PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptHHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
Am Nat. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 February 23.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC3285564
NIHMSID: NIHMS354841

Character displacement and the origins of diversity

Abstract

In The Origin of Species, Darwin proposed his ‘principle of divergence of character’ (a process now termed ‘character displacement’) to explain how new species arise and why they differ from one other phenotypically. Darwin maintained that the origin of species, and the evolution of differences between them, is ultimately caused by divergent selection acting to minimize competitive interactions between initially similar individuals, populations, and species. Here, we examine the empirical support for the various claims that constitute Darwin’s principle, specifically that: (1) competition promotes divergent trait evolution; (2) the strength of competitively mediated divergent selection increases with increasing phenotypic similarity between competitors; (3) divergence can occur within species; and (4) competitively mediated divergence can trigger speciation. We also explore aspects that Darwin failed to consider. In particular, we describe how: (1) divergence can arise from selection acting to lessen reproductive interactions; (2) divergence is fueled by the intersection of character displacement and sexual selection; and (3) phenotypic plasticity may play a key role in promoting character displacement. Generally, character displacement is well supported empirically, and it remains a vital explanation for how new species arise and diversify.

Keywords: Competition, Darwin’s divergence of character, hybridization, phenotypic plasticity, sexual selection, speciation

Introduction

“Divergence of character … is of high importance on my theory, and explains, as I believe, several important facts.” (Darwin 1859 (2009), P. 111)

“Natural selection, also, leads to divergence of character; for more living beings can be supported on the same area the more they diverge in structure, habits, and constitution, of which we see proof by looking at the inhabitants of any small spot or at naturalised productions. Therefore during the modification of the descendants of any one species, and during the incessant struggle of all species to increase in numbers, the more diversified these descendants become, the better will be their chance of succeeding in the battle of life. Thus the small differences distinguishing varieties of the same species, will steadily tend to increase till they come to equal the greater differences between species of the same genus, or even of distinct genera.” (Darwin 1859 (2009), Pp. 127–128)

With these words, Darwin first proposed that competition acts as a ubiquitous and powerful agent of divergent selection. Darwin’s ideas were groundbreaking, for none of his predecessors had viewed interactions among organisms as being significant in evolution (Ridley 2005). The crux of Darwin’s idea is that when organisms compete for scarce resources, natural selection should favor those individuals that are least like their competitors. Consequently, individuals, populations, and species that compete should become more dissimilar over time.

Darwin considered this process, which he dubbed “divergence of character,” to be “of high importance” for two reasons. First, it could explain the origin of species. According to Darwin, selection acting to minimize competition between “varieties” could drive divergence between them until they became separate species. Second, divergence of character could explain evolution’s distinctive ‘tree-like’ typology (reviewed in Ridley 2005). In particular, Darwin suggested that the divergent nature of evolution reflects the tendency for the strength of competition to increase with increasing taxonomic (and, hence, phenotypic) similarity between competitors. Darwin maintained that, “it is the most closely-allied forms, —varieties of the same species, and species of the same genus or related genera, —which, from having nearly the same structure, constitution and habits, generally come into the severest competition with each other. Consequently, each new variety or species, during the progress of its formation, will generally press hardest on its nearest kindred, and tend to exterminate them” (Darwin 1859 (2009), P. 110). Thus, by continually eliminating intermediate forms, competitively mediated selection has caused the history of life to resemble a tree, with numerous, diverging branches.

The concept of divergence of character was clearly important to Darwin’s thinking on the origin and diversity of species (reviewed in Mayr 1992; Tammone 1995; Ridley 2005; Costa 2009; Reznick 2010). Indeed, he devotes as much space in The Origin to discussing it as he does to discussing natural selection. Yet, until relatively recently, evolutionary biologists questioned the role of competitive interactions in evolution (reviewed in Schluter 2000).

Here, we examine the empirical support for Darwin’s ideas. We also discuss several key issues that Darwin failed to appreciate, which represent the focus of ongoing research. Specifically, we begin by describing how selection acting to lessen competitive interactions among organisms can lead to divergent trait evolution—a process now known as ‘character displacement’ (Brown and Wilson 1956; Grant 1972; Schluter 2000, 2002). We also describe how this process of character displacement can trigger speciation. As we will see in the next section, Darwin’s intuition was essentially correct: selection acting to minimize costly interactions between organisms can indeed promote divergent evolution and speciation. Yet, as we describe in the subsequent section, Darwin failed to appreciate the importance of divergence in reproductive characters, the effects of character displacement on sexual selection, and the importance of understanding the proximate mechanisms by which character displacement occurs. Nevertheless, Darwin’s insights into the role of competition in divergence were well founded. Indeed, although character displacement was initially defined as the process of phenotypic divergence caused by interspecific resource competition (Brown and Wilson 1956), we now know that (as Darwin maintained and as we describe in greater detail below) an analogous process can arise from intraspecific competition. In short, character displacement, acting both between and within species, is well supported empirically, and it remains a compelling explanation for how new species arise and why they tend to be so different from one other phenotypically.

Before proceeding, it is important to note that there is some disagreement over what Darwin actually meant by divergence of character. Among those who equated divergence of character with character displacement (as we have) was Mayr (1963, 1970, 1992), who stated that, “The basic point of the principle of divergence is simplicity itself: the more the coinhabitants of an area differ from each other in their ecological requirements, the less they will compete with each other; therefore natural selection will tend to favor any variation toward greater divergence” (Mayr 1992, p. 344). By contrast, William Brown (who, with E. O. Wilson, coined the term character displacement [see Brown and Wilson 1956]) took issue with Mayr’s interpretation and steadfastly maintained that the two terms were not synonymous, contending that, “Darwin was describing what we call today adaptive radiation” (Brown 1964, p. 51). More recently, Tammone (1995) and Costa (2009) asserted that divergence of character does not describe a process in which divergence arises in sympatry. Instead, they argue that it describes the coming together of already divergent species that had previously diverged in allopatry and were therefore able to coexist. Note, however, that this interpretation involves the sorting of species through differential extinction, which is not an evolutionary process (Schluter 2000).

While there is some ambiguity regarding what Darwin intended by divergence of character, Darwin’s claims (from the quotes above) that, “more living things can be supported on the same area the more they diverge” and that “each new variety or species … will generally press hardest on its nearest kindred” (italics added) suggest that he envisioned a process in which selection favored divergence between sympatric individuals that overlapped in ecological requirements. In other words, he seemed to be describing the process of character displacement (for further discussion, see Mayr 1970, 1992; Schluter 2001; Ridley 2005; Schemske in press).

What Darwin got right

In this section, we focus on the empirical support for Darwin’s claims that: (1) competition promotes divergent trait evolution; (2) the strength of competitively mediated divergent selection increases with increasing phenotypic similarity between competitors; (3) divergence can occur within species; and (4) competitively mediated divergence can trigger speciation. In our review, we emphasize studies of natural populations, especially those that combine observations with experiments. Generally, the most powerful method for demonstrating character displacement is to blend these two approaches (Schluter 2001, 2002).

Competition promotes divergent trait evolution

Darwin’s central claim was that competition promotes divergent trait evolution. Yet, he failed to provide any actual examples from natural populations. As it turns out, for Darwin to obtain such evidence would have been no trivial exercise; competitively mediated divergence is often difficult to detect. On the one hand, if two species are phenotypically similar enough to compete, they probably have not undergone much divergence. On the other hand, if two species have already undergone competitively mediated divergence, they are probably no longer similar enough to experience much competition with each other.

Lack (1947) was the first to resolve this conundrum. He introduced the method of comparing conspecific populations that are in sympatry with a heterospecific competitor versus those in allopatry. The logic behind this approach is as follows. Selection to lessen interspecific competition will only act in areas where species actually co-occur. Thus, if competition promotes divergence, species pairs should be more dissimilar where they occur together than where each occurs alone. In developing these ideas, Lack drew on detailed studies of Darwin’s finches from the Galápagos archipelago. He described several cases in which different finch species differed more in beak size where they were sympatric than where they were allopatric (Lack 1947).

Shortly after the appearance of Lack’s seminal study, Brown and Wilson (1956) presented several additional cases in which species pairs were recognizably different in sympatry but not in allopatry. Their analysis therefore suggested that selection acting to lessen competitive interactions among organisms may often promote divergent trait evolution, a process they termed ‘character displacement’ (Brown and Wilson 1956; see also Grant 1972; Schluter 2000, 2001, 2002). Following publication of this paper, researchers documented numerous instances of exaggerated divergence between sympatric species in diverse taxa (reviewed in Schluter 2000; Dayan and Simberloff 2005).

Despite mounting evidence of exaggerated divergence between sympatric species, some researchers remained skeptical of competition’s role in generating such divergence (reviewed in Schluter 2001). The reason for such skepticism is that species may differ for evolutionary reasons other than selection, and for selective reasons other than to avoid competition (Grant 1972; Arthur 1982; Losos 1992; Schluter and McPhail 1992; Taper and Case 1992; Marko 2005). Thus, a major challenge for researchers in the field of character displacement has been to rule out other causes of species divergence that could produce the same patterns as character displacement. One approach for doing so is to establish rigorous criteria, which, when met, would make a compelling case for character displacement (Grant 1972; Arthur 1982; Schluter and McPhail 1992; Taper and Case 1992; Losos 2000). A number of studies have applied such criteria to make a strong case for character displacement (e.g., see Schluter and McPhail 1992; Adams and Rohlf 2000; Caruso 2000). Moreover, documenting parallel evolution in resource acquisition traits—specifically establishing that a divergent trait has evolved repeatedly in closely related, independently evolving populations—implicates competitively mediated selection as the cause of trait divergence between species (e.g., see Schluter and McPhail 1992; Hansen et al. 2000; Matocq and Murphy 2007; Rice et al. 2009; Adams 2010).

A second approach for making a compelling case for character displacement is to utilize experiments. The ideal experimental demonstration of character displacement would be to manipulate the presence of a competitor and show that characters associated with resource use evolve in a focal species. However, such experiments are generally not attempted except in lab populations with short generation times (see Tyerman et al. 2008). Nevertheless, ‘natural experiments’ suggest that character displacement can be observed directly in the wild. For example, in long-term field studies of Darwin’s finches, Grant and Grant (2006) established that the medium ground finch, Geospiza fortis, diverged in beak size from the large ground finch, G. magnirostris, after the latter species invaded the former species’ island and became numerous enough to deplete the food supply (fig. 1).

Figure 1
Direct evidence of character displacement in medium ground finches, Geospiza fortis. Shown is the mean beak size (± 95% confidence intervals) for a population of G. fortis on an undisturbed Galápagos island. In 1982, the large ground finch, ...

Many species are also amenable to experiments aimed at testing key predictions of Darwin’s hypothesis. Two classes of such experiments have been used. One class observes the performance of a focal species after manipulating the presence (or relative abundance) of a heterospecific competitor (Schluter 1994; Pfennig et al. 2007). A second class of experiments uses species that respond to competitors through phenotypic plasticity (Pfennig and Murphy 2000, 2002). With such species, a causal link can be established between the presence of competitors and character change if, in the presence of a competitor, an individual facultatively expresses an alternative resource-use phenotype distinct from that of its competitor (fig. 2). The case for character displacement is especially persuasive if these experimentally demonstrated phenotypic shifts mirror divergence in these same traits observed between natural populations in sympatry versus allopatry with a competitor (e.g., see fig. 2).

Figure 2
Experimental evidence of character displacement in Mexican spadefoot toad tadpoles, Spea multiplicata. Spea tadpoles occur as (a) an omnivore ecomorph, which feeds on detritus on the pond bottom, and (b) a morphologically distinctive carnivore ecomorph, ...

In sum, although resource competition is not the only agent of divergent selection, the available data suggest that resource competition can indeed promote divergent trait evolution as Darwin initially proposed.

The strength of competitively mediated divergent selection increases with increasing phenotypic similarity between competitors

Darwin did not envision competitively mediated divergence as transpiring with equal likelihood or degree between all organisms. Instead, he argued that, “it is the most closely-allied forms, —varieties of the same species, and species of the same genus or related genera, —which, from having nearly the same structure, constitution and habits, generally come into the severest competition with each other” (Darwin 1859 (2009), P. 110). Thus, according to Darwin, competition is the most severe between those individuals that are the most closely related to each other, because such individuals should be most similar in resource use and in associated traits. This claim was central to Darwin’s arguments for explaining, among other things, why evolution, on the grand scale, has a tree-like structure (see Introduction). This claim is also vital for ecology, in that it forms the basis for the competitive exclusion principle (Hardin 1960), which states that species with similar resource requirements cannot stably coexist. The competitive exclusion principle was elegantly demonstrated by Gause’s (1934) pioneering experiments with Paramecium. But is there any evidence to suggest, as Darwin maintained, that the strength of competitively mediated divergent selection increases the more similar two species are to each other in resource use?

Experiments have shown that natural selection disfavors individuals during character displacement that express resource-use phenotypes most closely resembling those of their heterospecific competitor (e.g., see fig. 3; see also Pacala and Roughgarden 1985; Pritchard and Schluter 2001; Gray and Robinson 2002; Schluter 2003). Thus, as predicted by Darwin, the strength of divergent selection on resource-use traits increases the more similar two species are to each other in resource use.

Figure 3
Experimental evidence that divergent natural selection disfavors those individuals that are the most similar to their competitor in resource use. (a) Mexican spadefoot toad tadpoles, S. multiplicata compete with a heterospecific, S. bombifrons, for anostracan ...

A number of researchers have also evaluated Darwin’s claim by using phylogenetic distance as a proxy for similarity in resource use. The underlying assumption behind this alternative approach is that there should be a positive relationship between the phylogenetic distance of any two species and their overall ecological similarity (e.g., see Webb et al. 2002; Losos 2008). Although phylogenetic distance does not always predict ecological similarity (Losos 2008), phylogenetically close taxa might be more likely to compete if the fundamental niche is conserved evolutionarily (Maherali and Klironomos 2007). Indeed, there is evidence that communities are more species rich when they consist of phylogenetically distinct taxa that are less likely to overlap in resource use, and, hence, compete with each other (Maherali and Klironomos 2007). Moreover, communities may often consist of species that are more phylogenetically dissimilar than would be expected by chance (e.g., Cavender-Bares et al. 2004; but see Webb 2000; note, however, that we would not expect communities to be phylogenetically overdispersed if competing species within these communities undergo character displacement rather than competitive exclusion).

Thus, ample empirical evidence exists to support Darwin’s claim that the intensity of divergent selection increases the more similar two species are to each other ecologically, phenotypically, and (possibly) even phylogenetically. However, our discussion has focused so far on how the strength of competitively mediated divergent selection increases with increasing ecological similarity between heterospecific competitors. Yet, the most ecologically similar competitors that most organisms will confront are likely to be conspecifics, and Darwin clearly thought that competitively mediated divergence could also occur within species (see quote above). Indeed, compared to interspecific competition, intraspecific competition is probably more common and often stronger (Gurevitch et al. 1992). In particular, intraspecific competition is probably more common, because individuals likely encounter conspecifics more frequently than heterospecifics. Moreover, intraspecific competition is probably often stronger than interspecific competition, because conspecifics are typically more similar in resource-use requirements than are heterospecifics, thereby making interactants more evenly matched and competition more intense (see above). Given that intraspecific competition is common and frequently strong, can selection lessen such competition by promoting divergence within species through ‘intraspecific character displacement’ (sensu West-Eberhard 2003; see also Dayan and Simberloff 2005)?

Divergence can occur within species

Darwin maintained that competition promotes divergence within species. As we noted in the Introduction, selection acting to reduce such intraspecific competition can promote divergence within species through a process that is the intraspecific analog to interspecific character displacement (Dayan and Simberloff 2005; Pfennig and Pfennig 2009). Here, we describe how intraspecific character displacement can and does arise.

A common manifestation of intraspecific character displacement is niche width expansion (Huxley 1942 (2010); Van Valen 1965; MacArthur and Wilson 1967; Roughgarden 1972). An increase in niche width is frequently observed in populations on isolated archipelagos or similar island-like settings (e.g., lakes), where individuals encounter few heterospecific competitors (MacArthur and Wilson 1967). In such situations, populations are free to respond adaptively to intraspecific competition by expanding their niche width to take advantage of underexploited resources (a process termed ‘ecological release’ or ‘character release’; Wilson 1961; Grant 1972; for empirical examples, see Werner and Sherry 1987; Robinson and Wilson 1994; Simberloff et al. 2000).

Roughgarden (1972) described how this process could arise. Consider a hypothetical population that exploits a continuously varying resource gradient, such as a gradient of prey size. If all resource types along the gradient (e.g., all prey size classes) are not utilized to the same extent, then those individuals that specialize on underutilized portions of the resource-use gradient (e.g., size classes that are underutilized) should experience less intense intraspecific competition (Roughgarden 1972). Essentially, individuals experiencing intraspecific competition are expected to equalize the level of competition by spreading themselves out more or less evenly along the resource-use gradient (MacArthur 1972), assuming that such ecological opportunity is available to them (Simpson 1953). This process can drive a population onto a novel resource for which competition is less severe (Bolnick 2001) and thereby possibly promote the evolution of novel resource-use phenotypes (e.g., see Carrol et al. 1998; Jones 1998; Aubret et al. 2006; Herrel et al. 2008). By favoring novel resource-use phenotypes, intraspecific character displacement might even trigger adaptive radiation (Simpson 1953; Schluter 2000).

Another common manifestation of intraspecific character displacement—and one that can have important ramifications—is the evolution of discrete phenotypes within populations (i.e., polymorphism; Dieckmann and Doebeli 1999; Doebeli and Dieckmann 2003). Intraspecific competition has long been viewed as a key agent of disruptive selection (Rosenzweig 1978; Wilson and Turelli 1986; Day and Young 2004; Rueffler et al. 2006), which arises when extreme phenotypes have a fitness advantage over more intermediate phenotypes (Mather 1953). Dieckmann and Doebli (1999) and Doebli and Dieckmann (2003) described how this process could unfold. Consider a population that exploits a continuously varying resource gradient, such as a gradient of prey sizes, where prey of intermediate size are most common. Individuals that specialize on the intermediate resource type (e.g., prey of intermediate size) should start out with a fitness advantage, because the intermediate resource type is most frequently encountered. Over time, however, the intermediate resource type should become increasingly depleted, and individuals specializing on this resource should cease to be favored. Instead, individuals with extreme resource-use traits that specialize on less common resources on either end of the resource gradient (e.g., very small or very large prey) will have a fitness advantage (Dieckmann and Doebeli 1999; Doebeli and Dieckmann 2003). This process is ultimately driven by negative frequency-dependent selection, in which rare resource-use phenotypes have a fitness advantage because of decreased competition with more common phenotypes (Day and Young 2004; Rueffler et al. 2006). If such selection persists, it could promote the evolution of a resource polymorphism—the occurrence within a single population of discrete intraspecific morphs showing differential resource use (Smith and Skúlason 1996; see table 1).

Table 1
Resource polymorphisms in selected taxa showing the nature of the ecological differences between alternative resource-use ecomorphs. In cases marked with an asterisk, natural populations that differ in morph expression are (potentially) partially reproductively ...

Historically, disruptive selection was thought to be rare (Endler 1986). Yet, disruptive selection may be more widespread in nature than was formerly presumed (Kingsolver et al. 2001). Indeed, recent field research has uncovered evidence of both disruptive selection acting on resource-acquisition traits (Smith 1993; Medel et al. 2003; Bolnick 2004; Pfennig et al. 2007; Bolnick and Lau 2008; Calsbeek and Smith 2008; Hendry et al. 2009; Martin and Pfennig 2009) and frequency-dependent intraspecific competition for resources, such that individuals compete most against conspecifics bearing similar resource-use phenotypes (Pfennig 1992; Hori 1993; Benkman 1996; Maret and Collins 1997; Martin and Pfennig 2009). Thus, intraspecific character displacement—trait evolution stemming from selection to lessen intraspecific competition—might often promote divergence within species, as Darwin suggested.

Competitively mediated divergence can trigger speciation

Darwin (1859) first suggested that speciation could arise when individuals in a population began to diverge from one another as a result of selection to lessen competitive interactions (see quote in the Introduction). Like the previous claim, Darwin’s contention that competitively mediated divergence could lead to speciation initially met with opposition. In particular, Darwin’s emphasis on ecological differentiation within contiguous populations as a cause of speciation was first challenged by Moritz Wagner, George Romanes, John Gulick, and others who maintained that divergent evolution cannot occur without some sort of geographical isolation (Tammone 1995). Later, Mayr (1963) argued that the non-allopatric speciation implied by Darwin’s model is implausible, because gene flow would preclude divergence. In fact, as a mechanism for explaining the origin of species, Mayr (1992, p. 358) concluded that, “it is now evident that the principle of divergence is invalid.” However, while it is true that Darwin erred in, “not discriminating between intrapopulation variants and geographic subspecies, calling both of them varieties” and in “his failure to distinguish between isolation in an ecological niche and in a geographically isolated area” (Mayr 1992, p. 358), Darwin did highlight how competitive interactions could promote the formation of species. Furthermore, recent theory and data demonstrate that substantial divergence, and even speciation, can occur in the face of gene flow (Kirkpatrick and Ravigne 2002; Bush and Butlin 2004; Dieckmann et al. 2004; Bolnick and Fitzpatrick 2007; Nosil 2008; Sobel et al. 2010). Indeed, the notion that competitively mediated divergence can lead to speciation—even within a continuous population—is now seen as plausible (Dieckmann and Doebeli 1999; Polechová and Barton 2005).

Here, we review three evolutionary routes by which character displacement might promote speciation. The first two routes are noncontroversial. In these two routes, speciation either proceeds following secondary contact between previously divergent populations (the first route) or when populations that are (potentially) parapatric diverge (the second route). Only the third route involves the controversial notion that ecological differentiation within a contiguous population can result in speciation.

First, competitively mediated divergence might finalize the process of speciation. This scenario entails the classical allopatric model of speciation (Mayr 1963; Coyne and Orr 2004). Speciation begins when populations in allopatry start to diverge from one another (e.g., due to divergent selection or genetic drift). When such populations come into secondary contact, selection to minimize competition can accentuate divergence between them (Coyne and Orr 2004; Grant and Grant 2008; Price 2008). Moreover, if these incipient species interbreed and produce hybrids of low fitness, then reinforcement can finalize speciation by promoting the evolution of complete reproductive isolation (Dobzhansky 1940; Howard 1993; Servedio and Noor 2003; Coyne and Orr 2004). For possible examples, see reviews by Coyne and Orr 2004, Grant and Grant (2008), and Schluter (2009).

Second, competitively mediated divergence might initiate the evolution of reproductive isolation between (potentially parapatric) populations that differ in interactions with heterospecifics (Pfennig and Pfennig 2009; Rice and Pfennig 2010). Because individuals in sympatry with a heterospecific competitor will experience a different selective environment than conspecifics in allopatry, populations in these two types of environments should also diverge. Such divergence may indirectly promote speciation through the evolution of either post-mating or pre-mating barriers to gene flow between sympatric and allopatric populations. Post-mating barriers may arise when offspring created by matings between sympatric and allopatric parents express an intermediate phenotype that is not well-adapted for either parental environment (Rice and Hostert 1993; Hatfield and Schluter 1996, 1999; Pfennig and Rice 2007; Svedin et al. 2008). Such divergence would be further exaggerated if populations evolving independently of each other in divergent competitive environments accumulated alleles that were incompatible with genomes from the alternative environment (Coyne and Orr 2004). Moreover, as we describe in more detail below, pre-mating barriers may arise if shifts in habitat or resource use preclude mating between individuals from alternative competitive environments (reviewed in Rundle and Schluter 2004).

Third, competitively mediated divergence might promote sympatric speciation by favoring the evolution of resource polymorphism (table 1). As noted above, this route is more controversial than the first two routes, because divergence occurs among potentially interbreeding individuals (i.e., within sympatric populations). However, the evolution of a resource polymorphism has long been viewed as a critical early stage of the speciation process (Huxley 1942 (2010); Maynard Smith 1966; Felsenstein 1981; West-Eberhard 1989; Meyer 1993; Wimberger 1994; Smith and Skúlason 1996; Skúlason et al. 1999; West-Eberhard 2003; Stauffer and Gray 2004; West-Eberhard 2005; Mallet 2008; Elmer et al. 2010). This view is based partly on the observation that the phenotypic differences between alternative morphs are often comparable to those normally seen between species within the same clade (e.g., see Liem and Kaufman 1984; Hendry et al. 2006; Calsbeek et al. 2007). Moreover, populations that differ in expression of resource polymorphism typically possess some of the same characteristics as species, including ecological and genetic differences and even partial reproductive isolation (see table 1), suggesting the presence of incipient species (West-Eberhard 2005; Mallet 2008; Hendry 2009).

The evolution of resource polymorphism might be particularly effective at facilitating speciation, because the same conditions that promote resource polymorphism simultaneously foster speciation’s three components—genetic isolation, divergence, and reproductive isolation (Pfennig and McGee 2010). Genetic isolation can arise between morphs, because alternative resource-use morphs typically differ in the locations and times that they seek their separate resources (e.g., see Robinson and Wilson 1994; Wimberger 1994; Skúlason et al. 1999; Robinson and Parsons 2002; Nosil 2007) and thereby, potentially, where and when they seek mates [especially in phytophagous insects, where different host races (a form of resource polymorphism) mate on their host (Berlocher and Feder 2002; Bush and Butlin 2004)].

Such isolation between ecomorphs can thereby enable natural selection to further enhance existing differences between the alternative morphs (and also between populations that differ in the expression of such morphs). In particular, natural selection will generally favor morph-specific traits that improve a morph’s ability to exploit its particular niche (e.g., see Schluter 1993; Nosil 2007; Sobel et al. 2010). Moreover, as we describe in more detail later, even small-scale ecological separation can influence patterns of sexual selection, which could further enhance divergence between morphs (Boughman 2001).

Once alternative morphs begin to accumulate these ecological and genetic differences, matings between them should produce offspring with low fitness (Hatfield and Schluter 1999). Consequently, selection will favor the evolution of further reproductive isolation between ecomorphs (Nosil et al. 2002; Rundle and Schluter 2004). Moreover, matings between populations that differ in the expression of these phenotypic alternatives should also produce offspring of low fitness (Pfennig and Rice 2007). Ultimately, selection should favor the evolution of reproductive isolation between ecomorphs and also between populations expressing different frequencies of ecomorphs (Rice and Pfennig 2010), thereby completing speciation.

As evidence of resource polymorphism’s possible role in speciation, clades in which resource polymorphism has evolved are more species rich than their sister clades that lack resource polymorphism (Pfennig and McGee 2010). More direct tests are needed, however. Organisms with short generation times that shift hosts and mate on their host, such as certain microbes (Duffy et al. 2007) and phytophagous insects (Berlocher and Feder 2002; Bush and Butlin 2004), might prove especially useful for evaluating competition’s role in sympatric divergence and speciation.

In sum, competitively mediated divergence might play a critical role in promoting speciation, including between groups of organisms in a contiguous population.

What Darwin failed to appreciate

Having reviewed what Darwin got right regarding competition’s role in diversification, we now highlight those aspects of character displacement that he failed to appreciate. Specifically, we describe how: (1) divergence can arise from selection acting to lessen reproductive interactions; (2) divergence is fueled by the intersection of character displacement and sexual selection; and (3) phenotypic plasticity might play a key role in promoting character displacement.

Divergence can arise from selection acting to lessen reproductive interactions

Darwin’s principle of divergence of character was predicated on the notion that selection to lessen competition for resources is the primary driver of diversification. By contrast, he failed to appreciate that selection could act similarly to lessen reproductive interactions (fig. 4), and that such selection could generate divergence rivaling that generated by resource competition (Lack 1945; Brown and Wilson 1956).

Figure 4
Evidence of reproductive character displacement, as revealed by exaggerated divergence in sympatry between two species of stag beetles from Southeast Asia (genus Odontolabis). In allopatry, O. mouhoti and O. cuvera are similar in body size, genitalia ...

When sharing the environment for reproduction, heterospecifics can interact directly or indirectly (reviewed in Pfennig and Pfennig 2009). During direct interactions, individuals actually risk mating with heterospecifics. During indirect interactions, heterospecifics compete for access to the locations, signaling space, or means that allow for mate localization and attraction (Butlin and Ritchie 1994). Both types of interactions can exert strong selection on the evolution of the timing and nature of reproduction so as to minimize costly reproductive interactions with heterospecifics. Ultimately, this selection can cause species to diverge from one another in reproductive traits (a process known as ‘reproductive character displacement’; Brown and Wilson 1956; Crozier 1974; reviewed in Howard 1993; Andersson 1994; Butlin and Ritchie 1994; Gerhardt and Huber 2002; see also Groning and Hochkirch 2008; Pfennig and Pfennig 2009).

Whether via direct or indirect interactions, selection to avoid reproductive interactions with heterospecifics results in ‘reproductive partitioning’ of both the environment and trait space. In terms of partitioning of the environment, different species may use different locations or times for their reproductive activities (e.g., Ptacek 1992). In terms of trait space, males may adopt sexual signals that are more distinct from heterospecifics, whereas females may adopt mating preferences or evolve sensory filters that enhance the likelihood of mating with conspecifics (reviewed in Howard 1993; Andersson 1994; Butlin and Ritchie 1994; Gerhardt and Huber 2002). Moreover, females may evolve reproductive strategies that prevent hybrid zygote production, such as conspecific sperm precedence or reduced investment in, or production of, offspring resulting from heterospecific matings (reviewed in Howard 1999). In the absence of such reproductive partitioning, species may either coalesce via hybridization (e.g., Seehausen et al. 1997; Behm et al. 2010) or one may go extinct via reproductive exclusion (Hochkirch et al. 2007; Groning and Hochkirch 2008).

Divergence in reproductive traits is not divorced from divergence in ecological traits (Rice and Hostert 1993; Ryan 1998; Boughman 2002; Podos and Nowicki 2004; Grant and Grant 2008; Price 2008; Pfennig and Pfennig 2009; Sobel et al. 2010). Indeed, divergence in ecological traits is unlikely to occur without also potentially altering both male and female traits involved in reproduction (e.g., Boughman 2001; Podos 2001). For example, selection to minimize competition for resources among different species of Darwin’s finches has led to divergence in resource use, and, consequently, changes in beak size and morphology (e.g., see fig. 1). These changes in beak size and morphology are associated with a concomitant shift in the male’s song, which is directly involved in species recognition (reviewed in Podos and Nowicki 2004; Grant and Grant 2008; Price 2008). Indeed, within the finch Geospiza fortis, two morphs have evolved that are specialized for using alternative seed sizes (Hendry et al. 2006; Hendry et al. 2009). Their songs appear to have diverged in concert with changes in beak size, and this shift has resulted in assortative mating based on song type, and therefore, beak size (Huber and Podos 2006; Huber et al. 2007).

Divergence in ecological traits might also alter female mate preferences (reviewed in Ryan 1998; Boughman 2002; Pfennig and Pfennig 2009; Sobel et al. 2010). Consequently, such divergence in female mate preferences might minimize reproductive interactions between divergent populations and thereby initiate the evolution of reproductive isolation (e.g., Boughman 2001; reviewed in Boughman 2002; Pfennig and Pfennig 2009). For example, certain stickleback populations have diverged into two, sympatric ecomorphs in response to selection to lessen resource competition: benthics, which forage in the littoral zone, and limnetics, which forage in open water (reviewed in Rundle and Schluter 2004). These two ecomorphs have diverged not only in resource use, but also in female mate preferences. In particular, whereas red coloration is more difficult to detect in the littoral zone, it is more easily discerned in open water (Boughman 2001). Consequently, benthic females are less sensitive to variation in red than are limnetic females, and, unlike limnetic females, benthic females do not tend to prefer redder males (Boughman 2001, 2007). Moreover, males are redder in populations where females can detect and prefer redder males (Boughman 2001, 2007). Perhaps more critically, the two ecomorphs within any given lake are, at least partly, reproductively isolated from each other (reviewed in Rundle and Schluter 2004), and the extent to which they are isolated is negatively correlated with female red sensitivity and preference in a given population. Thus, shifts in mate preference—tied to divergent selective environments—dictates the degree to which reproductive divergence has occurred (Boughman 2001).

Is such divergence in reproductive traits a by-product of shifts in habitat associated with resource use, or have reproductive traits become targets of selection to minimize reproductive interactions between ecomorphs or species? In sticklebacks, divergence in reproductive characters does not appear to be an artifact: in a study that specifically controlled for the effects of divergence in resource use, sympatric limnetics and benthics displayed enhanced reproductive isolation relative to allopatric pairs (Rundle and Schluter 1998, 2004). Presumably, ecological divergence generated differences in traits used during mating that then became further elaborated to avoid the costs of mating with heterospecifics (Rundle and Schluter 1998, 2004; Boughman et al. 2005).

Although recent studies have focused on how divergence in ecological traits can affect divergence in reproductive traits, the reverse could also hold. In other words, divergence in reproductive traits could drive divergence in ecological characters (Konuma and Chiba 2007). If, for example, species segregate in space or time to avoid reproductive interactions, they may be concomitantly exposed to novel, underutilized resources. If utilization of these resources is selectively favored to minimize resource competition between species, then divergence in reproductive traits can instigate divergence in resource use (Konuma and Chiba 2007).

That both ecological and reproductive characters can diverge in concert has two important outcomes for diversity. First, divergence of resource-use traits in tandem with reproductive traits potentially stabilizes the evolution of divergent ecological traits (i.e., it explains how alternative morphs can persist). In the absence of reproductive divergence, different species or different ecomorphs might interbreed and any ecological specialization that each species/ecomorph has started to evolve might break down as a consequence of genetic mixing between the two divergent groups (e.g., Seehausen et al. 1997; Behm et al. 2010). If, however, reproductive characters diverge in concert with ecological traits, interbreeding is less likely to occur and specialized forms can be maintained and elaborated as we described above (e.g., Huber and Podos 2006; Huber et al. 2007).

A second significant outcome of the divergence of reproductive characters in conjunction with ecological specialization is enhanced reproductive isolation between existing species, or alternatively, speciation between alternative ecomorphs (Rice and Hostert 1993; Boughman 2002; Coyne and Orr 2004; Podos and Nowicki 2004; Rundle and Nosil 2005; Grant and Grant 2008; Price 2008; Pfennig and Pfennig 2009; Sobel et al. 2010). In particular, because differential reproductive behaviors are linked to alternative resource-use types, the likelihood of assortative mating within type is higher than it would be if these different types of traits were unlinked. Such assortative mating enhances the evolution of reproductive isolation and potentially increases the likelihood of speciation relative to situations where the traits are disassociated (Rice and Hostert 1993; Bolnick and Fitzpatrick 2007).

Moreover, as each species (or ecomorph) becomes increasingly specialized, the detrimental fitness consequences of hybridization can become exaggerated, because hybrids: (1) will not succeed in either parent’s niche; (2) cannot attract or identify mates; and/or (3) might suffer from genetic incompatibilities that arise between the alternate genomes (see above). Consequently, this selection against hybrids favors further reproductive divergence, which may ultimately contribute to reproductive isolation—and complete speciation—between competing species or ecomorphs (Coyne and Orr 2004; Rundle and Nosil 2005; Grant and Grant 2008; Price 2008; Ortiz-Barrientos et al. 2009; Pfennig and Pfennig 2009; Schluter 2009; Sobel et al. 2010). Reproductive character displacement thereby serves as a critical link between divergence in resource use and the origin of species.

Divergence is fueled by the intersection of character displacement and sexual selection

Darwin’s lack of focus on reproductive interactions driving divergence is ironic, because it was he who first proposed sexual selection as a mechanism for the adaptive evolution of sexual traits (Darwin 1859 (2009)). Darwin mentions sexual selection in The Origin as arising from interactions within species, specifically competition among males for access to females. Yet, he regarded sexual selection as “less rigid in its action than ordinary selection” (Darwin 1859 (2009), P. 157). Perhaps even more importantly, he failed to describe why species differ in sexually selected traits. Indeed, we still do not fully understand why species and populations differ in patterns of sexual selection. Variation in patterns of sexual selection could arise simply by chance (Lande 1981; Kirkpatrick 1982). Alternatively, differential experience with abiotic factors (e.g., the physical conditions through which signals are transmitted) and biotic factors (e.g., predators) could alter the nature and targets of selection on reproductive traits (reviewed in Endler and Basolo 1998; Pfennig 1998; Ryan 1998; Ptacek 2000; Boughman 2002; Price 2008). Here, we focus on how resource competition or reproductive interactions can influence the nature of sexual selection and thereby generate adaptive variation in patterns of sexual selection that possibly even promotes speciation.

As indicated above, character displacement in both ecological and reproductive traits will tend to alter sexual signals, mate preferences, and the habitat in which they are expressed. Such changes will not only minimize reproductive interactions and resource competition between species, but will also alter mate choice and male-male interactions within species (e.g., Pfennig 2000; Boughman 2007; Higgie and Blows 2007). Character displacement thereby alters the targets of sexual selection within species and the selective context in which sexual signals are expressed and perceived. In doing so, competitive and reproductive interactions between species can cause patterns of sexual selection to vary between them (reviewed in Pfennig 1998; Ryan 1998; Ptacek 2000; Boughman 2002; Price 2008; Ortiz-Barrientos et al. 2009; Pfennig and Pfennig 2009; Sobel et al. 2010).

Character displacement can also have important effects on the underlying fitness consequences of sexual selection. Consider, for example, that sexual selection theory generally predicts that females should prefer exaggerated traits because these indicate male quality (reviewed in Andersson 1994). If, however, heterospecifics possess elaborate traits, selection may promote the evolution of preferences for less exaggerated signals that are most dissimilar from those heterospecifics (Ryan and Rand 1993; Pfennig 1998). Yet, by adopting such preferences to avoid costly heterospecific interactions, females may concomitantly forego information about a prospective conspecific mate’s ability to convey additional fitness benefits (Pfennig 2000; Higgie and Blows 2007, 2008; Pfennig 2008).

The resulting fitness trade-offs (i.e., avoiding the costs of heterospecific interactions at the loss of benefits of high quality matings) can explain why divergent mating traits that evolve in sympatry do not spread back into allopatry via gene flow (Pfennig and Pfennig 2005; Higgie and Blows 2007). Indeed, when trade-offs in mate choice arise, sympatric and allopatric populations can experience nearly opposing patterns of mate-choice mediated sexual selection. Consequently, not only will mate preferences diverge between sympatry and allopatry, but sexual signals (and any correlated traits) will also diverge (Pfennig and Pfennig 2005; Higgie and Blows 2007, 2008).

Because divergence between species contributes to divergence within species between sympatric and allopatric populations, resource competition and reproductive interactions between species can lead to different patterns of sexual selection in different conspecific populations (Pfennig 1998; Ptacek 2000; Boughman 2002; Pfennig and Ryan 2007; Price 2008; Ortiz-Barrientos et al. 2009; Pfennig and Pfennig 2009). Likewise, different patterns of sexual selection can arise between ecomorphs in populations experiencing intraspecific character displacement (Boughman 2002). Such variation among populations in sexual selection can, in turn, potentially cause these populations (or ecomorphs) to diverge from each other even more. Indeed, because of its potential ‘runaway’ nature (Kirkpatrick 1982), sexual selection could continue to promote the elaboration of sexual signals even after there is no longer selection to minimize interactions between species or morphs. Thus, character displacement may initiate divergent trajectories of sexual selection between interacting species and, within species, between allopatric and sympatric populations or between alternative ecomorphs. Sexual selection may then cause the further differentiation of signals and preferences beyond what would have resulted if character displacement were the sole diversifying process (Lande 1982). Thus, diversity in sexual traits is likely maximized when sexual selection and character displacement operate in concert.

Once populations (or ecomorphs) diverge in patterns of sexual selection, the stage is set for speciation. Populations may become so divergent in male signals or female preferences that they fail to recognize each other as acceptable mates and become reproductively isolated (Howard 1993; Hoskin et al. 2005; Pfennig and Ryan 2006; Lemmon 2009; Ortiz-Barrientos et al. 2009; Pfennig and Pfennig 2009). Thus, although character displacement tends to initiate divergence between populations or alternative morphs, sexual selection can further enhance this differentiation, making speciation between them even more likely.

Phenotypic plasticity might play a key role in character displacement

Finally, Darwin had little to say about the proximate causes of competitively mediated divergence. This is not surprising, given that he knew little about how traits are produced through development and are inherited. However, understanding character displacement’s proximate bases is crucial, because different mechanisms can influence the speed at which new phenotypic variants arise in a population. Because the speed at which new variants arise determines the speed of character displacement (Doebeli 1996), different proximate mechanisms might ultimately influence whether such divergence even occurs in the first place (Pfennig and Pfennig 2009). In particular, any proximate mechanism that facilitates divergence in resource-use or reproductive phenotypes might render character displacement more likely to transpire, as opposed to competitive or reproductive exclusion. Here, we consider a general and important mechanism for mediating rapid, and potentially adaptive, divergence: phenotypic plasticity.

Phenotypic plasticity—the capacity of a single genome to produce different phenotypes in response to varying environmental conditions (Whitman and Agrawal 2009)—is ubiquitous (reviewed in West-Eberhard 2003; Gilbert and Epel 2009). Darwin clearly recognized that changing environmental conditions could directly induce trait variation among individuals, and he also seems to have grasped the importance of identifying the causes and consequences of phenotypic variation for the evolutionary process. Indeed, in a prefatory notice to August Weismann’s Studies in the Theory of Descent (1882; cited in Canfield and Greene 2009), Darwin wrote that “Several distinguished naturalists maintain with much confidence that organic beings tend to vary … independently of the conditions to which they and their progenitors have been exposed; whilst others maintain that all variation is due to such exposure, though the manner in which the environment acts is as yet quite unknown. At the present time there is hardly any question in biology of more importance than this of the nature and cause of variability.”

What Darwin appears to have failed to appreciate, however, is plasticity’s potential role in character displacement. Individual organisms often respond to the presence of other organisms by altering their phenotype adaptively through phenotypic plasticity (Robinson and Wilson 1994; Pfennig and Murphy 2000; Agrawal 2001; Pfennig and Murphy 2002; Fordyce 2006). For example, when faced with resource competition or reproductive interactions from a heterospecific, individuals of many species facultatively express traits that lessen competition or reproductive interactions (fig. 2; see also Werner and Hall 1976; Nobel 1997; Losos et al. 2000; Pfennig 2007).

Although such environmentally induced changes in resource use or reproductive traits were not initially considered to represent character displacement (e.g., see Grant 1972; Arthur 1982; Schluter and McPhail 1992; Schluter 2000), they often satisfy the widely accepted criteria (Schluter and McPhail 1992) for demonstrating character displacement. In particular, experiments have established that these shifts are triggered by the presence of heterospecifics per se (e.g., see fig. 2d) and that they lessen direct competition with heterospecifics for access to resources or mates (e.g., see Pfennig and Murphy 2000; Pfennig 2007). Moreover, these facultative shifts can evolve: experimentally demonstrated shifts in resource use or reproductive traits often mirror, in magnitude and direction, fixed phenotypic differences observed between naturally occurring populations (e.g., see fig. 2; see also Pfennig 2007) or species (Day et al. 1994; Robinson and Wilson 1994; Losos et al. 2000; Pfennig and Murphy 2002; Robinson and Parsons 2002; Wund et al. 2008) that have undergone character displacement. Indeed, the magnitude and direction of plastic responses are often genetically variable within natural populations (reviewed in Schlichting and Pigliucci 1998; Windig et al. 2004), suggesting that reaction norms themselves can serve as the targets of selection that diverge between interacting species. In other words, the degree to which individuals facultatively respond to heterospecifics might be the trait that evolves in response to resource or reproductive competition. For example, individuals in sympatry with a heterospecific may evolve different propensities to express traits associated with resource use or reproduction relative to those in allopatry and thereby undergo character displacement (e.g., see Pfennig and Murphy 2000, 2002). Essentially, the ability to respond to heterospecifics in the first place serves as another axis of variation (in addition to more commonly considered morphological, physiological, or behavioral traits) on which selection can act to generate divergence between species. In short, character displacement may often proceed via environmentally induced changes in trait expression.

Phenotypic plasticity might even play a key role in determining the initial rate and direction of character displacement. In particular, character displacement may often unfold as it transitions from an initial phase in which trait divergence is environmentally induced to one in which such divergence is expressed constitutively (see Wilson 1992, p. 174). Although this scenario is not the only way in which character displacement could proceed, it explains why character displacement often appears to unfold rapidly (Fenchel 1975; Diamond et al. 1989; Grant and Grant 2006; Pfennig and Martin 2009). Indeed, when trait divergence is environmentally induced, phenotypic differences between species (and, within species, between populations in sympatry with a heterospecific competitor versus conspecific populations in allopatry) can arise within a single generation of the focal species; i.e., divergence can occur on an ecological time scale (Pfennig and Martin 2009).

The hypothesis that character displacement unfolds as it transitions from an initial phase in which trait divergence is environmentally induced to one in which such divergence is expressed constitutively rests on the long-standing observation that induced phenotypes can lose their environmental sensitivity through the process of genetic assimilation (Waddington 1953). Genetic assimilation occurs when an initially environmentally-induced phenotype becomes a constitutively expressed trait (for a recent discussion of evolution by genetic assimilation, see Lande 2009). The loss of plasticity, and the subsequent fixation of an induced, divergent phenotype through genetic assimilation, can proceed via two routes. First, when maintenance or expression of plasticity is costly (Relyea 2002), selection may actively eliminate it (Lahti et al. 2009). Second, plasticity can be lost through mutational degradation or genetic drift (Masel et al. 2007). Essentially, when the members of a population encounter a heterospecific and facultatively expresses a divergent trait, then the alternative phenotype(s) that more closely resemble the heterospecific’s resource-use or reproductive phenotypes would be seldom exposed to selection and might therefore be lost. Experiments have demonstrated the loss of plasticity (Suzuki and Nijhout 2006), and data from natural populations suggests that the resulting differential fixation of alternative phenotypes in different populations can drive character displacement (Pfennig and Murphy 2002).

Phenotypic plasticity might also promote the evolution of genetic differences that ‘fine tune’ phenotypic differences between species that arise via character displacement. In particular, once individuals in a population facultatively express a phenotype that lessens costly competitive or reproductive interactions with heterospecifics, selection should favor those alleles or gene combinations that best stabilize, refine, and extend the divergent trait’s expression through the process of genetic accommodation (West-Eberhard 2003). Moreover, these environmentally initiated shifts may shield populations from extinction via competitive or reproductive exclusion until more permanent genetic differences between populations and species accumulate.

If the outcome of character displacement depends upon initially plastic phenotypes, then phenotypic plasticity in ancestral populations should resemble the constitutively expressed trait differences observed in derived populations that have undergone character displacement. Such a pattern has been found in spadefoot toads (see fig. 2; see also Pfennig and Murphy 2000, 2002; Pfennig and Martin in press) and sticklebacks (Wund et al. 2008). In both taxa, character displacement appears to have gone through an initial evolutionary phase in which divergence was environmentally induced to a later phase in which divergence has become expressed constitutively. Ancestral plasticity has also been implicated in promoting character displacement in numerous species of freshwater fish (Robinson and Wilson 1994) and Anolis lizards (Losos et al. 2000).

The above discussion highlights how plasticity might play a pivotal role in character displacement. Yet, the importance of plasticity in character displacement is currently unclear (in part because of historical bias; see above). Relatively few studies have actually explored plasticity’s role in mediating trait divergence in response to competition. Now is an ideal time to do so, however, because both character displacement and plasticity are undergoing a resurgence of interest (West-Eberhard 2003; Dayan and Simberloff 2005; Gilbert and Epel 2009; Pfennig and Pfennig 2009; Pfennig et al. in press).

Conclusions and future directions

In developing his ‘principle of divergence of character’, Darwin (1859 (2009)) maintained that the origin of species, and the evolution of differences between them, stem ultimately from divergent selection acting to minimize competitive interactions between initially similar individuals, populations, and species. Although evolutionary biologists have since identified numerous other selective and nonselective processes that can also generate diversification and speciation (reviewed in Schluter 2000, 2009; Coyne and Orr 2004; Sobel et al. 2010), character displacement remains an essential part of any general theory for how new species arise and diversify (reviewed in Schluter 2000; Coyne and Orr 2004; Grant and Grant 2008; Pfennig and Pfennig 2009).

Despite character displacement’s importance to Darwin’s thinking, there are key features that he failed to appreciate. Three features, in particular, stand out. First, Darwin appears to have not understood that selection could also act to lessen reproductive interactions, and that such selection could generate divergence rivaling that generated by resource competition. Indeed, reproductive character displacement may be the critical missing link between ecological divergence and the origin of species. Second, he failed to appreciate how divergence is fueled by the intersection of character displacement and sexual selection. Finally, Darwin had little to say about the source(s) of the variation that makes character displacement possible and how the nature of this variation affects the tempo and mode of character displacement. These gaps in Darwin’s thinking are the focus of ongoing research. Although more work remains to be done regarding these above issues, two areas in particular are promising.

First, we need to understand more about how character displacement (whether reproductive or ecological) and sexual selection interact. Unfortunately, researchers generally continue to study character displacement and sexual selection separately, as Darwin did. Although these barriers are breaking down, we still lack a fundamental understanding of: (1) how interactions between species alter mate choice, male competition, and sexual signaling within species; (2) how such interactions alter the fitness consequences of mate choice and mate attraction; (3) the degree to which sexual selection promotes—or inhibits—divergence in response to competitive interactions between species; and (4) how character displacement and sexual selection interact to promote reproductive isolation between populations within species. By addressing these issues, we will gain greater insight into how and why sexual selection varies both between and within species (for further discussion see Ryan 1998; Ptacek 2000; Boughman 2002). We will also better understand sexual selection’s role in the origins and maintenance of trait and species diversity.

A second major area requiring attention are the proximate mechanisms mediating character displacement. Two questions, in particular, stand out First, what is the source of the phenotypic variation on which selection acts during character displacement? Second, how do different sources of phenotypic variation affect the speed and manner in which character displacement unfolds? Although character displacement is assumed to reflect allelic differences between populations and species, it may alternatively begin with competitively mediated phenotypic plasticity. Indeed, as we explained above, character displacement may often unfold rapidly, as it transitions from an initial phase in which species differences arise through phenotypic plasticity to one in which species differences are expressed constitutively.

Additional research into character displacement promises to have far-reaching ramifications. Indeed, because character displacement is central in the origins, abundance, and distribution of biodiversity (reviewed in Schluter 2000; Dayan and Simberloff 2005; Pfennig and Pfennig 2009), understanding character displacement’s causes and consequences can shed light onto some of the most fundamental issues in evolutionary biology and ecology, including how new species arise, diversify, and coexist.

Acknowledgments

We thank D. Schemske for inviting us to contribute this article, and D. Schemske and two anonymous referees for helpful comments on the manuscript. We are also grateful to the National Science Foundation and the National Institutes of Health for funding.

Literature Cited

  • Abrahamson WG, Eubanks MD, Blair CP, Whipple AV. Gall flies, inquilines, and goldenrods: a model for host-race formation and sympatric speciation. American Zoologist. 2001;41:928–938.
  • Adams DC. Parallel evolution of character displacement driven by competitive selection in terrestrial salamanders. BMC Evolutionary Biology. 2010;10:72. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Adams DC, Rohlf FJ. Ecological character displacement in Plethodon: biomechanical differences found from a geometric morphometric study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 2000;97:4106–4111. [PubMed]
  • Agrawal AA. Phenotypic plasticity in the interactions and evolution of species. Science (Washington, D C) 2001;294:321–326. [PubMed]
  • Andersson M. Sexual selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1994.
  • Arthur W. The evolutionary consequences of interspecific competition. Advances in Ecological Research. 1982;12:127–187.
  • Aubret F, Burghardt GM, Maumelata S, Bonnet X, Bradshaw D. Feeding preferences in 2 disjunct populations of tiger snakes, Notechis scutatus (Elapidae) Behavioral Ecology. 2006;17:716–725.
  • Barluenga M, Stölting KN, Salzburger W, Muschick M, Meyer A. Sympatric speciation in Nicaraguan crater lake cichlid fish. Nature. 2006;439:719–723. [PubMed]
  • Behm JE, Ives AR, Boughman JW. Breakdown in postmating isolation and the collapse of a species pair through hybridization. American Naturalist. 2010;175:11–26. [PubMed]
  • Benkman CW. Are the ratios of bill crossing morphs in crossbills the result of frequency-dependent selection? Evolutionary Ecology. 1996;10:119–126.
  • Berlocher SH, Feder JL. Sympatric speciation in phytophagous insects: Moving beyond controversy? Annual Review Of Entomology. 2002;47:773–815. [PubMed]
  • Bolnick DI. Intraspecific competition favours niche width expansion in Drosophila melanogaster. Nature. 2001;410:463–466. [PubMed]
  • Bolnick DI. Can intraspecific competition drive disruptive selection? An experimental test in natural populations of sticklebacks. Evolution. 2004;58:608–618. [PubMed]
  • Bolnick DI, Fitzpatrick BM. Sympatric speciation: models and empirical evidence. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution and Systematics. 2007;38:459–487.
  • Bolnick DI, Lau OL. Predictable patterns of disruptive selection in stickleback in postglacial lakes. American Naturalist. 2008;172:1–11. [PubMed]
  • Boughman JW. Divergent sexual selection enhances reproductive isolation in sticklebacks. Nature. 2001;411:944–948. [PubMed]
  • Boughman JW. How sensory drive can promote speciation. Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 2002;17:571–577.
  • Boughman JW. Condition-dependent expression of red colour differs between stickleback species. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2007;20:1577–1590. [PubMed]
  • Boughman JW, Rundle HD, Schluter D. Parallel evolution of sexual isolation in sticklebacks. Evolution. 2005;59:361–373. [PubMed]
  • Brown WL. Two evolutionary terms. Systematic Zoology. 1964;13:50–52.
  • Brown WL, Wilson EO. Character displacement. Systematic Zoology. 1956;5:49–64.
  • Bush GL, Butlin R. Sympatric speciation in insects. In: Dieckmann U, Doebeli M, Metz JAJ, Tautz D, editors. Adaptive speciation. Cambridge, UK: Cambirdge University Press; 2004. pp. 229–248.
  • Butlin RK, Ritchie MG. Behaviour and speciation. In: Slater PJB, Halliday TR, editors. Behaviour and speciation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1994.
  • Calsbeek R, Smith TB. Experimentally replicated disruptive selection on performance traits in a Caribbean lizard. Evolution. 2008;62:478–484. [PubMed]
  • Calsbeek R, Smith TB, Bardeleben C. Intraspecific variation in Anolis sagrei mirrors the adaptive radiation of Greater Antillean anoles. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 2007;90:189–199.
  • Cavender-Bares J, Ackerly DD, Baum DA, Bazzaz FA. Phylogenetic overdispersion in Floridian oak communities. American Naturalist. 2004;163:823–843. [PubMed]
  • Canfield M, Greene E. Phenotypic plasticity and the semantics of polyphenism: a historical review and current perspectives. In: Whitman DW, Ananthakrishnan TN, editors. Phenotypic plasticity of insects. Enfield, NH: Science Publishers; 2009. pp. 65–80.
  • Carrol SP, Klassen SP, Dingle H. Rapidly evolving adaptations to host ecology and nutrition in the soapberry bug. Evolutionary Ecology. 1998;12:955–968.
  • Caruso CM. Competition for pollination influences selection on floral traits of Ipomopsis aggregata. Evolution. 2000;54:1546–1557. [PubMed]
  • Collins JP. Distribution, habitats and life-history variation in the tiger salamander, Ambystoma tigrinum, in east-central and southeast Arizona. Copeia. 1981;1981:666–675.
  • Collins JP, Cheek JE. Effect of food and density on development of typical and cannibalistic salamander larvae in Ambystoma tigrinum nebulosum. American Zoologist. 1983;23:77–84.
  • Costa JT. Annotated by JT Costa. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press; 2009. The annotated Origin: a facsimile of the first edition of On the origin of species.
  • Coyne JA, Orr HA. Speciation. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer; 2004.
  • Crozier RH. Niche shape and genetical aspects of character displacement. American Zoologist. 1974;14:1151–1157.
  • Darwin C. The annotated Origin: a facsimile of the first edition of On the origin of species. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press; 1859 (2009).
  • Day T, Pritchard J, Schluter D. Ecology and genetics of phenotypic plasticity: a comparison of two sticklebacks. Evolution. 1994;48:1723–1734.
  • Day T, Young KA. Competitive and facilitative evolutionary diversification. BioScience. 2004;54:101–109.
  • Dayan T, Simberloff D. Ecological and community-wide character displacement: the next generation. Ecology Letters. 2005;8:875–894.
  • Diamond J, Pimm SL, Gilpin ME, LeCroy M. Rapid evolution of character displacement in Myzomelid honeyeaters. American Naturalist. 1989;134:675–708.
  • Dieckmann U, Doebeli M. On the origin of species by sympatric speciation. Nature. 1999;400:354–357. [PubMed]
  • Dieckmann U, Doebeli M, Metz J, Tautz D. Adaptive speciation. Cambridge, U. K: Cambridge University Press; 2004.
  • Dobzhansky T. Speciation as a stage in evolutionary divergence. American Naturalist. 1940;74:312–321.
  • Doebeli M. An explicit genetic model for ecological character displacement. Ecology. 1996;77:510–520.
  • Doebeli M, Dieckmann U. Speciation along environmental gradients. Nature. 2003;421:259–264. [PubMed]
  • Duffy S, Burch CL, TPE Evolution of host specificity drives reproductive isolation among RNA viruses. Evolution. 2007;61:2614–2622. [PubMed]
  • Elmer KR, Lehtonen TK, Kautt AF, Harrod C, Meyer A. Rapid sympatric ecological differentiation of crater lake cichlid fishes within historic times. BMC Evolutionary Biology. 2010;8:60. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Endler JA. Natural selection in the wild. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1986.
  • Endler JA, Basolo AL. Sensory ecology, receiver biases and sexual selection. Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 1998;13:415–420. [PubMed]
  • Feder JL, Chilcote CA, Bush GL. Are the apple maggot, Rhagoletis pomonella, and blueberry maggot, Rhagoletis mendax, distinct species? Implications for sympatric speciation. Entomological Experiments and Applications. 1989;51:113–123.
  • Felsenstein J. Skepticism toward Santa Rosalia, or why are there so few kinds of animals? Evolution. 1981;35:124–138.
  • Fenchel T. Character displacement and coexistence in mud snails (Hydrobiidae) Oecologia. 1975;20:19–32.
  • Fordyce JA. The evolutionary consequences of ecological interactions mediated through phenotypic plasticity. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2006;209:2377–2383. [PubMed]
  • Funk DJ. Isolating a role for natural selection in speciation: host adaptation and sexual isolation in Neochlamisus bebbianae leaf beetles. Evolution. 1998;52:1744–1759.
  • Gause GF. The struggle for existence. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins; 1934.
  • Gerhardt HC, Huber F. Acoustic communication in insects and anurans: common problems and diverse solutions. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 2002.
  • Gilbert JJ. Induction and ecological significance of gigantism in the rotifer Asplancha sieboldi. Science (Washington, D C) 1973;181:63–66. [PubMed]
  • Gilbert SF, Epel D. Ecological developmental biology: integrating epigenetics, medicine, and evolution. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates; 2009.
  • Grant PR. Convergent and divergent character displacement. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 1972;4:39–68.
  • Grant PR, Grant BR. Evolution of character displacement in Darwin’s finches. Science (Washington, D C) 2006;313:224–226. [PubMed]
  • Grant PR, Grant BR. How and why species multiply: the radiation of Darwin’s finches. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 2008.
  • Gray SM, Robinson BW. Experimental evidence that competition between stickleback species favours adaptive character divergence. Ecology Letters. 2002;5:264–272.
  • Groning J, Hochkirch A. Reproductive interference between animal species. Quarterly Review of Biology. 2008;83:257–282. [PubMed]
  • Gurevitch J, Morrow LL, Wallace A, Walsh JS. A meta-analysis of competition in field experiments. American Naturalist. 1992;140:539–572.
  • Hansen TF, Armbruster WS, Antonsen L. Comparative analysis of character displacement and spatial adaptations as illustrated by the evolution of Dalechampia blossoms. American Naturalist. 2000;156:S17–S34.
  • Hardin G. The competitive exclusion principle. Science (Washington, D C) 1960;131:1292–1297. [PubMed]
  • Hatfield T, Schluter D. A test for sexual selection on hybrids of two sympatric sticklebacks. Evolution. 1996;50:2429–2434.
  • Hatfield T, Schluter D. Ecological speciation in sticklebacks: environment-dependent hybrid fitness. Evolution. 1999;53:866–873.
  • Hendry AP. Ecological speciation! Or lack thereof? Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences. 2009;66:1383–1398.
  • Hendry AP, Grant PR, Grant BR, Ford HA, Brewer MJ, Podos J. Possible human impacts on adaptive radiation: beak size bimodality in Darwin’s finches. Proceedings of The Royal Society, Series B. 2006;273:1887–1894. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Hendry AP, Huber SK, De Leon LF, Herrel A, Podos J. Disruptive selection in a bimodal population of Darwin’s finches. Proceedings of The Royal Society, Series B. 2009;276:753–759. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Hendry AP, Wenburg JK, Bentzen P, Volk EC, Quinn TP. Rapid evolution of reproductive isolation in the wild: evidence from introduced salmon. Science (Washington, D C) 2000;290:516–518. [PubMed]
  • Herrel A, Huyghe K, Vanhooydonck B, Backeljau T, Breugelmans K, Grbac I, Van Damme R, et al. Rapid large-scale evolutionary divergence in morphology and performance associated with exploitation of a different dietary resource. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 2008;105:4792–4795. [PubMed]
  • Higgie M, Blows MW. Are traits that experience reinforcement also under sexual selection? American Naturalist. 2007;170:409–420. [PubMed]
  • Higgie M, Blows MW. The evolution of reproductive character displacement conflicts with how sexual selection operates within a species. Evolution. 2008;62:1192–1203. [PubMed]
  • Hochkirch A, Groning J, Bucker A. Sympatry with the devil: reproductive interference could hamper species coexistence. Journal of Animal Ecology. 2007;76:633–642. [PubMed]
  • Hori M. Frequency-dependent natural selection in the handedness of scale-eating cichlid fish. Science (Washington, D C) 1993;260:216–219. [PubMed]
  • Hoskin CJ, Higgie M, McDonald KR, Moritz C. Reinforcement drives rapid allopatric speciation. Nature. 2005;437:1353–1356. [PubMed]
  • Howard DJ. Reinforcement: origin, dynamics, and fate of an evolutionary hypothesis. In: Harrison RG, editor. Hybrid zones and the evolutionary process. New York: Oxford University Press; 1993. pp. 46–69.
  • Howard DJ. Conspecific sperm and pollen precedence and speciation. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 1999;30:109–132.
  • Huber SK, De Leon LF, Hendry AP, Bermingham E, Podos J. Reproductive isolation of sympatric morphs in a population of Darwin’s finches. Proceedings of The Royal Society, Series B. 2007;274:1709–1714. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Huber SK, Podos J. Beak morphology and song features covary in a population of Darwin’s finches (Geospiza fortis) Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 2006;88:489–498.
  • Huxley J. The definitive edition. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; 1942 (2010). Evolution: the modern synthesis.
  • Jones CD. The genetic basis of Drosophila sechellias’s resistance to a host plant toxin. Genetics. 1998;149:1899–1908. [PubMed]
  • Kawano K. Character displacement in stag beetles (Coleoptera: Lucanidae) Annals of the Entomological Society of America. 2003;96:503–511.
  • Kingsolver JG, Hoekstra HE, Hoekstra JM, Berrigan D, Vignieri N, Hill CE, Hoang A, et al. The strength of phenotypic selection in natural populations. American Naturalist. 2001;157:245–261. [PubMed]
  • Kirkpatrick M. Sexual selection and the evolution of female choice. Evolution. 1982;36:1–12.
  • Kirkpatrick M, Ravigne V. Speciation by natural and sexual selection: models and experiments. American Naturalist. 2002;159:S22–S35. [PubMed]
  • Konuma J, Chiba S. Ecological character displacement caused by reproductive interference. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 2007;247:354–364. [PubMed]
  • Kopp M, Tollrian R. Trophic size polyphenism in Lembadion bullinum: costs and benefits of an inducible offense. Ecology. 2003;84:641–651.
  • Lack D. The Galápagos finches (Geospizinae): a study in variation. Occasional Papers of the California Academy of Sciences. 1945;21:1–159.
  • Lack D. Darwin’s Finches. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1947.
  • Lahti DC, Johnson NA, Ajie BC, Otto SP, Hendry AP, Blumstein DT, Coss RG, et al. Relaxed selection in the wild. Trends In Ecology & Evolution. 2009;24:487–496. [PubMed]
  • Lande R. Models of speciation by sexual selection on polygenic traits. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 1981;78:3721–3725. [PubMed]
  • Lande R. Rapid Origin of Sexual Isolation and Character Divergence in a Cline. Evolution. 1982;36:213–223.
  • Lande R. Adaptation to an extraordinary environment by evolution of phenotypic plasticity and genetic assimilation. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2009;22:1435–1446. [PubMed]
  • Lemmon EM. Diversification of conspecific signals in sympatry: geographic overlap drives multidimensional reproductive character displacement in frogs. Evolution. 2009;63:1155–1170. [PubMed]
  • Liem KF, Kaufman LS. Intraspecific macroevolution: functional biology of the polymorphic cichlid species Cichlasoma minckleyi. In: Echelle AA, Kornfield I, editors. Evolution of fish species flocks. Orono, ME: University of Maine Press; 1984. pp. 203–215.
  • Losos JB. The evolution of convergent structure in Caribbean Anolis communities. Systematic Biology. 1992;41:403–420.
  • Losos JB. Ecological character displacement and the study of adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 2000;97:5693–5695. [PubMed]
  • Losos JB. Phylogenetic niche conservatism, phylogenetic signal and the relationship between phylogenetic relatedness and ecological similarity among species. Ecology Letters. 2008;11:995–1007. [PubMed]
  • Losos JB, Creer DA, Glossip D, Goellner R, Hampton A, Roberts G, Haskell N, et al. Evolutionary implications of phenotypic plasticity in the hindlimb of the lizard Anolis sagrei. Evolution. 2000;54:301–305. [PubMed]
  • MacArthur RH. Geographical ecology: patterns in the distribution of species. New York: Harper and Row; 1972.
  • MacArthur RH, Wilson EO. The theory of island biogeography. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1967.
  • Maherali H, Klironomos J. Influence of phylogeny on fungal community assembly and ecosystem functioning. Science (Washington, D C) 2007;316:1746–1748. [PubMed]
  • Mallet J. Hybridization, ecological races and the nature of species: empirical evidence for the ease of speciation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B. 2008;363:2971–2986. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Maret TJ, Collins JP. Ecological origin of morphological diversity: a study of alternative trophic phenotypes in larval salamanders. Evolution. 1997;51:898–905.
  • Marko PB. An intraspecific comparative analysis of character divergence between sympatric species. Evolution. 2005;59:554–564. [PubMed]
  • Martin RA, Pfennig DW. Disruptive selection in natural populations: the roles of ecological specialization and resource competition. American Naturalist. 2009;174:268–281. [PubMed]
  • Martin RA, Pfennig DW. Field and experimental evidence that competition and ecological opportunity promote resource polymorphism. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society of London. 2010;100:73–88.
  • Masel J, King OD, Maughan H. The loss of adaptive plasticity during long periods of environmental stasis. American Naturalist. 2007;169:38–46. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Matocq MD, Murphy PJ. Fine-scale phenotypic change across a species transition zone in the genus Neotoma: disentangling independent evolution from phylogenetic history. Evolution. 2007;61:2544–2557. [PubMed]
  • Mather K. The genetical structure of populations. Symposium of the Society for Experimental Biology. 1953;2:196–216.
  • Maynard Smith J. Sympatric speciation. American Naturalist. 1966;104:487–490.
  • Mayr E. Animal species and evolution. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1963.
  • Mayr E. Populations, species, and evolution. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1970.
  • Mayr E. Darwin’s principle of divergence. Journal of the History of Biology. 1992;25:343–359.
  • Medel R, Botto-Mahan C, Kalin-Arroyo M. Pollinator-mediated selection on the nectar guide phenotype in the Andean monkey flower, Mimulus luteus. Ecology. 2003;84:1721–1732.
  • Meyer A. Trophic polymorphisms in cichlid fish: do they represent intermediate steps during sympatric speciation and explain their rapid adaptive radiation? In: Schröder J-H, Bauer J, Schartl M, editors. New trends in ichthyology. London: GSF-Bericht, Blackwell; 1993. pp. 257–266.
  • Nobel PS. Root distribution and seasonal production in the Northwestern Sonoran Desert for a C3 subshrub, a C4 bunchgrass, and a CAM leaf succulent. American Journal of Botany. 1997;84:949–955. [PubMed]
  • Nosil P. Divergent host plant adaptation and reproductive isolation between ecotypes of Timema cristinae walking sticks. American Naturalist. 2007;169:151–162. [PubMed]
  • Nosil P. Speciation with gene flow could be common. Molecular Ecology. 2008;17:2103–2106. [PubMed]
  • Nosil P, Crespi BJ, Sandoval CP. Host-plant adaptation drives the parallel evolution of reproductive isolation. Nature. 2002;417:440–443. [PubMed]
  • Ortiz-Barrientos D, Grealy A, Nosil P. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing; 2009. The genetics and ecology of reinforcement: implications for the evolution of prezygotic isolation in sympatry and beyond, Pages 156–182 Year in Evolutionary Biology 2009. [PubMed]
  • Pacala SW, Roughgarden J. Population experiments with the Anolis lizards of St. Maarten and St. Eustacius. Ecology. 1985;66:129–141.
  • Pfennig DW. Polyphenism in spadefoot toads as a locally adjusted evolutionarily stable strategy. Evolution. 1992;46:1408–1420.
  • Pfennig DW, Martin RA. A maternal effect mediates rapid population divergence and character displacement in spadefoot toads. Evolution. 2009;63:898–909. [PubMed]
  • Pfennig DW, Martin RA. Evolution of character displacement in spadefoot toads: different proximate mechanisms in different species. Evolution in press. [PubMed]
  • Pfennig DW, McGee M. Resource polyphenism increases species richness: a test of the hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B. 2010;365:577–591. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Pfennig DW, Murphy PJ. Character displacement in polyphenic tadpoles. Evolution. 2000;54:1738–1749. [PubMed]
  • Pfennig DW, Murphy PJ. How fluctuating competition and phenotypic plasticity mediate species divergence. Evolution. 2002;56:1217–1228. [PubMed]
  • Pfennig DW, Rice AM. An experimental test of character displacement’s role in promoting postmating isolation between conspecific populations in contrasting competitive environments. Evolution. 2007;61:2433–2443. [PubMed]
  • Pfennig DW, Rice AM, Martin RA. Field and experimental evidence for competition’s role in phenotypic divergence. Evolution. 2007;61:257–271. [PubMed]
  • Pfennig DW, Wund MA, Snell-Rood EC, Cruickshank T, Schlichting CD, Moczek AP. Phenotypic plasticity’s impacts on diversification and speciation. Trends in Ecology and Evolution. (in press) [PubMed]
  • Pfennig KS. The evolution of mate choice and the potential for conflict between species and mate-quality recognition. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences. 1998;265:1743–1748.
  • Pfennig KS. Female spadefoot toads compromise on mate quality to ensure conspecific matings. Behavioral Ecology. 2000;11:220–227.
  • Pfennig KS. Facultative mate choice drives adaptive hybridization. Science (Washington, D C) 2007;318:965–967. [PubMed]
  • Pfennig KS. Population differences in condition-dependent sexual selection may promote divergence in non-sexual traits. Evolutionary Ecology Research. 2008;10:763–773.
  • Pfennig KS, Pfennig DW. Character displacement as the ‘best of a bad situation’: fitness trade-offs resulting from selection to minimize resource and mate competition. Evolution. 2005;59:2200–2208. [PubMed]
  • Pfennig KS, Pfennig DW. Character displacement: ecological and reproductive responses to a common evolutionary problem. Quarterly Review of Biology. 2009;84:253–276. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Pfennig KS, Ryan MJ. Reproductive character displacement generates reproductive isolation among conspecific populations: an artificial neural network study. Proceedings of The Royal Society, Series B. 2006;273:1361–1368. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Pfennig KS, Ryan MJ. Character displacement and the evolution of mate choice: an artificial neural network approach. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B. 2007;362:411–419. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Podos J. Correlated evolution of morphology and vocal signal structure in Darwin’s finches. Nature. 2001;409:185–188. [PubMed]
  • Podos J, Nowicki S. Beaks, adaptation, and vocal evolution in Darwin’s finches. Bioscience. 2004;54:501–510.
  • Polechová J, Barton NH. Speciation through competition: a critical review. Evolution. 2005;59:1194–1210. [PubMed]
  • Price T. Speciation in birds. Greenwood Village, CO: Roberts and Company Publishers; 2008.
  • Pritchard JR, Schluter D. Declining interspecific competition during character displacement: Summoning the ghost of competition past. Evolutionary Ecology Research. 2001;3:209–220.
  • Ptacek MB. Calling sites used by male gray treefrogs, Hyla versicolor and Hyla chrysoscelis, in sympatry and allopatry in Missouri. Herpetologica. 1992;48:373–382.
  • Ptacek MB. The role of mating preferences in shaping interspecific divergence in mating signals in vertebrates. Behavioural Processes. 2000;51:111–134. [PubMed]
  • Ptashne M. A genetic switch. Cambridge, U. K: Cell Press; 1986.
  • Relyea RA. Costs of phenotypic plasticity. American Naturalist. 2002;159:272–282. [PubMed]
  • Reznick DN. The origin then and now: an interpretative guide to the Origin of Species. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 2010.
  • Rice AM, Leichty AR, Pfennig DW. Parallel evolution and ecological selection: replicated character displacement in spadefoot toads. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B. 2009;276:4189–4196. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Rice AM, Pfennig DW. Does character displacement initiate speciation? Evidence of reduced gene flow between populations experiencing divergent selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 2010 [PubMed]
  • Rice WR, Hostert EE. Laboratory experiments on speciation: what have we learned in 40 years? Evolution. 1993;47:1637–1653.
  • Ridley M. How to read Darwin. New York: Norton; 2005.
  • Robinson BW, Parsons KJ. Changing times, spaces, and faces: tests and implications of adaptive morphological plasticity in the fishes of northern postglacial lakes. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences. 2002;59:1819–1833.
  • Robinson BW, Wilson DS. Character release and displacement in fish: a neglected literature. American Naturalist. 1994;144:596–627.
  • Robinson BW, Wilson DS, Margosian AS, Lotito PT. Ecological and morphological differentiation of pumpkinseed sunfish in lakes without bluegill sunfish. Evolutionary Ecology. 1993;7:451–464.
  • Rosenzweig ML. Competitive speciation. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 1978;10:274–289.
  • Roughgarden J. Evolution of niche width. American Naturalist. 1972;106:683–718.
  • Rueffler C, Van Dooren TJM, Leimar O, Abrams PA. Disruptive selection and then what? Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 2006;21:238–245. [PubMed]
  • Rundle HD, Nagel L, Boughman JW, Schluter D. Natural selection and parallel speciation in sympatric sticklebacks. Science. 2000;287:306–308. [PubMed]
  • Rundle HD, Nosil P. Ecological speciation. Ecology Letters. 2005;8:336–352.
  • Rundle HD, Schluter D. Reinforcement of stickleback mate preferences: sympatry breeds contempt. Evolution. 1998;52:200–208.
  • Rundle HD, Schluter D. Natural selection and ecological speciation in sticklebacks. In: Dieckmann U, Doebeli M, Metz JAJ, Tautz D, editors. Adaptive speciation. Cambridge, UK: Cambirdge University Press; 2004. pp. 192–209.
  • Ryals PE, Smith-Somerville HE, Buhse HE., Jr Phenotype switching in polymorphic Tetrahymena: a single-cell Jekyll and Hyde. International Review of Cytology. 2002;212:209–238. [PubMed]
  • Ryan MJ. Sexual selection, receiver biases, and the evolution of sex differences. Science. 1998;281:1999–2003. [PubMed]
  • Ryan MJ, Rand AS. Species recognition and sexual selection as a unitary problem in animal communication. Evolution. 1993;47:647–657.
  • Schemske DW. Adaptation and the origin of species. American Naturalist in press. [PubMed]
  • Schlichting CD, Pigliucci M. Phenotypic evolution: a reaction norm perspective. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer; 1998.
  • Schluter D. Adaptive radiation in sticklebacks: size, shape, and habitat use efficiency. Ecology. 1993;74:699–709.
  • Schluter D. Experimental evidence that competition promotes divergence in adaptive radiation. Science. 1994;266:798–801. [PubMed]
  • Schluter D. The ecology of adaptive radiation. Oxford, U. K: Oxford University Press; 2000.
  • Schluter D. Ecological character displacement. In: Fox CW, Roff DA, Fairbairn DJ, editors. Evolutionary Ecology: Concepts and Case Studies. New York: Oxford University Press; 2001. pp. 265–276.
  • Schluter D. Character displacement. In: Pagel M, editor. Encyclopedia of evolution. Oxford, U.K: Oxford University Press; 2002. pp. 149–150.
  • Schluter D. Frequency dependent natural selection during character displacement in sticklebacks. Evolution. 2003;57:1142–1150. [PubMed]
  • Schluter D. Evidence for ecological speciation and its alternative. Science (Washington, D C) 2009;323:737–741. [PubMed]
  • Schluter D, McPhail JD. Ecological character displacement and speciation in sticklebacks. American Naturalist. 1992;140:85–108. [PubMed]
  • Seehausen O, vanAlphen JJM, Witte F. Cichlid fish diversity threatened by eutrophication that curbs sexual selection. Science. 1997;277:1808–1811.
  • Servedio MR, Noor MAF. The role of reinforcement in speciation: Theory and data. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 2003;34:339–364.
  • Simberloff D, Dayan T, Jones C, Ogura G. Character displacement and release in the small Indian Mongoose, Herpestes javanicus. Ecology. 2000;81:2086–2099.
  • Simpson GG. The major features of evolution. New York: Columbia University Press; 1953.
  • Skúlason S, Snorrason SS, Jónsson B. Sympatric morphs, populations and speciation in freshwater fish with emphasis on arctic charr. In: Magurran AE, May RM, editors. Evolution of biological diversity. Oxford, U. K: Oxford University Press; 1999. pp. 70–92.
  • Smith JW, Benkman CW. A coevolutionary arms race causes ecological speciation in crossbills. American Naturalist. 2007;169:455–465. [PubMed]
  • Smith TB. Disruptive selection and the genetic basis of bill size polymorphism in the African finch Pyrenestes. Nature. 1993;363:618–620.
  • Smith TB, Skúlason S. Evolutionary significance of resource polymorphisms in fishes, amphibians, and birds. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 1996;27:111–133.
  • Sobel JM, Chen GF, Watt LR, Schemske DW. The biology of speciation. Evolution. 2010;64:295–315. [PubMed]
  • Stauffer JR, Gray EV. Phenotypic plasticity: its role in trophic radiation and explosive speciation in cichlids (Teleostei: Cichlidae) Animal Biology. 2004;54:137–158.
  • Suzuki Y, Nijhout HF. Evolution of a polyphenism by genetic accommodation. Science. 2006;311:650–652. [PubMed]
  • Svedin N, Wiley C, Veen T, Gustafsson L, Qvarnström A. Natural and sexual selection against hybrid flycatchers. Proceedings of The Royal Society, Series B. 2008;275:735–744. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Tammone W. Competition, the division of labor, and Darwin’s principle of divergence. Journal of the History of Biology. 1995;28:109–131.
  • Taper ML, Case TJ. Coevolution among competitors. Oxford Surveys in Evolutionary Biology. 1992;8:63–109.
  • Tyerman JG, Bertrand M, Spencer CC, Doebel M. Experimental demonstration of ecological character displacement. BMC Evolutionary Biology. 2008;8:34. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Van Valen L. Morphological variation and width of ecological niche. American Naturalist. 1965;99:377–390.
  • Waddington CH. Genetic assimilation of an acquired character. Evolution. 1953;7:118–126.
  • Webb CO. Exploring the phylogenetic structure of ecological communities: an example for rain forest trees. American Naturalist. 2000;156:145–155. [PubMed]
  • Webb CO, Ackerly DD, McPeek MA, Donoghue MJ. Phylogenies and community ecology. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 2002;33:475–505.
  • Werner EE, Hall DS. Niche shifts in sunfishes: experimental evidence and significance. Science (Washington, D C) 1976;191:404–406. [PubMed]
  • Werner TK, Sherry TW. Behavioral feeding specialization in Pinaroloxias inornata, the “Darwin’s Finch” of Cocos Island, Costa Rica. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U S A. 1987;84:5506–5510. [PubMed]
  • West-Eberhard MJ. Phenotypic plasticity and the origins of diversity. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 1989;20:249–278.
  • West-Eberhard MJ. Developmental plasticity and evolution. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003.
  • West-Eberhard MJ. Developmental plasticity and the origin of species differences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 2005;102:6543–6549. [PubMed]
  • Whitman DW, Agrawal AA. What is phenotypic plasticity and why is it important? In: Whitman DW, Ananthakrishnan TN, editors. Phenotypic plasticity of insects. Enfield, NH: Science Publishers; 2009. pp. 1–63.
  • Wilson DS, Turelli M. Stable underdominance and the evolutionary invasion of empty niches. American Naturalist. 1986;127:835–850.
  • Wilson EO. The nature of the taxon cycle in the Melanesion ant fauna. American Naturalist. 1961;95:169–193.
  • Wilson EO. The diversity of life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1992.
  • Wimberger PH. Trophic polymorphisms, plasticity, and speciation in vertebrates. In: Stouder DJ, Fresh KL, Feller RJ, editors. Theory and application in fish feeding ecology. Columbia, SC: University of South Carolina Press; 1994.
  • Windig JJ, De Kovel CGF, De Jong G. Genetics and mechanics of plasticity. In: DeWitt TJ, Scheiner SM, editors. Phenotypic plasticity. Oxford, U. K: Oxford University Press; 2004. pp. 31–49.
  • Wund MA, Baker JA, Clancy B, Golub JL, Foster SA. A test of the “flexible stem” model of evolution: ancestral plasticity, genetic accommodation, and morphological divergence in the threespine stickleback radiation. American Naturalist. 2008;172:449–462. [PubMed]