] Puzzo D, Privitera L, Leznik E, Fa M, Staniszewski A, Palmeri A, Arancio O. Picomolar amyloid-β positively modulates synaptic plasticity and memory in hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2008;28:14537–45. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[69] Garcia-Osta A, Alberini CM. Amyloid beta mediates memory formation. Learn Mem. 2009;16:267–72. [PubMed]
[70] Yao Z-X, Papadopoulos V. Function of ß-amyloid in cholesterol transport: a lead to neurotoxicity. FASEB J. 2002;16:1677–9. [PubMed]
[71] Senechal Y, Kelly PH, Dev KK. Amyloid precursor protein knockout mice show age-dependent deficits in passive avoidance learning. Behav Brain Res. 2008;186:126–32. [PubMed]
[72] Ewbank DC, Arnold SE. Cool with plaques and tangles. New Engl J Med. 2009;360:2357–9. [PubMed]
[73] Whitson JS, Selkoe DJ, Cotman CW. Amyloid b protein enhances the survival of hippocampal neurons in vitro. Science. 1989;243:1488–90. [PubMed]
[74] Roberts GW, Gentleman SM, Lynch A, Murray L, Landon M, Graham DI. Beta amyloid protein deposition in the brain after severe head injury: implications for the pathogenesis of Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994;57:419–25. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[75] Meraz-Ríos MA, Lira-De León KI, Campos-Peña V, De Anda-Hernández MA, Mena-López R. Tau oligomers and aggregation in Alzheimer's disease. J Neurochem. 2010;112:1353–67. [PubMed]
[76] Berger Z, Roder H, Hanna A, Carlson A, Rangachari V, Yue M, et al. Accumulation of pathological tau species and memory loss in a conditional model of tauopathy. J Neurosci. 2007;27:3650–62. [PubMed]
[77] Gómez-Isla T, Hollister R, West H, Mui S, Growdon JH, Petersen RC, Parisi JE, Hyman BT. Neuronal loss correlates with but exceeds neurofibrillary tangles in Alzheimer's disease. Ann Neurol. 1997;41:17–24. [PubMed]
[78] de Calignon A, Spires-Jones TL, Pitstick R, Carlson GA, Hyman BT. Tangle-bearing neurons survive despite disruption of membrane integrity in a mouse model of tauopathy. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2009;68:757–61. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[79] Spires-Jones TL, De Calignon A, Matsui T, Zehr C, Pitstick R, Wu H-Y, et al. In vivo imaging reveals dissociation between caspase activation and acute neuronal death in tangle-bearing neurons. J Neurosci. 2008;28:862–7. [PubMed]
[80] Kimura N, Nakamura S, Goto N, Narushima E, Hara I, Shichiri S, et al. Senile plaques in an aged western lowland gorilla. Exp Anim. 2001;50:77–81. [PubMed]
[81] Rosen RF, Walker LC, LeVine H. PIB binding in aged primate brain: Enrichment of high-affinity sites in humans with Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2011;32:223–34. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[82] Sherwood CC, Gordon AD, Allen JS, Phillips KA, Erwin JM, Hof PR, et al. Aging of the cerebral cortex differs between humans and chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2011 Jul 25; [PubMed]
[83] Sarasa M, Gallego C. Alzheimer-Like Neurodegeneration as a Probable Cause of Cetacean Stranding. Poster session presented at: 5th FENS Forum; Vienna, Austria. 2006.
[84] Nakayama H, Katayama K, Ikawa A, Miyawaki K, Shinozuka J, Uetsuka K, et al. Cerebral amyloid angiopathy in an aged great spotted woodpecker (Picoides major) Neurobiol Aging. 1999;20:53–6. [PubMed]
[85] Fahlström A, Yu Q, Ulfhake B. Behavioral changes in aging female C57BL/6 mice. Neurobiol Aging. 2011;32:1868–80. [PubMed]
[86] Landsberg G, Araujo JA. Behavior problems in geriatric pets. Vet Clin Small Anim. 2005;35:675–98. [PubMed]
[87] Roses AD. The medical and economic roles of pipeline pharmacogenetics: Alzheimer's disease as a model of efficacy and HLA-B(*)5701 as a model of safety. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:6–17. [PubMed]
[88] Rapaport L, Haight J. Some observations regarding allomaternal caretaking among captive Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) J Mammal. 1987;68:438–42.