PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptNIH Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
Ann N Y Acad Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC Aug 1, 2012.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC3369216
NIHMSID: NIHMS377871
Blunted Diurnal Decline of Cortisol among Older Adults with Low Socioeconomic Status
Owhofasa O. Agbedia,1 Vijay Varma,1 Christopher L. Seplaki,2 Teresa Seeman,3 Linda P. Fried,4 Lingsheng Li,1 Gregory C. Harris,1 George W. Rebok,1 Qian-Li Xue,5 Erwin Tan,6 Elizabeth Tanner,7 Jeanine Parisi,1 Sylvia McGill,8 and Michelle C. Carlson1
1Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, Maryland
2Department of Community and Preventive Medicine, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, New York
3Department of Medicine, University of California, Los Angeles
4Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University, New York, New York
5Division of Geriatric Medicine and Gerontology, Department of Medicine, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland
6The Corporation for National and Community Service, Washington, DC
7Johns Hopkins School of Nursing, Baltimore, Maryland
8Greater Homewood Community Corporation, Baltimore, Maryland
Address correspondences to: Michelle C. Carlson, PhD, Department of Mental Health, Center on Aging and Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, 2024 East Monument Street, Suite 2-700, Baltimore, MD 21205. mcarlson/at/jhsph.edu
Low socioeconomic status (SES) is associated with increased risk for adverse health outcomes; those with low SES are thought to experience more environmental disadvantage and exposure to chronic stress over the life course. The effects of chronic stress on health have been measured by cortisol levels and variations in their diurnal pattern. However, the patterns of association between SES and cortisol have been equivocal in older adults. This paper examined in 98 older adults participating in the Brain Health Substudy of the Baltimore Experience Corps Trial baseline patterns of diurnal variation in salivary cortisol associated with lower versus higher SES using total income and perceived SES relative to others. For each measure, participants stratified into lower vs. higher SES showed a more blunted rate of decline in diurnal salivary cortisol over the day in adjusted models (P values ≤ 0.05). There were no SES-related differences in awakening cortisol, cortisol awakening response, or area under the curve. These findings confirm prior evidence of a biologic pathway through which socioeconomic disadvantage is linked to biologic vulnerability, and through which the impact of volunteer service in Experience Corps may be measured.
Keywords: socioeconomic status, salivary cortisol, stress, diurnal pattern, HPA axis, resilience
A key question researchers have sought to answer relates to the mechanism through which low socioeconomic status (SES) gets under the skin to affect health and aging. Previous studies have shown that SES has a significant impact on health outcomes in adulthood, predicting increased risk for chronic diseases and mortality.16 The cumulative effects of SES are visible across multiple biological systems.6, 7
The most widely studied pathway, or mechanism, by which low SES is thought to impact health is through chronic excitation of the stress response in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, a major biological regulatory system. Evidence indicates that low SES is often associated with a shallower, or, blunted pattern of diurnal decline in cortisol secretion, sometimes resulting in persistently higher salivary levels over the course of the day.811 Salivary cortisol is a noninvasive, surrogate marker for blood cortisol.12 Among individuals with healthy patterns, levels rise steeply during the 30–45 minute period immediately after wake-up this is known as the cortisol awakening response (CAR) – and then decline gradually from this peak (with mild elevations at meal times) to reach a nadir at bed time. Various features of the daily cortisol profile have been examined, including cortisol levels at wake-up, CAR, area under the curve (AUC), and the diurnal slope.811 There is a lack of consensus regarding those component measures of diurnal cortisol that relate most directly to health.13 Conflicting results in prior studies may be due, in part, to the lack of standardization in the use of cortisol measures and compliance.13 Low SES is often associated with a shallower or, blunted pattern of diurnal decline in cortisol secretion and persistently elevated levels of cortisol over the course of the day.811
Prior research on the association between SES and health have used measures of objective and perceived SES, including level of education attained, income, occupational status or level within an organization, and perceived SES relative to others. More recent measures of SES include annual combined household income, total savings, and home ownership to yield a more comprehensive index of the financial situation over years.
The majority of empirical evidence relating SES to cortisol is derived from young and middle-aged populations. In older populations, low SES has been associated with lower cortisol levels upon waking and flattened, or ‘blunted’, pattern of diurnal cortisol secretion.14 In the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis, Hajat et al. examined objective measures of SES and reported that low SES groups and Blacks and Hispanics had lower levels of waking cortisol and a flattened decline during the early part of the day.15 Wright and Steptoe examined subjective SES, gender, and CAR in an older population and found an association between low social status on the MacArthur Scale of Subjective Social Status 16 and a larger CAR after adjusting for biobehavioral and other factors that may mediate the relationship between SES and cortisol.17 Although these results are consistent with the notion that SES is independently associated with various cortisol measures, few studies have investigated this relationship in a sample of majority lower SES, sociodemographically at risk, minority older adults.
Studying the relationship between SES and diurnal cortisol in older, socio-demographically at-risk adults is important for evaluating how SES over the life course influences vulnerability to chronic diseases, such as diabetes, stroke, heart disease, and dementia, which increases exponentially with age. Additionally, through the identification and use of sensitive measures of biologic susceptibility within lower SES groups, we will be better able to understand behaviors and factors that promote resilience despite environmental disadvantage.
This aim of this study was to explore the baseline relationship between measures of SES and salivary cortisol in individuals participating in the Baltimore Experience Corps Trial (BECT) Brain Health Substudy (BHS). We hypothesized that greater biologic susceptibility associated with lower SES would be associated with blunted decline in diurnal cortisol and elevated diurnal cortisol levels.
Study Population
Salivary cortisol and SES were measured on participants enrolled in the BHS, a substudy within the BECT. From 2006 to 2010, the BECT randomized a total of 702 older adults to volunteer service designed to improve the academic outcomes of children and increase older adults’ health through increased physical, social, and cognitive activity. Participants randomized to the intervention volunteered for 15 hours per week over two academic years in Baltimore City public elementary schools, and were given standardized cognitive, psychological, physical, and functional health assessments at baseline and two annual follow ups. The study design and eligibility criteria for the BECT have been described elsewhere.18 Inclusion criteria included a Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT19) reading score above 44 (equivalent to 6th grade level) and a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE20) equal to or above 24. Additional enrollment criteria for BHS participants included: right-hand dominance, no prior history of a pacemaker or other ferrous metal objects in the body, and no history of brain cancer, brain aneurism, or stroke in the prior year. Protocol for data collection was approved by the Johns Hopkins School of Medicine Institutional Review Board, and all participants provided written, informed consent. We measured salivary cortisol levels in 114 of the 123 participants who were enrolled between 2006 and 2009. Based on criteria described in the next section, 16 participants were excluded from the final analysis, leaving 98 participants in this cross-sectional analysis. Those excluded did not differ from the final sample in socio-demographic characteristics including age, sex, race, and education (P values > 0.05).
Cortisol Data Collection
Salivary swabs were taken at home by participants immediately after their BHS baseline evaluation at four time points: (i) immediately after wake-up (before brushing), (ii) between 1/2-1 hour after wake-up, (iii) before dinner, and (iv) before bed. Participants were asked to record the time of sample collection. Saliva was collected on small cotton swabs housed in plastic vials (Salivettes, Sardstedt, Numbrecht, Germany) by asking subjects to chew each swab until the swab was saturated. Swabs were refrigerated until laboratory analysis. Cortisol was measured using a competitive immunoassay (HS-Cort Kit, Salimetic LLC, State College, PA) designed for quantitative measurement of salivary cortisol. We excluded 16 participants, noted above, who provided samples outside an acceptable window (>10mins after wake-up at first cortisol sample collection and >60mins between first and second sample collection), and participants who did not provide at least three samples (times i, ii, and iii/iv).
Outcome Measures and Independent Variables
Four measures of the diurnal cortisol pattern were used as outcomes: wake-up cortisol level, CAR (a measure of diurnal HPA axis activity), AUC (a summary measure that captures total diurnal cortisol secretion over the course of the day), and the rate of decline over the period during which samples 2–4 were collected. All salivary cortisol values were log-transformed in analyses except for AUC.
Two measures of socioeconomic status were examined. Perceived location on the SES ladder assessed a participant’s perceived position relative to others in society as summarized by “high” SES (upper 5 rungs) or “low” SES (lower 5 rungs) and is thought to be more comprehensive than income or education alone.16 In addition, participants reported total current, combined household yearly income before tax, which was dichotomized based on median split in the sample as less than $25,000 (low SES) and $25,000 or more (high SES). Covariates included in all models were identified based on prior research demonstrating their association with cortisol levels and include: age, sex, body mass index (BMI; calculated using height and weight) and years of education.
In depicting and modeling diurnal decline, we noticed variability in the times of sample collection (e.g. mean (range): time 2: 8:00 a.m. (4:45–12:20); time 3: 6:14 p.m. (4:00–9:55); time 4: 8:59 p.m. (7:00–12:30). Therefore, to better model individual rates of decline, we used a response feature analysis, or a two-stage analysis, to summarize cortisol samples at times 2–4 for each participant to a single biologically meaningful response that more sensitively captures variability in collection time. For each individual, we estimated the rate of decline as the slope of a linear regression of logarithm of the cortisol values at exact sample times 2–4. This methodology substantially improved model fit compared to standard linear regression methodologies that assume fixed collection times. In addition to all other cortisol measures, the rate of decline generated from the model was analyzed using fixed effects one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) to model the relationship between cortisol and SES measures. We performed a univariate linear regression to test the unadjusted association between salivary cortisol measures and (1) position on the SES ladder and (2) combined household income before tax. We then included covariates and reported the significance of regression coefficients (Table 2). Analyses were performed using STATA 11.2 (StataCorp, College Station, TX).
Table 2
Table 2
Mean values of diurnal cortisol response by perceived SES position with unadjusted and adjusted regression coefficients.
Table 1 shows baseline characteristics of the study sample. Participants were majority female (71.4%) and African American (90.8%) with a mean age of 66.8 years. Thirty-seven percent of participants had a high school or less education, the median income before tax was $30,000, and 23.5% fell below the poverty threshold using income/household size and based on 2011 Health and Human Services poverty guidelines.21 Participants had a mean MMSE score of 28.4, and, based on self-reported health status, 74.2% had hypertension, 60.8% had osteoarthritis, and 30.9% had diabetes. Approximately 42% of participants placed themselves on the upper half of the SES ladder, and 56% reported incomes of $25,000 or more.
Table 1
Table 1
Baseline demographic, health, and cognitive characteristics of the Baltimore Experience Corps Trial (BECT) Brain Health Substudy.
Table 2 shows the mean value of diurnal cortisol parameters by perceived SES position, the unadjusted and the adjusted (regression coefficients; P-values) measures of association between cortisol measures and perceived position on the SES ladder. Participants in the low SES group (mean rate of decline = −0.04) had a significantly blunted rate of decline in diurnal cortisol relative to those in the high SES group (mean rate of decline = −0.05) as evident by the unadjusted (β = −0.02; P = 0.04) and adjusted coefficients (β = −0.03, P = 0.05) (note: high SES used as the reference group). There were no group differences in awakening cortisol, CAR, and AUC before or after adjustment for age, sex, BMI and education (P > 0.05). Similar results are depicted in Table 3 when stratifying participants by combined household income before tax. Participants with a combined household income of less than $25,000 (mean rate of decline = −0.04) had a significantly blunted rate of decline in diurnal cortisol compared to those with a combined household income of $25,000 or more (mean rate of decline = −0.06) as evident by the unadjusted (β = −0.002; P = 0.03) and adjusted coefficients (β = −0.04, P = 0.02) (note: high SES used as the reference group). Again we did not observe significant group differences in awakening cortisol, CAR, and AUC.
Table 3
Table 3
Mean values of diurnal cortisol response by combined household income before tax with unadjusted and adjusted regression coefficients.
Overall, whether defining low SES according to perceived location on the SES ladder or by combined household income, participants with low SES exhibited a blunted or slower decline in the diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion over the day.
We explored in a socioeconomically at-risk cohort of urban-dwelling older adults enrolled in a large-scale trial of a social health promotion program the baseline associations between measures of diurnal variation in cortisol and SES. Whether classifying participants according to perceived location in the lower vs. upper half of the SES ladder or by combined current household income of above and below $25,000, we observed blunted decline in diurnal cortisol among participants in the lower vs. higher SES strata. These associations were unaltered when adjusting for age, sex, and BMI. We did not observe SES-related differences in awakening cortisol, CAR, or AUC. Our results confirm studies reporting a blunted decline in diurnal cortisol among lower vs. higher SES groups2224, extending this work to an older, community-dwelling sample of socioecomically at-risk adults. They suggest that one major biologic mechanism through which environmental disadvantage accumulates is through dysregulation, e.g. chronic elevation, in daily cortisol levels as demonstrated here by blunted diurnal decline.
Blunted diurnal cortisol appears to be a sensitive measure of decreased physiologic resilience under the concepts of frailty and disability, and, allostatic load (AL) and risk for chronic disease. Frailty has been conceptualized as a disease-independent state of impaired regulation across multiple systems and is associated with increased vulnerability to physical disability.2527 In earlier work among older women, we provided the first epidemiologic evidence that higher levels and blunted diurnal cortisol were associated with increasing frailty burden, whereas awakening was not.28 AL has been described by Seeman et al. 6 as a biological pathway through which environmental risk factors over the life course cumulatively impose a significant physiological burden on multiple, interrelated body systems.2931
Biological evidence for the deleterious effects of over-stimulation of the HPA-axis can be observed using numerous markers of brain health, including atrophy, deactivation, and cell death in the hippocampus 32, a region important to memory and risk for Alzheimer’s Disease. Behaviorally, healthy older adults with increasing and elevated cortisol levels over the preceding four years exhibited poorer memory and attention relative to those with lower levels.33 Elevated and increasing levels of cortisol were also associated with reduced hippocampal volumes.34 These effects may be compounded, or exacerbated, in those experiencing chronic environmental disadvantage through low SES.
The socioeconomic composition of older adults participating in the BECT offers a critical opportunity to examine the intersection of aging and life time cumulative risk through low SES, given that these individuals are at elevated risk for disease and disability. Education was variable with 37% having a high school education or less. The median total income before taxes in our sample was $30,000, and often represents combined income in many participants who report living with others. The federal poverty guidelines commonly used by the Department of Health and Human Services (HHS) (2011) for a family of four is $22,350. In addition, the majority of participants are African-American, and represent a segment of the population experiencing increased financial strain that likely affects health.35
In this group, we nonetheless found numerous similarities across socioeconomic strata in diurnal cortisol levels for awakening, CAR, and AUC. Specifically, we did not observe a difference in awakening cortisol level according to perceived SES or combined household income. Several studies have reported higher awakening cortisol levels among high SES participants8, 9, 36 although few studies have reported the opposite association.37, 38 Other studies, like ours, reported no difference in awakening cortisol by SES group.14, 15, 37, 39, 40 These equivocal findings may reflect variability in socioeconomic strata studied, and variable sleep-wake patterns, as observed here with wake times ranging from 4:00 a.m. to 10:00 a.m. Similarly, for the CAR, results are inconclusive with some finding an association with SES.14, 37, 41 We did not observe a difference in CAR by perceived SES and by total current household income, confirming reports from several other studies.14, 36, 37, 41 Whereas a lower CAR has been associated with chronic health problems, posttraumatic stress disorder, chronic fatigue syndrome, and sleep disorders, day-to-day fluctuations in CAR have been observed.42 Finally, AUC is a commonly used measure of total cortisol secreted with studies finding that lower SES was significantly related to a higher AUC.15, 17, 43 However, our study showed no association between AUC and SES. This may be due, in part, to limited sensitivity of the AUC when two participants have the same AUC value but different patterns of diurnal secretion that may suggest chronically elevated levels.
An important factor in the assessment of diurnal cortisol that may partially explain equivocal findings is the variability in and accuracy with which one takes and reports collection at the indicated times. Differential adherence to timing contributes to artificial blunting of the diurnal cortisol pattern.7, 41 However, few investigators report compliance rates in the collection of samples at designated times and this has a significant impact on the reported results.7, 13, 14, 41, 43, 44 In modeling the data, we observed great variability in sample collection times and recognized the potential for measurement error by assuming fixed collection times. Therefore, we applied one fitted regression slope and incorporated time administered for samples 2–4, thus reducing the potential for measurement error and increasing sensitivity in detecting associations between SES and diurnal cortisol (see methods). The methodologic approach used here to model diurnal decline in cortisol represents a novel metric intended to mitigate the effect of this unmeasured variability and a represents a particular strength of the study.
This study identified biologic vulnerability among individuals with lower SES who are participating in a two-year randomized trial studying the effects of activity and environmental enrichment through volunteer service to elementary school children. Having collected annual follow-up cortisol samples over the two years of the BECT, we will now be able to examine whether this biologic link to chronic environmental deprivation and disadvantage over the life course can be partially reversed when these individuals volunteer in an enriched environment. Specifically, we will extend this work to determine how such vulnerability over the life course may be partially reversed through these same plastic regulatory pathways. Animal models show that enriched environments elicit neurogenesis or synaptogenesis (i.e., growth of new neurons or formation of new synapses between neurons), and reduced neuronal death, especially in hippocampal structures important to memory 45 and dementia risk. There is ample evidence of these neuroplastic changes in older animals.4650 However, this work has only recently begun to be translated to human studies of chronic stress 51, and remains to be explored in older adults who may have experienced decades of accumulated physiologic burden in multiple, interrelated systems.
There are a number of study limitations that warrant consideration. First, while the study sample was relatively small (N <100), but was nonetheless comparable to other studies examining diurnal cortisol in older adults and was sufficiently powered to observe SES-related differences in diurnal decline using two different measures. Second, our collection of cortisol samples was restricted to one day unlike other studies that obtained samples over three or more days.14, 15, 52, 53 To address this limitation, we are currently administering a validation study to assess cortisol concentrations over two days. Third, we did not adjust for the day of the week during which samples were obtained, as daily patterns may be less variable in retired adults. Nonetheless, this limitation suggests that our results may represent a conservative estimate of the association between cortisol and SES. Finally, regarding genearlizability, these findings were from a study of participants willing to enroll in a two-year trial of high-intensity volunteer service. These individuals may represent the health-conscious members of their community, which again would lead to a conservative estimate of the association between SES and biologic vulnerability as measured by diurnal cortisol.
We used perceived position on the SES ladder as an SES measure to complement current total income; because most studies exploring this association have not used the SES ladder, we were unable to compare our results to multiple studies. However it should be noted that utilization of the SES ladder provides more information about the social status of an individual across multiple SES indicators compared to education and or current income especially in an older population.16
These cross-sectional findings contribute to our understanding as to how environmental disadvantage and social conditions ‘get under the skin’ to affect health and aging over the life course.54 Elevated cortisol levels in low SES older adults may be indicative of cumulative disadvantage and associated physiologic burden over the life course that leads to decreased biologic resilience in the face of additional insult. Although many studies have focused on the correlation between SES and salivary cortisol in children, adolescents, and adults, few have examined this association in older adults who may have experienced a life time of accumulated SES burden and who are entering an age of elevated risk for disease and disability. The results of this study among socioeconomically at-risk participants enrolled in a social health promotion model to promote cognitive, physical, and psychological health will provide further insight into how increased biologic vulnerability, as indexed by blunted diurnal cortisol levels, may be reversed along the same physiologic pathways through environmental enrichment and advantage.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging (NIA) BSR Grant # P01AG027735-03, by the NIA Claude D. Pepper Older Americans Independence Center Grant #P30AG021334, and by the Johns Hopkins Neurobehavioral Research Unit. Dr. Seplaki is supported by Mentored Research Scientist Development Award number K01AG031332 from the National Institute on Aging.
1. Baum A, Garofalo JP, Yali AM. Socioeconomic status and chronic stress. Does stress account for SES effects on health? Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;896:131–144. [PubMed]
2. Drever F, Whitehead M. Health inequalities. London: Office for National Statistics; 1997.
3. Evans R, Barer M, Marmor T. Why are some people healthy and others not? The determinants of health of populations. New York: Aldine de Gruyter; 1994.
4. Kunst AE, Geurts JJ, van den Berg J. International variation in socioeconomic inequalities in self reported health. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1995 Apr;49(2):117–123. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
5. Kunst AE, Groenhof F, Mackenbach JP, Health EW. Occupational class and cause specific mortality in middle aged men in 11 European countries: comparison of population based studies. EU Working Group on Socioeconomic Inequalities in Health. Bmj. 1998 May 30;316(7145):1636–1642. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
6. Seeman T, Epel E, Gruenewald T, Karlamangla A, McEwen BS. Socio-economic differentials in peripheral biology: cumulative allostatic load. Ann N Y Acad Sci. Feb;1186:223–239. [PubMed]
7. Dowd JB, Simanek AM, Aiello AE. Socio-economic status, cortisol and allostatic load: a review of the literature. Int J Epidemiol. 2009 Oct;38(5):1297–1309. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
8. Steptoe A, Kunz-Ebrecht S, Owen N, et al. Socioeconomic status and stress-related biological responses over the working day. Psychosom Med. 2003 May-Jun;65(3):461–470. [PubMed]
9. Brandtstadter J, Baltes-Gotz B, Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer D. Developmental and personality correlates of adrenocortical activity as indexed by salivary cortisol: observations in the age range of 35 to 65 years. J Psychosom Res. 1991;35(2–3):173–185. [PubMed]
10. Kristenson M, Kucinskiene Z, Bergdahl B, Orth-Gomer K. Risk factors for coronary heart disease in different socioeconomic groups of Lithuania and Sweden--the LiVicordia Study. Scand J Public Health. 2001 Jun;29(2):140–150. [PubMed]
11. Lupien SJ, King S, Meaney MJ, McEwen BS. Child’s stress hormone levels correlate with mother’s socioeconomic status and depressive state. Biol Psychiatry. 2000 Nov 15;48(10):976–980. [PubMed]
12. Hellhammer DH, Wust S, Kudielka BM. Salivary cortisol as a biomarker in stress research. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009 Feb;34(2):163–171. [PubMed]
13. Garcia MC, de Souza A, Bella GP, Grassi-Kassisse DM, Tacla AP, Spadari-Bratfisch RC. Salivary cortisol levels in Brazilian citizens of distinct socioeconomic and cultural levels. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008 Dec;1148:504–508. [PubMed]
14. Cohen S, Schwartz JE, Epel E, Kirschbaum C, Sidney S, Seeman T. Socioeconomic status, race, and diurnal cortisol decline in the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study. Psychosom Med. 2006 Jan-Feb;68(1):41–50. [PubMed]
15. Hajat A, Diez-Roux A, Franklin TG, et al. Socioeconomic and race/ethnic differences in daily salivary cortisol profiles: the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. Jul;35(6):932–943. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
16. Adler NE, Epel ES, Castellazzo G, Ickovics JR. Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy white women. Health Psychol. 2000 Nov;19(6):586–592. [PubMed]
17. Wright CE, Steptoe A. Subjective socioeconomic position, gender and cortisol responses to waking in an elderly population. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005 Jul;30(6):582–590. [PubMed]
18. Fried LP, Carlson MC, McGill S, et al. Experience Corps: a dual trial to promote the health of older adults and children’s academic success. Under Review 2011 [PubMed]See also Carlson MC, Erickson KI, Kramer AF, et al. Evidence for neurocognitive plasticity in at-risk older adults: the experience corps program. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64:1275–1282. [PubMed]
19. Wilkinson GS. WRAT-3: Wide Range Achievement Test, Administration Manual. Wilmington, DE: Wide Range, Inc; 1993.
20. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975 Nov;12(3):189–198. [PubMed]
21. The U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. The 2011 HHS poverty guidelines. < http://aspe.hhs.gov/poverty/11poverty.shtml>.
22. Eller NH, Netterstrom B, Hansen AM. Psychosocial factors at home and at work and levels of salivary cortisol. Biol Psychol. 2006 Oct;73(3):280–287. [PubMed]
23. Smyth JM, Ockenfels MC, Gorin AA, et al. Individual differences in the diurnal cycle of cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1997 Feb;22(2):89–105. [PubMed]
24. Stone AA, Schwartz JE, Smyth J, et al. Individual differences in the diurnal cycle of salivary free cortisol: a replication of flattened cycles for some individuals. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2001 Apr;26(3):295–306. [PubMed]
25. Fried LP, Tangen CM, Walston J, et al. Frailty in older adults: evidence for a phenotype. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2001 Mar;56(3):M146–156. [PubMed]
26. Rockwood K, Fox RA, Stolee P, Robertson D, Beattie BL. Frailty in elderly people: an evolving concept. Cmaj. 1994 Feb 15;150(4):489–495. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
27. Fried LPWJ. Frailty and failure to thrive. In: Hazzard WR, Vlass JP, Ettinger WH Jr, Halter JB, Ouslander J, editors. Principles of Geriatric Medicine and Gerontology. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1998. pp. 1387–1402.
28. Varadhan R, Walston J, Cappola AR, Carlson MC, Wand GS, Fried LP. Higher levels and blunted diurnal variation of cortisol in frail older women. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2008 Feb;63(2):190–195. [PubMed]
29. McEwen BS. Stress, adaptation, and disease. Allostasis and allostatic load. Ann N YAcad Sci. 1998 May 1;840:33–44. [PubMed]
30. McEwen BS, Stellar E. Stress and the individual. Mechanisms leading to disease. Arch Intern Med. 1993 Sep 27;153(18):2093–2101. [PubMed]
31. Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009 Jun;10(6):434–445. [PubMed]
32. McEwen BS. Stress and the aging hippocampus. Front Neuroendocrinol. 1999 Jan;20(1):49–70. [PubMed]
33. Seeman TE, Singer BH, Rowe JW, Horwitz RI, McEwen BS. Price of adaptation--allostatic load and its health consequences. MacArthur studies of successful aging. Arch Intern Med. 1997 Oct 27;157(19):2259–2268. [PubMed]
34. Lupien S, Lecours AR, Lussier I, Schwartz G, Nair NP, Meaney MJ. Basal cortisol levels and cognitive deficits in human aging. J Neurosci. 1994 May;14(5 Pt 1):2893–2903. [PubMed]
35. Szanton SL, Thorpe RJ, Jr, Johnson B, Whitfield K. Life course financial strain and health in African-Americans. Soc Sci Med. 2010;71:259–265. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
36. Bennett GG, Merritt MM, Wolin KY. Ethnicity, education, and the cortisol response to awakening: a preliminary investigation. Ethn Health. 2004 Nov;9(4):337–347. [PubMed]
37. Ockenfels MC, Porter L, Smyth J, Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH, Stone AA. Effect of chronic stress associated with unemployment on salivary cortisol: overall cortisol levels, diurnal rhythm, and acute stress reactivity. Psychosom Med. 1995 Sep-Oct;57(5):460–467. [PubMed]
38. Williams DR. Symposium on cognitive function and development. American Association for the Advancement of Science 2010. Annual meeting; February 19, 2010; San Diego, CA. 2010.
39. Kunz-Ebrecht SR, Kirschbaum C, Marmot M, Steptoe A. Differences in cortisol awakening response on work days and weekends in women and men from the Whitehall II cohort. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004 May;29(4):516–528. [PubMed]
40. Ranjit N, Young EA, Kaplan GA. Material hardship alters the diurnal rhythm of salivary cortisol. Int J Epidemiol. 2005 Oct;34(5):1138–1143. [PubMed]
41. Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Baum A. Socioeconomic status is associated with stress hormones. Psychosom Med. 2006 May-Jun;68(3):414–420. [PubMed]
42. Adam EK, Hawkley LC, Kudielka BM, Cacioppo JT. Day-to-day dynamics of experience--cortisol associations in a population-based sample of older adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006 Nov 7;103(45):17058–17063. [PubMed]
43. Li L, Power C, Kelly S, Kirschbaum C, Hertzman C. Life-time socio-economic position and cortisol patterns in mid-life. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007 Aug;32(7):824–833. [PubMed]
44. DeSantis AS, Adam EK, Doane LD, Mineka S, Zinbarg RE, Craske MG. Racial/ethnic differences in cortisol diurnal rhythms in a community sample of adolescents. J Adolesc Health. 2007 Jul;41(1):3–13. [PubMed]
45. Kempermann G, Chesler EJ, Lu L, Williams RW, Gage FH. Natural variation and genetic covariance in adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006 Jan 17;103(3):780–785. [PubMed]
46. Black JE, Isaacs KR, Anderson BJ, Alcantara AA, Greenough WT. Learning causes synaptogenesis, whereas motor activity causes angiogenesis, in cerebellar cortex of adult rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1990 Jul;87(14):5568–5572. [PubMed]
47. Kempermann G, Kuhn HG, Gage FH. More hippocampal neurons in adult mice living in an enriched environment. Nature. 1997 Apr 3;386(6624):493–495. [PubMed]
48. Rosenzweig MR, Bennett EL. Psychobiology of plasticity: effects of training and experience on brain and behavior. Behav Brain Res. 1996 Jun;78(1):57–65. [PubMed]
49. Reige Environmental influences on brain and behavior of year-old rats. Dev Psychobiol. 1971;4(2):157–167. [PubMed]
50. Diamond MC. Response of the brain to enrichment. An Acad Bras Cienc. 2001 Jun;73(2):211–220. [PubMed]
51. Liston C, McEwen BS, Casey BJ. Psychosocial stress reversibly disrupts prefrontal processing and attentional control. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009 Jan 20;106(3):912–917. [PubMed]
52. Hellhammer J, Fries E, Schweisthal OW, Schlotz W, Stone AA, Hagemann D. Several daily measurements are necessary to reliably assess the cortisol rise after awakening: state- and trait components. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007 Jan;32(1):80–86. [PubMed]
53. Decker SA. Salivary cortisol and social status among Dominican men. Horm Behav. 2000 Aug;38(1):29–38. [PubMed]
54. Lupie SJ, King S, Meaney MJ, McEwen BS. Can poverty get under your skin? basal cortisol levels and cognitive function in children from low and high socioeconomic status. Dev Psychopathol. 2001 Summer;13(3):653–676. [PubMed]