PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptNIH Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
Am J Psychiatry. Author manuscript; available in PMC Sep 1, 2013.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC3610571
NIHMSID: NIHMS444967
Effect of Alzheimer Disease Risk Genes on Trajectories of Cognitive Function in the Cardiovascular Health Study
Robert A. Sweet, M.D.,1,2,3 Howard Seltman, M.D., Ph.D.,4 James E. Emanuel, B.S.,1 Oscar L. Lopez, M.D.,1,2 James T. Becker, Ph.D.,1,2 Joshua C. Bis, Ph.D.,5 Elise A. Weamer, M.P.H.,1,2 Mary Ann A. DeMichele-Sweet, Ph.D.,1 and Lewis H. Kuller, M.D. Ph.D.6
1Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburg, Pittsburgh, PA
2Department of Neurology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA
3VISN 4 Mental Illness Research, Education and Clinical Center (MIRECC), VA Pittsburgh Healthcare System, Pittsburgh, PA
4Department of Statistics, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA
5School of Medicine, University of Washington, Seattle, WA
6Department of Epidemiology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA
For questions, correspondence, and reprints please contact: Robert A. Sweet, M.D., Professor of Psychiatry and Neurology, Vice-Chair, Institutional Review Board, University of Pittsburgh. Co-Associate Director for Research, Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center, VA Pittsburgh Healthcare System, Ph- 412-383-8548, Fax 412-624-9910, sweetra/at/upmc.edu, http://www.wpic.pitt.edu/research/sweetlab. Mail: Biomedical Science Tower, Rm W-1645, 3811 O'Hara Street, Pittsburgh, PA 15213-2593. Express Mail: Biomedical Science Tower, Rm W-1645, Lothrop and Terrace Streets, Pittsburgh, PA 15213-2593
Objective
Patients with late onset Alzheimer disease (AD) vary widely in their trajectory of cognitive decline. Genetic variations in CLU, PICALM, and CR1 are associated with AD, but it is unknown if they exert their effects via altering cognitive trajectory in elderly subjects at risk for AD.
Methods
We developed a Bayesian model to fit cognitive trajectories in a cohort of elderly subjects and test for genetic effects. We first validated the model’s ability to detect the previously established effects of APOE epsilon 4 alleles on age at cognitive decline and of psychosis on the rate of cognitive decline in 802 subjects from the Cardiovascular Health Study Cognition Study who were without dementia at study entry and developed incident dementia during follow-up. We then evaluated the effects of CLU, PICALM, and CR1 on age and rate of decline in 1831 subjects without dementia at study entry, including those who did, or did not develop incident dementia at study’s end.
Results
Our model generated robust fits to the observed cognitive trajectory data. Validation analysis supported the utility of the model. CLU and CR1 were associated with more rapid cognitive decline. PICALM was associated with an earlier age at midpoint of cognitive decline. Associations remained after accounting for the effects of APOE and demographic factors.
Conclusions
Evaluation of cognitive trajectories provides a powerful approach to dissecting genetic effects on the processes leading to cognitive deterioration and AD.
Keywords: Alzheime's disease, MCI (mild cognitive impairment), genetic, cognitive decline, endophenotype
Some progress has been made in determining the genetic architecture of late onset Alzheimer disease (AD). The association of late onset AD with the ε4 variant of apolipoprotein E (APOE) is well established (1). More recently, replicated genome-wide associations (GWA) of late onset Alzheimer disease with genetic variation in CLU, CR1, and PICALM, have been reported (2;3). Ultimately, these detected associations must be related to specific neurobiologic processes leading to AD in order for them to translate into meaningful clinical predictors or therapies. Current understanding of the neurobiology of AD suggests that disease causation is dependent on a sustained period of pathologic accumulation of amyloid β protein (Aβ) [reviewed in(4)]. The initial accumulation of Aβ occurs prior to clinically detectable cognitive impairment, setting off downstream events including generation of hyperphosphorylated tau, gray matter volume reductions, and synapse loss (5;6). It is measures of these downstream effectors that appear to change most rapidly during phases of disease characterized by more rapid cognitive decline (4).
Conceptually, genetic variation may thus be linked to cognitive change in AD in one of several ways. Genetic variations which increase the production of pathologic Aβ protein, including mutations and duplications of the Aβ precursor protein gene (APP), and mutations in presenilin 1 and 2 (PSEN1 and PSEN2), all lead to an earlier age of disease presentation (7). Similarly, apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 alleles, which reduce Aβ clearance (8), are associated with earlier age of disease presentation (9). Alternatively, one might postulate that genetic variation whose primary role is to interact with the generated or accumulated Aβ so as to modify its downstream effects would influence the rate of overt cognitive decline. Currently, no such genetic variations have been definitively identified. However, psychotic symptoms in AD subjects define a genetically determined behavioral phenotype (10) that is strongly associated with a more rapid rate of cognitive decline (1113).
It would therefore be useful for interpreting the nature of a genetic variation’s relationship to AD risk to be able to measure cognitive trajectories in a way that would allow testing of genetic associations with both age and rate of cognitive decline. Intuitively, it can be seen that an individual traversing the complete course of AD will have a period prior to disease onset of relatively stable cognitive test performance, followed by a period of declining performance, until reaching a period of scores asymptotically approaching a minimum score. Such a trajectory is readily approximated by a 4-parameter logistic curve, however, several obstacles exist in practice. These include the fact that few longitudinal data sets gather complete information on all stages of the cognitive trajectory for individual AD subjects, although the picture may be complete for the group as a whole. Additionally, it is not uncommon for elderly individuals to have poor cognitive test performance in a given session due to factors unrelated to AD, such as concomitant illness, poor sleep, or medication effects. Trajectory modeling must not be overly sensitive to these outlier values.
To address these issues, we developed a novel Bayesian implementation of a hierarchical non-linear model using a 4-parameter logistic curve. Bayesian hierarchical models have the characteristics of borrowing strength and appropriately modeling multiple sources of error. This means that subjects with fewer data points automatically contribute less to the analysis, that prediction for each subject is based on a combination of whatever information is available for that subject weighted with information from other similar subjects, and that the degree of uncertainty about parameters is not underestimated [as occurs in two-stage methods that treat the parameters fitted to individual subject curves as having no uncertainty (14)]. We evaluated our model’s ability to fit trajectories reflecting cognitive deterioration and validated its ability to detect the previously established effects of APOE genotype on age of cognitive decline and of psychosis on rate of cognitive decline. We then implemented our approach to examine the effects of SNPs in CLU, CR1, and PICALM on the age at midpoint and the rate of cognitive decline in a large cohort of elderly subjects.
Subjects (Table 1)
Information on the CHS and CHS Cognition Study methods have been published previously (1517). For the validation cohort, we analyzed data from subjects who by the end of the main study in 1998–9 developed AD (with or without comorbid Vascular Dementia) or had Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) and who had at least four Modified Mini-Mental Status Examination (3MS) or Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) measurements and were thus suitable for trajectory modeling. For the implementation cohort, we analyzed data from Caucasian subjects who had either progressed to AD (with or without comorbid Vascular Dementia), had MCI, or were without a cognitive disorder diagnosis at study endpoint, had at least four 3MS or DSST measurements, and were successfully genotyped for at least one of the SNPs in our genes of interest (18;19). All participants provided written informed consent to participate in the study. Genetic analyses in this paper utilize data only from participants who provided consent for their samples to be used in research on disorders other than cardiovascular diseases.
Table 1
Table 1
Demographic and clinical characteristics of the Validation and Implementation Cohorts.
Assessments
Cognition
The 3MS (20), a measure of global cognition, and the DSST, a measure of attention (21), were administered to participants annually.
Psychosis
The Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) was completed in 1998–9 in a subset of validation cohort subjects (22). Classification as Ever or Never Psychotic was as previously described (16).
Statistical Analysis
We fit data to a four parameter logistic curve in the form:
equation M1
where t is the time (age) of measurement, E(Yit) is the mean outcome at time t for subject i, Ai is the asymptotic outcome value for subject i at small ages, Bi is the asymptotic outcome value for subject i at large ages, Mi is the age at midpoint of cognitive decline, that is the age at which the trajectory for subject i is half-way between Ai and Bi, i.e. the age at which the fit cognitive score is halfway between the fit maximum and the fit minimum value for that subject. Ri is a measure of the rate of change from Ai to Bi. Ri can be interpreted as follows: for any given individual the total change in outcome is AiBi, and the time period over which the middle half of that change occurs, i.e., the time from ¼ to ¾ of the total change, is 2.20(Ri). We explicitly modeled the effects of covariates on M and R by replacing the simple mean parameter with a linear combination of an intercept and the products of covariates with corresponding slope parameters. For A and B, we did not model covariate effects, but allowed for differences between subjects by modeling the random effects. Effects of covariates are reported as the posterior properties of the difference in the mean of each parameter for a change in level of the covariate. Finally, it should be noted that summaries of the posterior distributions in Bayesian analysis take the place of the p-values and confidence intervals of classical statistics. Assuming that we are using an appropriate model and prior distributions, the probability that a parameter is inside its credible interval is 95%.
We tested the effects of APOE ε4 status, psychosis status (Ever versus Never Psychotic), and individual SNPs which have been associated with AD risk: CLU (rs11136000), CR1 (rs3818361), and PICALM (rs3851179, rs541458) (2;3) on M and R. For APOE ε4 we coded the presence or absence of at least one ε4 allele. SNPs were tested individually, examining additive effects of the allele identified as the risk allele in the GWA studies. Analysis of APOE ε4 included demographic factors as covariates. Analyses of the effects of psychosis status and of individual SNPs included APOE ε4 and demographic factors as covariates.
For additional methodologic details, see supplementary text.
Validation Cohort
The basic model, including the effects of demographics on 3MS and DSST trajectories (Supplemental Table 1), was fit to the data from subjects in the validation cohort. Several 3MS trajectories are shown in Figure 1 along with the 95% credible (curvewise) intervals (CI). Examples of fitted trajectories for the DSST, and in the face of occasional outlier test scores, are shown in Supplemental Fig 1.
Figure 1
Figure 1
Examples of four parameter non-linear fits of individual 3MS trajectories
After adjusting for demographic covariates, the presence of an APOE ε4 allele was associated with a mean [95% CI] −3.36 [−4.58, −2.16] year change in age at midpoint for the 3MS and a −2.26 [−3.41, −0.92] year change in age at midpoint of the DSST, both in the predicted directions. In contrast, APOE ε4 carriers showed no change in rate of decline of the 3MS (−0.14 [−0.46, 0.16]) or the DSST (−0.14 [−0.77, 0.47]). Analysis of the effect of Psychosis similarly revealed changes in the predicted directions, with a more rapid rate of decline for the 3MS (−0.42 [−0.83, −0.03]) and for the DSST (−1.06 [−1.98, −0.20]). The effect of Psychosis results in the middle 50% of decline taking on average 1.83 fewer years (3MS) or 2.33 fewer years (DSST). In addition, analysis of Psychosis revealed a change in age at midpoint for the 3MS (−3.74 [−5.51, −2.03]), but not the DSST (−1.70 [−3.67, 0.42]).
Implementation Cohort
We next undertook to examine the effects of SNPs in CLU, CR1, and PICALM on cognitive trajectory in the larger group of subjects, including individuals with and without incident cognitive impairment (Table 2 and Figure 2). CLU SNP rs11136000 was associated with more rapid cognitive decline on the 3MS, and nearly reached significance for the DSST. CR1 SNP rs3818361 was associated with more rapid cognitive decline on the DSST. PICALM SNP rs3851179 was not associated with change in either parameter for the 3MS or the DSST. PICALM SNP rs541458 was associated with earlier age at midpoint of decline of the 3MS, but not with rate of decline, and was not associated with either parameter for the DSST. The associations of CLU SNP rs11136000 and CR1 SNP rs3818361 with rate of decline for the DSST did not differ between individuals who did or did not carry an APOE ε4 allele (Mean [95% CI] difference in R between APOE ε4 positive and negative subjects for CLU SNP rs11136000: 0.07 [−0.66, 0.88] for CR1 SNP rs3818361: 0.37 [−0.57, 1.40]). The association of PICALM SNP rs541458 with midpoint time for the 3MS did not differ between individuals who did or did not carry an APOE ε4 allele (Mean [95% CI] difference between APOE ε4 positive and negative subjects for PICALM SNP rs541458: −1.32 [−3.36, 0.86]. The mean trajectories for CR1, CLU, and PICALM risk allele carriers are shown in Figure 3.
Table 2
Table 2
Effects of genotype on the 3MS and DSST age at midpoint of cognitive decline (M) and rate of cognitive decline (R) parameters in the Implementation Cohort
Figure 2
Figure 2
Effect of genetic variation on Age at Midpoint (M) of Cognitive Decline and Rate of Cognitive Decline (R)
Figure 3
Figure 3
Cognitive trajectories in individuals with, or without, CR1, CLU, and PICALM risk alleles
To address whether one or more of the associations of the four AD risk SNPs (rs11136000, rs3818361, rs3851179, and rs541458) might be a false positive due to multiple testing, we evaluated these SNPs concurrently in additive models for 3MS and for DSST trajectory. We again included APOE genotype, age, sex, and education in the models. The approach of evaluating all SNPs concurrently is to widen the 95% credible intervals, creating a more conservative estimate of significance. We found that the significant association of CR1 SNP rs3818361with DSST rate of decline persists [Mean (95% CI) −0.61 (−1.11, −0.12)], and the nearly significant association of CLU SNP rs11136000 with DSST rate of decline now reaches significance [Mean (95% CI) −0.50 (−0.86, −0.03)]. These observed effects are strong, equal to 0.83 – 1.0 of the standard deviation of the random effect for the subject-to-subject variation in DSST rate in our additive model (estimated at 0.60). Significant associations with 3MS did not persist. It should be noted that evaluating the associations with M and R for a given measure can represent a single 2-dimensional test, not two tests, of association with trajectory. We therefore confirmed that the M=0, R=0 origin is not within the 95% posterior ellipsoid for DSST for CR1 SNP rs3818361and CLU SNP rs11136000.
We found that a non-linear model, implemented with a Bayesian hierarchical analytic approach, generated robust fits to the observed cognitive trajectories of elderly subjects. Validating our approach, APOE ε4 alleles were associated with reduced age at midpoint of cognitive decline and psychosis was associated with increased rate of cognitive decline. We then implemented our model to test whether SNPs associated with an increased risk for late onset AD alter cognitive trajectory after accounting for the effects of APOE and demographic factors. We found that rs11136000 in CLU and rs3818361 in CR1 were associated with more rapid cognitive decline. rs541458 in PICALM was associated with an earlier age at midpoint of cognitive decline, although this latter association did not persist in an additive model designed to correct, in part, for multiple testing. For all SNPs except CLU SNP rs11136000 the allele conferring an adverse cognitive trajectory was the same as that associated with AD in prior GWA studies.
We utilized a non-linear model that allowed for censoring at the maximum and minimum values. This choice was based on a common property of cognitive tests, which often have defined maximum and minimum scores. As a result, prior to disease onset individuals may sustain maximal scores, while in end-stage disease sustained minimal scores may be present. Such observations cannot be fit adequately with a trajectory derived from a linear or quadratic model, although multiple linear fits (e.g. as applied in changepoint models), may be used to approximate subsets of points within a longitudinal cognitive trajectory (23). Other psychometric properties of cognitive tests also contribute to non-linearity. Many tests are admixtures of easier, moderately difficult, and difficult questions, not necessarily in equal proportion. As a consequence, scores may change little during a given phase of cognitive decline, e.g. during early illness if there are few difficult questions. Alternatively, scores may show greater change during another phase, such as moderate disease, if there are proportionally more questions of moderate difficulty. Many global cognitive tests, including the 3MS, follow just such a pattern (24). In contrast, the DSST requires repeating the same cognitive function, and is scored based on how many times it is successfully completed within a timed interval. However, the overall difficulty of the task results in non-linearity in moderate to severely impaired individuals, supporting the use of a non-linear model to fit this data (25). Whether there may be a relative advantage to the use of cognitive measurements that demonstrate more consistent linear measurement properties across levels of disease severity for detection of SNP effects on cognitive decline would benefit from testing using additional measures that differ with regard to these properties.
We found rs11136000 in CLU was associated with more rapid cognitive decline, however, the risk for more rapid decline was conferred by the allele opposite to that previously associated with AD (2;3;18;26). Although rate of cognitive decline and presence of AD are not identical, it seems unlikely that a causal allele would confer different directions of risk for these two outcomes. Several authors have argued that significant allelic association in opposing directions is statistically unlikely (27;28), and probably reflects true association via one of several underlying mechanisms (2730). CLU encodes the protein clusterin, which is expressed at increased levels in the brains of individuals with AD, can serve to prevent fibrillization of Aβ, and inhibit complement activation (31). In individuals with MCI and AD, elevated plasma clusterin correlated with more rapid cognitive decline, though plasma clusterin concentrations were not associated with genetic variants in CLU, including rs11136000 (32).
We found that rs3818361 in CR1 was associated with more rapid cognitive decline. CR1 is a complement receptor expressed in cerebral cortex. There is emerging evidence that the complement cascade contributes to targeting synapses for elimination during development and in neurodegeneration via astrocyte-mediated opsonization with the complement component 3 fragment, C3b (33). CR1 supports clearance of opsonized targets due to its high affinity for C3b (34). Recent evidence indicates that the reported associations of genetic variation in CR1 with AD may arise from linkage disequilibrium between these variants and a low copy repeat which codes for a CR1 isoform with an additional C3b binding site (35). It is not currently known if CR1 contributes to synapse elimination in AD.
Finally, we found that rs541458 in PICALM was associated with an earlier age at midpoint of cognitive decline, although it should be noted that this association did not persist in our additive model. PICALM encodes the phosphatidylinositol-binding clathrin assembly (Picalm) protein, an essential factor in clathrin-mediated endocytosis (36). Recent evidence indicates that Picalm is primarily expressed in vascular endothelial cells in human brain (37), where it may affect the clearance of Aβ from brain. For example, rs541458 in PICALM is significantly associated with cerebrospinal fluid levels of Aβ42 (38). This interpretation would be congruent with a mechanism by which variants that result in overproduction or over-accumulation of Aβ most strongly impact age of cognitive decline.
The above associations of CLU, CR1, and PICALM were detected in models that accounted for demographic variables and the effect of APOE ε4. This finding stands in contrast to a recent analysis of several cohorts, including the CHS, which found that rs11136000 in CLU and rs3851179 in PICALM did not add to the prediction of AD onset beyond demographic and APOE effects (18). This may indicate there is increased power available in using a cognitive trajectory, rather than a dichotomous diagnosis, to identify SNP associations with longitudinal outcome in subjects at risk for AD. Of interest, we found that genetic variation in PICALM was associated with lower age at midpoint of decline in models that included APOE. This is somewhat surprising given recent evidence that APOE and PICALM genotypes are correlated, resulting in substantial reduction in the association of PICALM SNPs with AD risk after accounting for APOE status (26). Unlike PICALM, CLU and CR1 associations with AD risk did not display confounding with APOE genotype (26), thus it is not surprising they demonstrated detectable effects after controlling for APOE ε4 alleles.
We used a Bayesian approach to fit cognitive trajectories. The principal advantage of Bayesian methods over classical (e.g. mixed model) analyses is that calculation of posterior distributions is typically straightforward even for complex models. However, Bayesian methods are often computer intensive, creating a potential limitation of our approach; it may not be practical for sequential screening of large numbers of SNPs. Another potential limitation in any model fitting is inadequate model convergence. In Bayesian hierarchical models, completely uninformative prior distributions may result in invalid (improper) posterior distributions. The standard solution to this is to incorporate a small amount of prior information to create “weakly informative” prior distributions. In clinical settings like the ones we modeled, this is simple and effective, because we can put reasonable limits on parameters, e.g., we know that for our subjects the age at midpoint of cognitive decline is between 30 and 130 years. The utility of these weak prior distributions can be seen in the fidelity of our trajectory fits to the observed data. Finally, an important modification to our Bayesian implementation was the use of a t-distribution, rather than a normal distribution, to model deviations of measurements from trajectory means. This approach substantially reduced the effects of occasional outlier values, a not uncommon occurrence in our observed data (Supplemental Fig 1).
Other possible limitations to our findings should be considered. We selected for testing a limited number of SNPs based on replicated evidence of genome wide significant association in GWA studies. Other SNPs in these genes, which did not themselves reach genome wide significant association with AD risk, may nevertheless associate with trajectory of cognitive decline. The same may be true for genetic variation in other genes, including a number of SNPs since identified as demonstrating genome wide significant association in GWA studies of AD. Determining a trajectory requires a minimum number of observed measurements. We chose as inclusion criteria the presence of four measurements on the 3MS and DSST tests. This may have by necessity excluded some individuals with more rapid decline, who thus did not complete four tests. Similarly, the individuals who participated in the CHS Cognition Study and were thus available for this analysis were a non-random subset of all CHS participants, healthier and younger than the parent cohort. As such, our findings may not fully generalize to the elderly population.
In summary, we developed a Bayesian approach to addresses several goals for the analysis of cognitive trajectories in aging subjects, including the ability to: fit individual as well as group cognitive trajectories, make meaningful descriptive statements about the pattern of change and the variability in that pattern across subjects, develop credible regions for the resultant curves which include appropriate prediction limits, and evaluate the specific effects of covariates, including genetic covariates, by estimating their impact on the trajectory parameters. We validated this approach with two established predictors of different cognitive trajectory parameters, the presence of APOE ε4 alleles and the development of psychosis. We then used our approach to detect effects of recently identified AD risk alleles on cognitive trajectories. Replication of this approach using other data sets is warranted.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgement
Supported in part by USPHS grant AG05133, AG027224, and AG20098 from the National Institute of Aging. The research reported in this article was also supported by contract numbers N0-1-HC-85239, N01-HC-85079 through N01-HC-85086, N01-HC-35129, N01 HC-15103, N01 HC-55222, N01-HC-75150, N01-HC-45133, grant numbers U01 HL080295 and HL087251 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, with additional contribution from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke and grant AG15928 from the National Institute on Aging, and grant HL087251 from NHLBI. A full list of principal CHS investigators and institutions can be found at http://www.chs-nhlbi.org/pi.htm
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest: The authors have no conflict of interest to report. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health or the Department of Veterans Affairs.
1. Farrer LA, Cupples LA, Haines JL, Hyman B, Kukull WA, Mayeux R, Myers RH, Pericak-Vance MA, Risch N, van Duijn CM. Effects of age sex, and ethnicity on the association between apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease. A meta-analysis. APOE and Alzheimer Disease Meta Analysis Consortium. JAMA. 1997;278:1349–1356. [PubMed]
2. Harold D, Abraham R, Hollingworth P, Sims R, Gerrish A, Hamshere ML, Pahwa JS, Moskvina V, Dowzell K, Williams A, Jones N, Thomas C, Stretton A, Morgan AR, Lovestone S, Powell J, Proitsi P, Lupton MK, Brayne C, Rubinsztein DC, Gill M, Lawlor B, Lynch A, Morgan K, Brown KS, Passmore PA, Craig D, McGuinness B, Todd S, Holmes C, Mann D, Smith AD, Love S, Kehoe PG, Hardy J, Mead S, Fox N, Rossor M, Collinge J, Maier W, Jessen F, Schurmann B, van den BH, Heuser I, Kornhuber J, Wiltfang J, Dichgans M, Frolich L, Hampel H, Hull M, Rujescu D, Goate AM, Kauwe JS, Cruchaga C, Nowotny P, Morris JC, Mayo K, Sleegers K, Bettens K, Engelborghs S, De Deyn PP, Van Broeckhoven C, Livingston G, Bass NJ, Gurling H, McQuillin A, Gwilliam R, Deloukas P, Al Chalabi A, Shaw CE, Tsolaki M, Singleton AB, Guerreiro R, Muhleisen TW, Nothen MM, Moebus S, Jockel KH, Klopp N, Wichmann HE, Carrasquillo MM, Pankratz VS, Younkin SG, Holmans PA, O'Donovan M, Owen MJ, Williams J. Genome-wide association study identifies variants at CLU and PICALM associated with Alzheimer's disease. Nat Genet. 2009;41:1088–1093. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
3. Lambert JC, Heath S, Even G, Campion D, Sleegers K, Hiltunen M, Combarros O, Zelenika D, Bullido MJ, Tavernier B, Letenneur L, Bettens K, Berr C, Pasquier F, Fievet N, Barberger-Gateau P, Engelborghs S, De Deyn P, Mateo I, Franck A, Helisalmi S, Porcellini E, Hanon O, de Pancorbo MM, Lendon C, Dufouil C, Jaillard C, Leveillard T, Alvarez V, Bosco P, Mancuso M, Panza F, Nacmias B, Bossu P, Piccardi P, Annoni G, Seripa D, Galimberti D, Hannequin D, Licastro F, Soininen H, Ritchie K, Blanche H, Dartigues JF, Tzourio C, Gut I, Van Broeckhoven C, Alperovitch A, Lathrop M, Amouyel P. Genome-wide association study identifies variants at CLU and CR1 associated with Alzheimer's disease. Nat Genet. 2009;41:1094–1099. [PubMed]
4. Jack CR, Jr, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, Shaw LM, Aisen PS, Weiner MW, Petersen RC, Trojanowski JQ. Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer's pathological cascade. Lancet Neurol. 2010;9:119–128. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
5. Jack CR, Jr, Vemuri P, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, Aisen PS, Trojanowski JQ, Shaw LM, Bernstein MA, Petersen RC, Weiner MW, Knopman DS. Evidence for Ordering of Alzheimer Disease Biomarkers. Arch. Neurol. 2011 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
6. DeKosky ST, Scheff SW. Synapse loss in frontal cortex biopsies in Alzheimer's disease: Correlation with cognitive severity. Ann. Neurol. 1990;27:457–464. [PubMed]
7. Sweet RA, Wilkosz PA. In: Genetics in Textbook of Geriatric Psychiatry. Blazer DG, Steffens DC, editors. Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc; 2009.
8. Deane R, Sagare A, Hamm K, Parisi M, Lane S, Finn MB, Holtzman DM, Zlokovic BV. apoE isoform-specific disruption of amyloid beta peptide clearance from mouse brain. J. Clin. Invest. 2008;118:4002–4013. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
9. Corder EH, Saunders AM, Strittmatter WJ, Schmechel DE, Gaskell PC, Small GW, Roses AD, Haines JL, Pericak-Vance MA. Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer's disease in late onset families. Science. 1993;261:921–923. [PubMed]
10. Sweet RA, Nimgaonkar VL, Devlin B, Jeste DV. Psychotic symptoms in Alzheimer disease: evidence for a distinct phenotype. Mol. Psychiatry. 2003;8:383–392. [PubMed]
11. Ropacki SA, Jeste DV. Epidemiology of and risk factors for psychosis of Alzheimer's disease: a review of 55 studies published from 1990 to 2003. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2005;162:2022–2030. [PubMed]
12. Emanuel JE, Lopez OL, Houck PR, Becker JT, Weamer EA, DeMichele-Sweet MA, Kuller L, Sweet RA. Trajectory of cognitive decline as a predictor of psychosis in early Alzheimer disease in the cardiovascular health study. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 2011;19:160–168. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
13. Wilkosz PA, Seltman HJ, Devlin B, Weamer EA, Lopez OL, DeKosky ST, Sweet RA. Trajectories of cognitive decline in Alzheimer's disease. Int.Psychogeriatr. 2010;22:281–290. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
14. Ashby D. Bayesian statistics in medicine: a 25 year review. Stat. Med. 2006;25:3589–3631. [PubMed]
15. Lopez OL, Kuller LH, Fitzpatrick A, Ives D, Becker JT, Beauchamp N. Evaluation of dementia in the cardiovascular health cognition study. Neuroepidemiology. 2003;22:1–12. [PubMed]
16. Fried LP, Borhani NO, Enright P, Furberg CD, Gardin JM, Kronmal RA, Kuller LH, Manolio TA, Mittelmark MB, Newman A. The Cardiovascular Health Study: design and rationale. Ann. Epidemiol. 1991;1:263–276. [PubMed]
17. Tell GS, Fried LP, Hermanson B, Manolio TA, Newman AB, Borhani NO. Recruitment of adults 65 years and older as participants in the Cardiovascular Health Study. Ann. Epidemiol. 1993;3:358–366. [PubMed]
18. Seshadri S, Fitzpatrick AL, Ikram MA, DeStefano AL, Gudnason V, Boada M, Bis JC, Smith AV, Carassquillo MM, Lambert JC, Harold D, Schrijvers EM, Ramirez-Lorca R, Debette S, Longstreth WT, Jr, Janssens AC, Pankratz VS, Dartigues JF, Hollingworth P, Aspelund T, Hernandez I, Beiser A, Kuller LH, Koudstaal PJ, Dickson DW, Tzourio C, Abraham R, Antunez C, Du Y, Rotter JI, Aulchenko YS, Harris TB, Petersen RC, Berr C, Owen MJ, Lopez-Arrieta J, Varadarajan BN, Becker JT, Rivadeneira F, Nalls MA, Graff-Radford NR, Campion D, Auerbach S, Rice K, Hofman A, Jonsson PV, Schmidt H, Lathrop M, Mosley TH, Au R, Psaty BM, Uitterlinden AG, Farrer LA, Lumley T, Ruiz A, Williams J, Amouyel P, Younkin SG, Wolf PA, Launer LJ, Lopez OL, van Duijn CM, Breteler MM. Genome-wide analysis of genetic loci associated with Alzheimer disease. JAMA. 2010;303:1832–1840. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
19. Newman AB, Walter S, Lunetta KL, Garcia ME, Slagboom PE, Christensen K, Arnold AM, Aspelund T, Aulchenko YS, Benjamin EJ, Christiansen L, D'Agostino RB, Sr, Fitzpatrick AL, Franceschini N, Glazer NL, Gudnason V, Hofman A, Kaplan R, Karasik D, Kelly-Hayes M, Kiel DP, Launer LJ, Marciante KD, Massaro JM, Miljkovic I, Nalls MA, Hernandez D, Psaty BM, Rivadeneira F, Rotter J, Seshadri S, Smith AV, Taylor KD, Tiemeier H, Uh HW, Uitterlinden AG, Vaupel JW, Walston J, Westendorp RG, Harris TB, Lumley T, van Duijn CM, Murabito JM. A meta-analysis of four genome-wide association studies of survival to age 90 years or older: the Cohorts for Heart and Aging Research in Genomic Epidemiology Consortium. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2010;65:478–487. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
20. Teng EL, Chui HC. The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 1987;48:314–318. [PubMed]
21. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Manual. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1955.
22. Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, Rosenberg-Thompson S, Carusi DA, Gornbein J. The neuropsychiatric inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology. 1994;44:2308–2314. [PubMed]
23. Wilson RS, Beck TL, Bienias JL, Bennett DA. Terminal cognitive decline: accelerated loss of cognition in the last years of life. Psychosom. Med. 2007;69:131–137. [PubMed]
24. Crane PK, Narasimhalu K, Gibbons LE, Mungas DM, Haneuse S, Larson EB, Kuller L, Hall K, van BG. Item response theory facilitated cocalibrating cognitive tests and reduced bias in estimated rates of decline. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2008;61:1018–1027. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
25. Proust-Lima C, Amieva H, Dartigues JF, Jacqmin-Gadda H. Sensitivity of four psychometric tests to measure cognitive changes in brain aging-population-based studies. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2007;165:344–350. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
26. Wijsman EM, Pankratz ND, Choi Y, Rothstein JH, Faber KM, Cheng R, Lee JH, Bird TD, Bennett DA, Diaz-Arrastia R, Goate AM, Farlow M, Ghetti B, Sweet RA, Foroud TM, Mayeux R. Genome-wide association of familial late-onset Alzheimer's disease replicates BIN1 and CLU and nominates CUGBP2 in interaction with APOE. PLoS Genet. 2011;7:e1001308. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
27. Lin PI, Vance JM, Pericak-Vance MA, Martin ER. No gene is an island: the flip-flop phenomenon. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2007;80:531–538. [PubMed]
28. Clarke GM, Cardon LR. Aspects of observing and claiming allele flips in association studies. Genet. Epidemiol. 2010;34:266–274. [PubMed]
29. Devlin B, Cook EH, Jr, Coon H, Dawson G, Grigorenko EL, McMahon W, Minshew N, Pauls D, Smith M, Spence MA, Rodier PM, Stodgell C, Schellenberg GD. Autism and the serotonin transporter: the long and short of it. Mol. Psychiatry. 2005;10:1110–1116. [PubMed]
30. Zaykin DV, Shibata K. Genetic flip-flop without an accompanying change in linkage disequilibrium. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2008;82:794–796. [PubMed]
31. Nuutinen T, Suuronen T, Kauppinen A, Salminen A. Clusterin: a forgotten player in Alzheimer's disease. Brain Res. Rev. 2009;61:89–104. [PubMed]
32. Thambisetty M, Simmons A, Velayudhan L, Hye A, Campbell J, Zhang Y, Wahlund LO, Westman E, Kinsey A, Guntert A, Proitsi P, Powell J, Causevic M, Killick R, Lunnon K, Lynham S, Broadstock M, Choudhry F, Howlett DR, Williams RJ, Sharp SI, Mitchelmore C, Tunnard C, Leung R, Foy C, O'Brien D, Breen G, Furney SJ, Ward M, Kloszewska I, Mecocci P, Soininen H, Tsolaki M, Vellas B, Hodges A, Murphy DG, Parkins S, Richardson JC, Resnick SM, Ferrucci L, Wong DF, Zhou Y, Muehlboeck S, Evans A, Francis PT, Spenger C, Lovestone S. Association of plasma clusterin concentration with severity, pathology, and progression in Alzheimer disease. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2010;67:739–748. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
33. Rosen AM, Stevens B. The role of the classical complement cascade in synapse loss during development and glaucoma. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 2010;703:75–93. [PubMed]
34. Krych-Goldberg M, Atkinson JP. Structure-function relationships of complement receptor type 1. Immunol. Rev. 2001;180:112–122. [PubMed]
35. Brouwers N, Van CC, Engelborghs S, Lambert JC, Bettens K, Le BN, Pasquier F, Montoya AG, Peeters K, Mattheijssens M, Vandenberghe R, De Deyn PP, Cruts M, Amouyel P, Sleegers K, Van BC. Alzheimer risk associated with a copy number variation in the complement receptor 1 increasing C3b/C4b binding sites. Mol. Psychiatry. 2011 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
36. Ford MG, Pearse BM, Higgins MK, Vallis Y, Owen DJ, Gibson A, Hopkins CR, Evans PR, McMahon HT. Simultaneous binding of PtdIns(4,5)P2 and clathrin by AP180 in the nucleation of clathrin lattices on membranes. Science. 2001;291:1051–1055. [PubMed]
37. Baig S, Joseph SA, Tayler H, Abraham R, Owen MJ, Williams J, Kehoe PG, Love S. Distribution and expression of picalm in Alzheimer disease. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 2010;69:1071–1077. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
38. Schjeide BM, Schnack C, Lambert JC, Lill CM, Kirchheiner J, Tumani H, Otto M, Tanzi RE, Lehrach H, Amouyel P, von Arnim CA, Bertram L. The role of clusterin, complement receptor 1, and phosphatidylinositol binding clathrin assembly protein in Alzheimer disease risk and cerebrospinal fluid biomarker levels. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2011;68:207–213. [PubMed]