PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptNIH Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
J Am Board Fam Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC Feb 24, 2012.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC3286113
NIHMSID: NIHMS354827
Information Chaos in Primary Care: Implications for Physician Performance and Patient Safety
John W Beasley, MD,1,2 Tosha B. Wetterneck, MD, MS,3 Jon Temte, MD, PhD,1 Jamie A Lapin, MS,2 Paul Smith, MD,1 A. Joy Rivera-Rodriguez, MS,2 and Ben-Tzion Karsh, PhD*1,2
1Department of Family Medicine, UW-Madison School of Medicine and Public Health
2Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, UW-Madison
3Department of Medicine, UW-Madison School of Medicine and Public Health
* Corresponding author: Ben-Tzion Karsh, PhD Industrial and Systems Engineering, UW-Madison, 1513 University Avenue, Room 3218, Madison, WI 53706, tel: 608-262-3002, fax: 608-262-8454, bkarsh/at/engr.wisc.edu
Purpose
The purpose of this paper is to explore the concept of information chaos as it applies to the issues of patient safety and physician workload in primary care and to propose a research agenda.
Methods
We use a human factors engineering perspective to discuss the concept of information chaos in primary care and explore implications for its impact on physician performance and patient safety.
Results
Information chaos is comprised of various combinations of information overload, information underload, information scatter, information conflict, and erroneous information. We provide a framework for understanding information chaos, its impact on physician mental workload and situation awareness, its consequences, discuss possible solutions and suggest a research agenda which may lead to methods to reduce the problem.
Conclusions
Information chaos is experienced routinely by primary care physicians. This is not just inconvenient, annoying and frustrating; it has implications for physician performance and patient safety. Additional research is needed to define methods to measure and eventually reduce information chaos.
Keywords: Primary Health Care, Information Management/Informatics, Complexity Science, Medical Errors, Practice Management
Primary care is comprised of first contact care, longitudinal care, comprehensive care, and coordinated care.1, 2 Without information that is comprehensive, accessible, timely and correct, primary care physicians (PCPs) cannot deliver high quality care. Information problems do exist, 3-7 with an extrapolation from one study suggesting that errors related to information handling account for 29% of family medicine errors. 8 Because information is central to the effectiveness of primary care, we propose a conceptual framework to explain the relationship between information hazards, i.e. information problems that may contribute to errors, and PCPs’ ability to provide high quality care.
This framework is especially timely given the emphasis on electronic health record (EHR) use in primary care. The patient centered medical home (PCMH) movement encourages EHR implementation to improve information handling. However, EHR systems may not meet this goal. 9-11 Furthermore, in 2010 the “Meaningful Use” regulations brought incentives and penalties to promote EHR use. 12 EHRs can transform the quality, comprehensiveness, timeliness, and accessibility of information in both good and bad ways. 13 Both the move toward PCMH and the need to adopt EHRs will have effects on information needs and the design of support systems in primary care. It is therefore important to understand how information relates to primary care.
The vignette in Figure 1 illustrates five specific information hazards that arise in primary care and how they can lead to information chaos.
Figure 1
Figure 1
Clinical Vignette Illustrating Information Hazards
Information overload
Information overload occurs when there are too many data, e.g. written, verbal and non-verbal, physician’s memory, for the clinician to organize, synthesize, draw conclusions from, or act on. Primary care involves the integration of an increasing number of patient problems while taking into account family and community factors,1, 14, 15 The PCP care coordination role further adds to the volume of information PCPs must process and act on. 2, 16, 17 EHRs may make the situation worse by encouraging electronic copying and pasting, adding irrelevant information through use of templates, and the mixing of data needed for billing and legal protection with that needed for clinical care. One physician has called the problem “high fiber medical records” (Russo P, personal communication).
Information underload
Information underload occurs when necessary information is lacking. The lack of sufficient and available information is common in primary care.18 Problems include unavailable or incomplete records or poor recall by physician or patient.8 Patients may decide to not disclose sensitive information. At times, physicians are unaware that information is even available. For example, a physician may prescribe a “new” medication, only to find out later from the patient that it was already tried and stopped because of a side effect. Although this information may be in the chart, the physician may have not known it was there. Information that is theoretically “in the chart,” but not readily accessible, is for all intents and purposes, invisible.
Information scatter
Information scatter refers to having information located in multiple places. In any given primary care encounter, the needed information may be located on the intake form, in the paper chart or EHR, at the clinic of a consultant or a hospital, or in the patient’s or the clinician’s mind. While EHRs may reduce the problem of information scatter,19 they can also worsen the problem due to inadequate search methods to find information quickly and effectively.13
Information conflict
Information conflict occurs when the clinician is unable to determine which data are correct. Often the clinician is confronted with a variety of conflicting data regarding an issue. For example, the patient may think he or she is on one medication, the clinic record suggests a different one, and the hospital discharge note states yet another.
Erroneous information
This is when the information is wrong. For example, the patient tells the doctor they are taking aspirin for pain when in fact they are taking acetaminophen or information is entered into the wrong patient chart.4. When incorrect information gets into a record it may be hard to purge it or it may simply get replicated by clinicians without re-validating it with the patient.20
We conceptualize these five information hazards: information overload, underload, scatter, conflict and erroneous information, as information chaos. Separately or together, these hazards increase the risk of an information-related error and are experienced by primary care clinicians on a daily basis. While the concept of information chaos resonates with PCPs, the nature of information chaos and the factors that can affect the relationship between it and patient outcomes have not been explored in primary care and are likely underappreciated by clinicians, administrators, EHR programmers and planners. Information chaos is more than inconvenient, annoying and frustrating; there are operational implications that can impair physician performance, increase workload and reduce the safety and quality of care delivered.
The field of human factors engineering and ergonomics (HFE)21-24 contributes to our understanding of information chaos. HFE studies and applies information about human cognitive and physical limitations, and human abilities to the design of systems, including, for example, work processes, tools, technologies, and environments, and in turn how systems impact human cognitive and physical performance. From HFE science, we hypothesize a conceptual framework for the way information chaos relates to the cognitive processes in primary care. This is presented in Figure 2.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Information Chaos, Mental Workload, Situational Awareness, Moderators and Impacts
Information chaos imposes demands on PCPs. Because of information overload, they may not be able to adequately review all the information and may miss important details. Because of information underload, scatter, and conflict, PCPs must expend effort searching for needed information through charts, phone calls, further questions to the patient and perhaps unnecessary testing. Erroneous information may lead to re-work, errors in diagnosis, or treatment decisions that threaten patient safety. The work PCPs do in response to information chaos is not value-added to the visit; it is only necessary to deal with the chaos. Importantly, it distracts the PCP from the primary work of the visit: caring for the patient.
From a cognitive point of view, information chaos, may contribute to two unwanted outcomes: impaired situation awareness (SA)25-30 and increased mental workload.31-33 SA is defined as a person’s awareness and understanding of his/her task-related situation. It has three levels: (1) perception of elements in the environment, e.g., patient cues/stimuli (pulse, color), chart, EHR, nurse, (2) comprehension of the meaning of those elements by integrating the disparate pieces of information and determining what is important and (3) projection of future status so that decisions can be made.25, 34 Whether or not accurate SA is achieved is dependent on the timing, comprehensiveness, accessibility, and quality of the information obtained. That means information chaos can directly impair SA. If SA is poor, it can result in impaired decision making.25, 26, 29, 30, 35
A related variable is mental workload, which is the amount of cognitive resources required for a task. Mental workload is a function of demand, which may be increased due to information chaos, and available resources, such as time.31, 33 Mental workload and SA are related. Poor SA increases mental workload, and high mental workload can degrade SA.36 High physician mental workload occurs when the mental demands imposed on the physician exceeds the physician’s resources cope with those demands. Both the poor SA and the high mental workload generated by information chaos can impair cognitive work29, 31, 33, 35 (e.g., diagnosis and treatment decisions), and may have negative impacts on safety.35
At the start of an office visit a PCP will have some SA, but it is incomplete. The clinician only knows what he/she remembers from previous visits, from a brief look at the patient’s chart, and possibly from a short meeting with the nurse who roomed the patient. As soon as the clinician enters the room, SA is dynamically updated based on sensory inputs such as how the patient looks, feels and sounds and from higher level processes such as communication with the patient and searching in the medical record. Whether or not accurate SA is achieved is dependent on the timing and quality of the information obtained through sensation, perception, communication and record searching. Because of information chaos, the clinician may never fully develop SA, and, due to information underload, may not even know they are not fully or accurately aware of the situation as is suggested by work in related fields. 37
During times of high mental workload which may be increased by information chaos, people involuntarily focus on fewer cues, considering fewer options and solutions because of cognitive tunneling.32 This occurs when people zoom in on a narrow set of cues or options because mentally they cannot handle more. In such situations, people are at risk for decision errors38 because they miss things they should have noticed such as patient symptoms, or abnormal lab results. In other words, in situations of high mental workload, people operate with selective and reduced capacity.32 Among PCPs, higher mental workload has been positively associated with a higher perceived probability of medical error.39
The magnitude of the effects of information chaos on the quality of care delivered by PCPs is affected by a variety of moderators. We highlight three that affect the relationship: interruptions, physician expertise, and time.
Interruptions
Interruptions occur when something or someone in the environment unexpectedly disrupts the physician’s physical or mental workflow.40-42 This may shift the physician’s attention from the task he/she was originally engaged in, the primary task, to the interrupting task.43 Once this shift in attention occurs, memory of the primary task begins to decay in order to make room for the processes required to deal with the interrupting task.44 Many interruptions are unnecessary such as EHR systems that impede the flow of the physician-patient conversation.45 Interruptions may increase mental workload, reduce the ability to cope with information chaos and thereby potentially impair performance.
Expertise
Expertise is another variable that affects the relationship between information chaos and its consequences.46 It is probable that a PCP with more expertise can handle more information chaos, maintain better SA, and have less mental workload than a PCP with less expertise. With experience comes more accurate expectations about what is going on, where information resides, and what ambiguous or confusing information implies. Because of that, the same level of information chaos may have less impact on a more experienced clinician.
Time
Finally, the available time to cope with information chaos is important for patient safety. Physicians working for organizations that allow them to schedule patient visits with sufficient time to manage their patients’ problems will suffer fewer effects of information chaos; they can take the time to deal with the situation. On the other hand, when information chaos occurs in a time constrained environment (e.g. during a 15 minute visit for a patient with several chronic conditions and medications to manage), the impact on physician performance is exacerbated. Time is perhaps the most important resource available to deal with information chaos.
Reducing information chaos requires mechanisms to ensure that the necessary information is available and presented in a useable manner at the right time. Two general strategies have been proposed to calm the chaos. These include improved visit preparation and EHRs.
Visit Preparation
PCPs need to know the reason(s) for a patient visit. Clinics may have patients complete some type of “patient agenda” form, either electronic or paper, to be completed prior to the visit. Another approach is the teamlet47 or huddle,48 in which the nurse who rooms the patient meets briefly with the PCP to talk about the patient’s stated or written agenda. Utilizing more staff support to assist in obtaining and organizing information during the visit has been proposed as well.49 This is a good start, but at best lets the PCP know the agenda mere minutes before he/she enters the room and there may not be time to obtain needed information from external sources or review information in depth.
Electronic Health Records
EHRs contribute to more timely and available information but caveats exist, many of which have been discussed.11,45 EHRs are generally designed to facilitate data entry to conduct and document the process of care. As more and more data are available in an EHR, there is an even greater need for improved search methods and display techniques to present the data needed at the time of the patient visit. Ideal EHR design would allow relevant, needed information to be pushed to the clinician based on the reasons for the visit.
Our hypothesis is that appropriate practice redesign will reduce information chaos and provide the PCP with needed situational awareness during all phases of the visit, allowing him/her make better care decisions, and accomplish more (and perhaps with less fatigue) than would otherwise be the case. More research is needed to determine just what this practice redesign should be.
We propose that if progress is to be made on controlling information chaos and its effects on physician performance, the following questions, at minimum, need to be addressed:
  • Which aspects of information chaos are most common and which ones pose the greatest threats to patient safety?
  • How can we measure and quantify the elements of information chaos or resulting behaviors to determine the success of an intervention?
  • What interventions can reduce the information hazards and lead to the greatest improvements in SA, mental workload and ultimately patient safety?
  • What are valid ways to measure situation awareness in the healthcare settings? Our team had to develop new measures since none existed specific to primary care and measuring it during actual patient care delivery is challenging.
  • What is the nature of the effects of the moderators? Can clinician expertise or patient scheduling help to address information chaos?
  • When is the right time and what is the right information to provide to PCPs so that they can prepare appropriately for a visit and reduce information chaos?
  • What is the role of the clinic staff, patients and caregivers to prepare information for a PCP visit? When is the right time to collect the information and how should it be presented?
The questions above are examples of what needs to be done to help improve primary care. Technology cannot alone solve the problems of information chaos, nor can the transformation to a PCMH. New thinking is necessary. We cannot accept that information chaos is “just the way primary care is.” If we do this, we are not taking advantage of primary care redesign and the power of information systems to maximize the quality and efficiency of patient care delivery.
Acknowledgments
Support: This study was supported in part by grants 1P20HS017115 from the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (PI: Karsh), 1R18SH017899 from the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (PI: Karsh), and 1UL1RR025011 from the Clinical & Translational Science Award (CTSA) program of the National Center for Research Resources National Institutes of Health.
Abbreviations
EHRElectronic Health Record
HFEHuman Factors Engineering
PCMHPatient Centered Medical Home
PCPPrimary Care Physician
SASituational Awareness

Footnotes
Prior Presentations: Short talk at Wonca International Meeting, Cancun Mexico, May 20, 2010. 2 poster presentations.
Conflict of Interest: None
The published version of this article can be accessed on the Journal of the American Board of Family Medicine website at: http://jabfm.org/content/24/6/745.full
Human Subjects: Not applicable as no actual research on human subjects was conducted.
1. Donaldson M, Yordy K, Lohr K. Primary Care America’s Health in a New Era. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press; 1996.
2. Starfield B. Primary Care. J Ambulatory Care Manage. 1993;16(4):27–37. [PubMed]
3. Elder NC, Graham D, Brandt E, et al. The Testing Process in Family Medicine: Problems, Solutions and Barriers as Seen by Physicians and Their Staff: A Study of the American Academy of Family Physicians’ Research Network. Journal of Patient Safety. 2006;2(1):25–32.
4. Kuzel AJ, Woolf SH, Gilchrist VJ, et al. Patient reports of preventable problems and harms in primary health care. Ann Fam Med. 2004 Jul-Aug;2(4):333–340. [PubMed]
5. Phillips RL, Dovey SM, Graham D, Elder NC, Hickner J. Learning From Different Lenses: Reports of Medical Errors in Primary Care by Clinicians, Staff, and Patients: A Project of the American Academy of Family Physicians National Research Network. Journal of Patient Safety. 2006;2(1):140–146.
6. Kuo GM, Phillips RL, Graham D, Hickner JM. Medication errors reported by US family physicians and their office staff. Qual Saf Health Care. 2008 Aug;17(4):286–290. [PubMed]
7. Makeham MA, Stromer S, Bridges-Webb C, et al. Patient safety events reported in general practice: a taxonomy. Qual Saf Health Care. 2008 Feb;17(1):53–57. [PubMed]
8. Dovey SM, Meyers DS, Phillips RL, et al. A preliminary taxonomy of medical errors in family practice. Qual Saf Health Care. 2002;11(3):233–238. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
9. Nutting PA, Miller WL, Crabtree BF, Jaen CR, Stewart EE, Stange KC. Initial lessons from the first national demonstration project on practice transformation to a patient-centered medical home. Ann Fam Med. 2009;7(3):254–260. [PubMed]
10. Fernandopulle R, Patel N. How the electronic health record did not measure up to the demands of our medical home practice. Health Aff (Millwood) 2010 Apr;29(4):622–628. [PubMed]
11. Karsh BT, Weinger MB, Abbott PA, Wears RL. Health information technology: fallacies and sober realities. J Am Med Inform Assoc. 2010;17(6):617–623. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
12. US. 42 CFR Parts 412, 413, 422 Services DoHaH. Vol. 75. Federal Register; 2010. Medicare and Medicaid Programs; Electronic Health Record Incentive Program; Final Rule; pp. 44314–44518.
13. Schiff GD, Bates DW. Can electronic clinical documentation help prevent diagnostic errors? N Engl J Med. 2010;362(12):1066–1069. [PubMed]
14. Beasley JW, Hankey TH, Erickson R, et al. How many problems do family physicians manage at each encounter? a WReN study. Ann Fam Med. 2004;2:405–410. [PubMed]
15. Abbo ED, Zhang Q, Zelder M, Huang ES. The Increasing Number of Clinical Items Addressed During the Time of Adult Primary Care Visits. J Gen Intern Med. 2008;23(12):1058–1065. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
16. Bodenheimer T. Coordinating Care -- A Perilous Journey through the Health Care System. N Engl J Med. 2008;358(10):1064–1071. [PubMed]
17. Pham HH, O’Malley AS, Bach PB, Saiontz-Martinez C, Schrag D. Primary care physicians’ links to other physicians through Medicare patients: the scope of care coordination. Ann Intern Med. 2009 Feb 17;150(4):236–242. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
18. Smith PC, Rodrig AG, Bublitz C, et al. Missing clinical information during primary care visits. JAMA. 2005;293:565–571. [PubMed]
19. Institute of Medicine. Crossing the Quality Chasm: A New Health System for the 21st Century. Washington DC: National Academy Press; 2001. [PubMed]
20. Thielke S, Hammond K, Helbig S. Copying and pasting of examinations within the electronic health record. Int J Med Informatics. 2007;76S:S122–128. [PubMed]
21. Beasley JW, Hamilton-Escoto K, Karsh B. Human Factors in Primary Care. In: Carayon P, editor. Handbook of Human Factors and Ergonomics in Patient Safety. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2006. pp. 921–936.
22. Salvendy G, editor. Handbook of Human Factors and Ergonomics. 2. John Wiley and Sons; 1997.
23. Sanders MS, McCormick EJ. Human Factors in Engineering and Design. 7. New York: McGraw-Hill Science; 1993.
24. Wickens CD, Lee JD, Liu Y, Gordon-Becker S. An Introduction to Human Factors Engineering. 2. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education; 2004.
25. Endsley MR. Towards a theory of situation awareness in dynamic systems. Human Factors. 1995;37:32–64.
26. Hazlehurst B, McMullen CK, Gorman PN. Distributed cognition in the heart room: How situation awareness arises from coordinated communications during cardiac surgery. Journal of Biomedical Informatics. 2007 Oct;40(5):539–551. [PubMed]
27. Hogan MP, Pace DE, Hapgood J, Boone DC. Use of human patient simulation and the Situation Awareness Global Assessment Technique in practical trauma skills assessment. Journal of Trauma-Injury Infection and Critical Care. 2006 Nov;61(5):1047–1052. [PubMed]
28. Salmon PM, Stanton NA, Walker GH, Jenkins D, Baber C, McMaster R. Representing situation awareness in collaborative systems: A case study in the energy distribution domain. Ergonomics. 2008;51(3):367–384. [PubMed]
29. Smith K, Hancock PA. Situation Awareness Is Adaptive, Externally Directed Consciousness. Human Factors. 1995;37(1):137–148.
30. Stanton NA, Stewart R, Harris D, et al. Distributed situation awareness in dynamic systems: theoretical development and application of an ergonomics methodology. Ergonomics. 2006 Oct;49(12-13):1288–1311. [PubMed]
31. Hancock PA, Hoffman JE. National Research Council. Tactical display for soldiers: human factors considerations. Washington DC: National Academy Press; 1997. Stress and cognitive workload; pp. 129–162.
32. Hancock PA, Szalma JL. Operator stress & display design. Ergonomics in Design. 2003;11(2):13–18.
33. Hancock PA, Warm JS. A Dynamic-Model of Stress and Sustained Attention. Human Factors. 1989 Oct;31(5):519–537. [PubMed]
34. Endsley MR, Garland DJ, editors. Situation Awareness Analysis and Measurement. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2000.
35. Stanton NA, Chambers PRG, Piggott J. Situational awareness and safety. Safety Science. 2001;39(3):189–204.
36. Hancock PA, Desmond PA, editors. Stress, Workload and Fatigue. CRC Press; 2000.
37. Endsley MR, Jones DG. Disruptons, Interuruptions and Information Attack: Impact on Situation Awareness and Decision Making. Paper presented at: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting Proceedings; 2001.
38. Kahneman D, Tversky A, Slovic P. Judgement under Uncertainty: Heuristics & Biases. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1982.
39. Temte J, Grasmick M, O’Halloran P, et al. Mental workload in primary care. Paper presented at: AAFP National Research Network 8th Annual Convocation of Practices and Networks; March 7-9, 2008; Colorado Springs, CO. 2008.
40. Brixey JJ, Robinson DJ, Johnson CW, Johnson TR, Turley JP, Zhang J. A concept analysis of the phenomenon interruption. ANS Adv Nurs Sci. 2007;30(1):E26–42. [PubMed]
41. Chisholm CD, Dornfeld AM, Nelson DR, Cordell WH. Work interrupted: a comparison of workplace interruptions in emergency departments and primary care offices. Ann Emerg Med. 2001;38(2):146–151. [PubMed]
42. Paxton F, Heaney D, Howie J, Porter BA. A study of interruption rates for practice nurses and GPs. Nurs Stand. 1996 Jul;10(43):33–36. [PubMed]
43. Potter P, Wolf L, Boxerman S, et al. Understanding the cognitive work of nursing in the acute care environment. J Nurs Adm. 2005;35(7-8):327–335. [PubMed]
44. Altmann EM, Trafton JG. Task Interruption: Resumption Lag and the Role of Cues. Proceedings of the 26th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society. 2004:42–47.
45. Zheng K, Haftel HM, Hirschl RB, O’Reilly M, Hanauer DA. Quantifying the impact of health IT implementations on clinical workflow: a new methodological perspective. J Am Med Inform Assoc. 2010 Jul-Aug;17(4):454–461. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
46. Hancock PA, Weaver JL. On time distortion under stress. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomic Science. 2006;6(2):193–211.
47. Bodenheimer T, Laing BY. The teamlet model of primary care. Ann Fam Med. 2007;5(5):457–461. [PubMed]
48. Stewart EE, Johnson BC. Huddles: improve your office efficiency in mere minutes. Fam Pract Manag. 2007:27–29. [PubMed]
49. Sinsky CA. Improving office practice: working smarter, not harder. Fam Pract Manag. 2006 Nov-Dec;13(10):28–34. [PubMed]