PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptHHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
N Engl J Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 August 26.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC3162622
NIHMSID: NIHMS315742

General Anesthesia, Sleep, and Coma

Emery N. Brown, M.D., Ph.D., Ralph Lydic, Ph.D., and Nicholas D. Schiff, M.D.

In the united states, nearly 60,000 patients per day receive general anesthesia for surgery.1 General anesthesia is a drug-induced, reversible condition that includes specific behavioral and physiological traits — unconsciousness, amnesia, analgesia, and akinesia — with concomitant stability of the autonomic, cardiovascular, respiratory, and thermoregulatory systems.2 General anesthesia produces distinct patterns on the electroencephalogram (EEG), the most common of which is a progressive increase in low-frequency, high-amplitude activity as the level of general anesthesia deepens3,4 (Fig. 1). How anesthetic drugs induce and maintain the behavioral states of general anesthesia is an important question in medicine and neuroscience.6 Substantial insights can be gained by considering the relationship of general anesthesia to sleep and to coma.

Figure 1
Electroencephalographic (EEG) Patterns during the Awake State, General Anesthesia, and Sleep

Humans spend approximately one third of their lives asleep. Sleep, a state of decreased arousal that is actively generated by nuclei in the hypothalamus, brain stem, and basal forebrain, is crucial for the maintenance of health.7,8 Normal human sleep cycles between two states — rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep and non-REM sleep — at approximately 90-minute intervals. REM sleep is characterized by rapid eye movements, dreaming, irregularities of respiration and heart rate, penile and clitoral erection, and airway and skeletal-muscle hypotonia.7 In REM sleep, the EEG shows active high-frequency, low-amplitude rhythms (Fig. 1). Non-REM sleep has three distinct EEG stages, with higher-amplitude, lower-frequency rhythms accompanied by waxing and waning muscle tone, decreased body temperature, and decreased heart rate.

Coma is a state of profound unresponsiveness, usually the result of a severe brain injury.9 Comatose patients typically lie with eyes closed and cannot be roused to respond appropriately to vigorous stimulation. A comatose patient may grimace, move limbs, and have stereotypical withdrawal responses to painful stimuli yet make no localizing responses or discrete defensive movements. As the coma deepens, the patient’s responsiveness even to painful stimuli may diminish or disappear. Although the patterns of EEG activity observed in comatose patients depend on the extent of the brain injury, they frequently resemble the high–amplitude, low-frequency activity seen in patients under general anesthesia10 (Fig. 1). General anesthesia is, in fact, a reversible drug-induced coma. Nevertheless, anesthesiologists refer to it as “sleep” to avoid disquieting patients. Unfortunately, anesthesiologists also use the word “sleep” in technical descriptions to refer to unconsciousness induced by anesthetic drugs.11 (For a glossary of terms commonly used in the field of anesthesiology, see the Supplementary Appendix, available with the full text of this article at NEJM.org.)

This review discusses the clinical and neurophysiological features of general anesthesia and their relationships to sleep and coma, focusing on the neural mechanisms of unconsciousness induced by selected intravenous anesthetic drugs.

CLINICAL SIGNS AND EEG PAT TERNS OF UNCONSCIOUSNESS INDUCED BY GENER AL ANESTHESIA

The clinical signs and EEG patterns of general anesthesia–induced unconsciousness can be described in relation to the three periods in which they appear: induction, maintenance, and emergence.

INDUCTION PERIOD

Before induction, the patient has a normal, active EEG, with prominent alpha activity (10 Hz) when the eyes are closed (Fig. 1). Administration of a small dose of a hypnotic drug such as propofol, a barbiturate, or etomidate, all of which act on γ-aminobutyric acid type A (GABAA) receptors, induces a state of sedation in which the patient is calm and easily arousable, with the eyes generally closed.12 As the dose is slowly increased, the patient may enter a state of paradoxical excitation,13 characterized by purposeless or defensive movements, incoherent speech, euphoria or dysphoria, and an increase in beta activity on the EEG (13 to 25 Hz).3,4,1316 This state is termed paradoxical because the drug that is intended to induce unconsciousness induces excitation instead.

As more of the hypnotic agent is administered — typically as a bolus over a period of 10 to 15 seconds — an increasingly irregular respiratory pattern develops that progresses to apnea, at which point bag-mask ventilation must be initiated to support breathing. There is a concomitant loss of response to oral commands and skeletal-muscle tone. Loss of consciousness can be easily assessed by having patients follow the movement of the anesthesiologist’s finger with their eyes. As unconsciousness ensues, eye tracking stops, nystagmus may appear, and blinking increases. The oculocephalic, eyelash, and corneal reflexes are lost,17 yet the pupillary light reflex remains.18 There can be either an increase or a decrease in blood pressure, whereas the heart rate typically increases. Administering an opioid or a benzodiazepine before or during induction may mitigate the increased heart-rate response, and vaso-pressors may be given to maintain blood pressure. Tracheal intubation is usually performed at the end of induction, after the administration of a muscle relaxant.

MAINTENANCE PERIOD

General anesthesia is maintained by a combination of hypnotic agents, inhalational agents, opioids, muscle relaxants, sedatives, and cardiovascular drugs, along with ventilatory and thermoregulatory support. During the maintenance period, changes in the heart rate and blood pressure are among the clinical signs used to monitor the level of general anesthesia (see the Supplementary Appendix). When the state of general anesthesia is inadequate for the level of nociceptive stimulation from surgery, the heart rate and blood pressure can increase dramatically, alerting the anesthesiologist to the possibility of increased nociception and arousal. Other indicators of inadequate general anesthesia are perspiration, tearing, changes in pupil size, the return of muscle tone, movement,19 and changes in EEG measures of brain activity.20 At levels appropriate for surgery, general anesthesia can functionally approximate brain-stem death,21 because patients are unconscious, have depressed brain-stem reflexes, do not respond to nociceptive stimuli, have no apneic drive, and require cardiorespiratory and thermoregulatory support.9

Four EEG patterns define the phases of the maintenance period (Fig. 1). Phase 1, a light state of general anesthesia, is characterized by a decrease in EEG beta activity (13 to 30 Hz) and an increase in EEG alpha activity (8 to 12 Hz) and delta activity (0 to 4 Hz).22 During phase 2, the intermediate state, beta activity decreases and alpha and delta activity increases, with so-called anteriorization — that is, an increase in alpha and delta activity in the anterior EEG leads relative to the posterior leads.22,23 The EEG in phase 2 resembles that seen in stage 3, non-REM (or slow-wave) sleep. Phase 3 is a deeper state, in which the EEG is characterized by flat periods interspersed with periods of alpha and beta activity — a pattern called burst suppression.15 As this state of general anesthesia deepens, the time between the periods of alpha activity lengthens, and the amplitudes of the alpha and beta activity decrease. Surgery is usually performed during phases 2 and 3. In phase 4, the most profound state of general anesthesia, the EEG is isoelectric (completely flat). An isoelectric EEG may be purposely induced by the administration of a barbiturate or propofol to protect the brain during neurosurgery24 or to stop generalized seizures.25,26

EMERGENCE PERIOD

Emergence from general anesthesia is a passive process that depends on the amounts of drugs administered; their sites of action, potency, and pharmacokinetics; the patient’s physiological characteristics; and the type and duration of the surgery. Recovery from general anesthesia is generally assessed by monitoring physiological and behavioral signs. The return of spontaneous respirations is typically one of the first clinical signs observed once peripheral neuromuscular blockade is decreased. This marks the patient’s return from a functional state that approximates brainstem death (Table 1). The heart rate and blood pressure typically increase, provided that these responses are not pharmacologically blocked. Salivation and tearing begin, followed by nonlocalizing responses to painful stimulation, suggesting that the patient’s state is most similar to a vegetative state with the notable exception that the eyes remain closed. As skeletal-muscle tone returns, the patient begins to grimace, swallow, gag, and cough and make defensive movements, such as reaching for the endotracheal or naso-gastric tube. At this point the anesthesiologist will perform extubation, provided that there is sufficient return of brain-stem reflexes to maintain spontaneous respirations and airway protection, even if there is no response to oral commands. The eyes may still not open spontaneously. As the patient emerges from general anesthesia, the EEG patterns proceed in approximately reverse order from phases 2 or 3 of the maintenance period to an active EEG that is consistent with a fully awake state (Fig. 1). Between extubation and discharge from the postanesthesia care unit, the patient passes through a minimally conscious state.27 Functional responses that are beyond a minimally conscious state must be evident before the patient is discharged from the postanesthesia care unit.27,28 The patient should be able to answer simple questions and to convey any discomfort, such as pain or nausea.

Table 1
Emergence from General Anesthesia and Stages of Recovery from Coma.*

MECHANISMS OF UNCONSCIOUSNESS INDUCED BY GENER AL ANESTHESIA

CORTICAL CIRCUITS AND ALTERED AROUSAL

Observations from clinical practice and basic science indicate that anesthetic drugs induce unconsciousness by altering neurotransmission at multiple sites in the cerebral cortex,2935 brain stem, and thalamus. If a procedure does not require full general anesthesia, it is standard clinical practice to use low doses of a hypnotic or sedative drug to achieve sedation, which is defined as diminished cognitive function (cortical activity)35 with intact respiratory and cardiovascular (brain-stem) function. Substantial decreases in neural activity in the cortex have been observed in a rodent model of general anesthesia.30 Similarly, positron-emission tomographic studies in humans under general anesthesia revealed appreciable decreases in cortical metabolic activity.31,32 Functional magnetic resonance imaging33 and local-field-potential recordings34 in humans have provided additional evidence of cortical mechanisms of unconsciousness induced by general anesthesia. In vivo and in vitro molecular pharmacologic studies have identified GABAA and N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors in the cortex, thalamus, brain stem, and striatum as two of the important targets of hypnotic drugs.36,37 Because small numbers of inhibitory interneurons control large numbers of excitatory pyramidal neurons, the enhanced GABAA inhibition induced by general anesthesia can efficiently inactivate large regions of the brain and contribute to unconsciousness (Fig. 2A).38,39

Figure 2
Possible Neural-Circuit Mechanisms of Altered Arousal Induced by Anesthetic Agents

THE BRAIN STEM, SLEEP, AND ALTERED AROUSAL

A hypnotic drug administered as a bolus during induction of general anesthesia rapidly reaches brain-stem arousal centers, where it contributes to unconsciousness.40 The clinical signs cited above are consistent with actions in the brain stem. Losses of the oculocephalic and corneal reflexes are nonspecific indicators of impaired brain-stem function due to the actions of the hypnotic agent on the oculomotor, trochlear, abducens, trigeminal, and facial nuclei in the midbrain and pons.9

In a study in rodents, direct injection of a barbiturate into the mesopontine tegmental area led to unconsciousness.41 Such observations confirm brain-injury studies showing that brain-stem unconsciousness typically involves the lateral dorsal tegmental areas of the pons and the midbrain paramedian region.42 Apnea can be explained, in part, by the actions of the hypnotic agent on GABAA interneurons in the respiratory control network in the ventral medulla and pons.43

The rapid atonia that occurs after bolus administration of propofol is most likely due to the actions of this drug in the spinal cord44 and in the pontine and medullary reticular nuclei that control the antigravity muscles.45 Certain observations are consistent with this concept of propofol’s mechanism of action — for example, the atonia that follows the inadvertent injection of local anesthetics into the subarachnoid space or basilar artery during an interscalene block46,47 or the inadvertent injection of alcohol into the cervical spinal cord during a facet block.9 Furthermore, observations in patients who have pontine strokes and locked-in syndromes support this concept,9 as do observations with the muscle-relaxing and soporific effects of the GABA agonist baclofen.48 Such observations may explain why near the end of surgery, small doses of propofol can be used to provide rapid muscle relaxation of short duration and why, in this case, propofol is preferable to muscle relaxants that have a slower onset and longer duration of action. In contrast to the drug-induced atonia described above, rigidity and spasticity are typically seen in patients who are in a coma or a vegetative state,9 and muscle tone is preserved during slow-wave sleep.7

Signs of the loss of brain-stem function (apnea, atonia, and losses of the oculocephalic and corneal49 reflexes) and hence unconsciousness reliably indicate when to initiate bag-mask ventilation or to place a laryngeal mask airway during induction of general anesthesia.

Neural circuits involved in sleep provide additional insights into basal forebrain, brain-stem, and hypothalamic mechanisms of unconsciousness. During the awake state, the locus ceruleus provides norepinephrine-mediated inhibition of the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus in the hypothalamus.50,51 Therefore, GABAA-mediated and galanin-mediated inhibition of the ascending arousal circuits by the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus is inhibited and the awake state is promoted.51 Adenosine, one of the brain’s principal somnogens, accumulates from degradation of adenosine triphosphate during prolonged intervals of wakefulness.8 Binding of adenosine in the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus is associated with increased activity in this brain center.52 Adenosine binding and inhibition of the locus ceruleus lead to activation of the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus, which inhibits the ascending arousal circuits and promotes non-REM sleep. The sedative dexmedetomidine is an α2-adrenergic agonist that inhibits the release of norepinephrine from the locus ceruleus and thus allows the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus to reduce arousal by inhibiting the ascending arousal circuits (Fig. 2B).53,54 The EEG patterns of dexmedetomidine-induced sedation closely resemble those of non-REM sleep.55 Propofol also promotes unconsciousness, in part, by GABAA-mediated inhibition of release of the arousal-promoting neurotransmitter histamine in the cortex from the tuberomammillary nucleus in the hypothalamus.50

The active EEG patterns observed during REM sleep are due in part to strong cholinergic inputs from the lateral dorsal tegmental and pedunculopontine tegmental nuclei to the medial pontine reticular formation and thalamus and from the basal forebrain to the cortex.56 The synthetic opioid fentanyl decreases arousal by reducing acetylcholine in the medial pontine reticular formation, whereas morphine decreases arousal by inhibiting the neurons in the lateral dorsal teg-mental nucleus, medial pontine reticular formation,56 and basal forebrain57 (Fig. 2C). Opioids further contribute to unconsciousness by binding to opioid receptors in the periaqueductal gray,58 rostral ventral medulla,58,59 spinal cord,59 and possibly peripheral tissue60 to reduce nociceptive transmission in the central nervous system. That opioids act primarily in the nociceptive pathways rather than in the cortex to alter arousal and to partially alter cognition helps explain the high incidence of postoperative awareness in patients undergoing cardiac surgery, for which high-dose opioids have, until recently, been the primary anesthetic.61,62

Clinical studies have shown that unconsciousness induced by propofol can be reversed by the administration of the cholinomimetic agent physostigmine.63 A combination of imaging,31 molecular,37 and neurophysiological30 studies suggests that propofol acts, in part, by enhancing GABAA-mediated inhibition by interneurons of pyramidal neurons in the cortex and subcortical areas,50 whereas physostigmine counteracts this effect by enhancing cholinergic activity throughout the cortex (Fig. 2A). Physostigmine is a standard treatment for emergence delirium,64,65 a state of confusion seen on emergence from general anesthesia.

The EEG patterns and other features of general anesthesia generally differ from those of sleep (Fig. 1). The highly active state of the cortex during REM sleep is mediated by a cholinergic drive emanating from the basal forebrain and from the lateral dorsal and pedunculopontine tegmental nuclei to the cortex via the thalamus.56 As discussed below, paradoxical excitation under general anesthesia may represent GABA-mediated disinhibition in striatothalamic pathways. The skeletal-muscle hypotonia observed during REM sleep is partially due to cholinergic activation of pontomedullary networks, resulting in glycine-mediated inhibition of alpha motor neurons in the spinal cord,66 whereas during paradoxical excitation, motor tone is preserved.13

There is similarity between the EEG patterns seen in slow-wave sleep and those seen in phase 2 of the maintenance period of general anesthesia (Fig. 1). This period of general anesthesia is sufficiently deep to perform surgery. During sleep, the greatest decrease in pain perception occurs in the slow-wave stage. Arousal is possible during this stage, but it requires stronger stimulation than during other stages of sleep.67 Slow-wave sleep has been shown to represent the switch of the thalamus from its tonic to its bursting mode.68 The tonic mode favors transmission of somato-sensory information through the thalamus, whereas the bursting mode inhibits transmission of such information. Phase 2 of the maintenance period of general anesthesia and slow-wave sleep represent profound decreases in cortical activity achieved by pharmacologic means and by an endogenous circuit mechanism, respectively.

CENTRAL THALAMIC CIRCUITS AND CONTROL OF AROUSAL

The central thalamus plays an important role in normal arousal regulation.69 The convergence in this area of ascending pathways from the brain stem and basal forebrain and descending pathways from the frontal cortex helps regulate forebrain arousal and maintain organized behavior during wakefulness. Both direct injury of the central thalamus and marked deafferentation of its neurons due to diffuse brain insults are associated with the severe impairment of arousal and of forebrain functional integration observed in several brain disorders.70 Electrical stimulation of the central thalamus in a minimally conscious patient has been reported to improve cognitive function, mobility, and oral feeding.71 Similarly, unconsciousness induced by general anesthesia has been reversed experimentally by the direct injection of cholinergic agonists into the central thalamus.72

The central thalamus may mediate several of the phenomena seen in general anesthesia, as described above (Fig. 3). A possible explanation for the paradoxical excitation observed with a low dose of nearly every anesthetic drug3,4,1316 is suggested by the circuit mechanism,69,73 which has been proposed to explain the paradoxical arousal of a patient from a minimally conscious state after administration of the GABAA1 agonist zolpidem.74 After administration of zolpidem, the marked downregulation of the anterior-forebrain network that accompanied this patient’s brain injury was reversed, with notable functional improvement. The globus pallidus interna normally provides tonic inhibitory inputs to the central thalamus that are opposed by striatal inhibition of the pallidum. Binding of zolpidem in the GABAA1-receptor–rich globus pallidus interna may suppress this tonic inhibitory input to the thalamus,75 fostering activation of thalamocortical and thalamostriatal circuits and, consequently, enhancing arousal (Fig. 3A). For anesthetic drugs that promote GABAA inhibition, paradoxical excitation may be explained by a similar action in these circuits. This hypothesis could also account for the paradoxical excitation observed during endoscopy and the delirium observed in intensive care units that is commonly associated with sedation induced with benzodiazepines, which are GABAA agonists.76,77 The fact that purpose-less movements are associated with paradoxical excitation is consistent with a possible basal ganglia mechanism.13

Figure 3
Paradoxical Excitation, Cerebral Metabolism, and Electroencephalographic (EEG) Activity in Stages of Coma Recovery

An imaging study in humans suggests a role for the corticobasal ganglia–thalamic circuit in propofol-induced unconsciousness.78 This circuit may also underlie anteriorization (Fig. 3B). The delta and alpha rhythms seen on the EEG during general anesthesia and sleep79 could arise from sustained hyperpolarization of pyramidal neurons in the lower output layers of the cortex owing to withdrawal (during sleep) or inhibition (during general anesthesia) of excitatory synaptic inputs.80 Initiation of these changes in the anterior forebrain could be a consequence of active inhibition of the central thalamus that occurs when cortical inputs to the striatum are insufficient (Fig. 3A). A theoretical model has shown that as propofol is increased to a dose beyond that which produces paradoxical excitation, its actions in the thalamocortical circuits lead to a coherent alpha oscillation between the thalamus and the anterior forebrain.81

Finally, burst suppression is believed to be a strong, synchronized outflow of thalamic discharges to a widely unresponsive cortex82 (Fig. 1). It is a deeper state of general anesthesia than is phase 2 of the maintenance period, which resembles the tonic bursting mode of the thalamus seen in slow-wave sleep. Bursts become more widely separated during burst suppression as the level of general anesthesia deepens. This suggests that a larger fraction of the cortex is inactive during burst suppression relative to phase 2 of general anesthesia or slow-wave sleep. Supporting this hypothesis is the observation that burst suppression is also seen in coma due to diffuse anoxic damage,83 induced hypothermia,84 and epilepsy due to the Ohtahara syndrome.85 The absence of burst suppression during sleep is an important electrophysiological distinction between sleep and general anesthesia.

ACTIVE BR AIN STATES AND UNCONSCIOUSNESS

In contrast to unconsciousness induced by most hypnotic agents, which is predominantly associated with slow EEG patterns, unconsciousness induced by the NMDA antagonist ketamine is associated with active EEG patterns.86,87 Seizures are commonly associated with active, highly organized EEG patterns. Unconsciousness due to seizures most likely results from organized, aberrant brain activity that impedes the normal communications necessary to maintain arousal and cognition.88 Similarly, a highly active brain state most likely plays a role in unconsciousness induced by ketamine. Ketamine preferentially inhibits NMDA-mediated glutamatergic inputs to GABAergic interneurons, leading to aberrant excitatory activity in the cortex, hippocampus, and limbic system and ultimately unconsciousness (Fig. 4).89,90 Hallucinations may result because the aberrant activation allows the association of information in a manner that is inconsistent in time and space. The hallucinations can be mitigated by the concurrent administration of a benzodiazepine,91 which presumably acts to enhance GABAA-mediated activity of the interneurons and hence leads to sedation. The potent antinociceptive effects of ketamine on NMDA receptors in the spinal cord and its inhibition of acetylcholine release from the pons also contribute to unconsciousness (Fig. 4).9294

Figure 4
Unconsciousness and Active Brain States

EMERGENCE FROM GENER AL ANESTHESIA AND RECOVERY FROM COMA

Recovery from an initial coma — if it occurs — may require hours to years. In contrast, emergence from general anesthesia typically requires minutes. Nevertheless, it is useful to compare emergence from general anesthesia and recovery from coma (Table 1). The early clinical signs of emergence from general anesthesia — return of regular breathing, salivation, tearing, swallowing, gagging, and grimacing — approximate the caudal–rostral progression in the brain stem of the return of sensory, motor, and autonomic function (Table 1). A later sign, such as response to oral commands, indicates the return of cortical function. The quantitative neurobehavioral metrics used to monitor recovery from coma could be used to track the emergence from general anesthesia from a functional state that can approximate brain-stem death to states similar to a vegetative state and, eventually, to a minimally conscious state.95 The fact that general anesthesia can be functionally equivalent to brain-stem death indicates how deeply general anesthesia can depress brain function and perhaps explains why some patients do not fully recover consciousness for several hours after general anesthesia and why postoperative cognitive dysfunction could persist in elderly patients for several months.96

In conclusion, a better understanding of sleep and coma may lead to new approaches to general anesthesia based on new ways to alter consciousness,29,97,98 provide analgesia,99,100 induce amnesia, and provide muscle relaxation.66

Supplementary Material

Supplement

Acknowledgments

Supported by the Massachusetts General Hospital Department of Anesthesia, Critical Care, and Pain Medicine and by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) Director’s Pioneer Award (DP1OD003646, to Dr. Brown); by the University of Michigan, Department of Anesthesiology, and by NIH grants (HL40881 and HL65272, to Dr. Lydic); and by grants from the James S. McDonnell Foundation (to Dr. Schiff) and the NIH (HD51912, to Dr. Schiff).

Footnotes

Disclosure forms provided by the authors are available with the full text of this article at NEJM.org.

References

1. Sentinel event alert: preventing, and managing the impact of, anesthesia awareness. Oakbrook Terrace, IL: The Joint Commission; 2004. ( http://www.jointcommission.org/sentinel_event_alert_issue_32_preventing_and_managing_the_impact_of_anesthesia_awareness.)
2. Evers A, Crowder M. Cellular and molecular mechanisms of anesthesia. In: Barash PG, Cullen BF, Stoelting RK, Cahalan M, Stock MC, editors. Clinical anesthesia. 6. New York: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2006. pp. 95–114.
3. Gibbs FA, Gibbs LE, Lennox WG. Effects on the electroencephalogram of certain drugs which influence nervous activity. Arch Intern Med. 1937;60:154–66.
4. Kiersey DK, Bickford RG, Faulconer A., Jr Electro-encephalographic patterns produced by thiopental sodium during surgical operations; description and classification. Br J Anaesth. 1951;23:141–52. [PubMed]
5. Watson C, Bagdoyan H, Lydic R. A neurochemical perspective on states of consciousness. In: Hudetz AG, Pearce RA, editors. Suppressing the mind: anesthetic modulation of memory and consciousness. New York: Springer/Humana Press; 2010. pp. 33–80.
6. Kennedy D, Norman C. What don’t we know? Science. 2005;309:75. [PubMed]
7. Kryger M, Roth T, Dement W. Principles and practice of sleep medicine. 5. New York: Elsevier Saunders; 2010.
8. McCarley RW. Neurobiology of REM and NREM sleep. Sleep Med. 2007;8:302–30. [PubMed]
9. Posner J, Saper C, Schiff N, Plum F. Plum and Posner’s diagnosis of stupor and coma. New York: Oxford University Press; 2007.
10. Young GB. The EEG in coma. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2000;17:473–85. [PubMed]
11. Gawande A, Denno DW, Truog RD, Waisel DM. Physicians and execution — highlights from a discussion of lethal injection. N Engl J Med. 2008;358:448–51. [PubMed]
12. Davis MH, Coleman MR, Absalom AR, et al. Dissociating speech perception and comprehension at reduced levels of awareness. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:16032–7. [PubMed]
13. Bevan JC, Veall GR, Macnab AJ, Ries CR, Marsland C. Midazolam premedication delays recovery after propofol without modifying involuntary movements. Anesth Analg. 1997;85:50–4. [PubMed]
14. McCarthy MM, Brown EN, Kopell N. Potential network mechanisms mediating electroencephalographic beta rhythm changes during propofol-induced paradoxical excitation. J Neurosci. 2008;28:13488–504. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
15. Clark DL, Rosner BS. Neurophysiologic effects of general anesthetics. I. The electroencephalogram and sensory evoked responses in man. Anesthesiology. 1973;38:564–82. [PubMed]
16. Rampil IJ. A primer for EEG signal processing in anesthesia. Anesthesiology. 1998;89:980–1002. [PubMed]
17. Coté CJ, Goudsouzian NG, Liu LM, Dedrick DF, Rosow CE. The dose response of intravenous thiopental for the induction of general anesthesia in unpremedicated children. Anesthesiology. 1981;55:703–5. [PubMed]
18. Gray AT, Krejci ST, Larson MD. Neuromuscular blocking drugs do not alter the pupillary light reflex of anesthetized humans. Arch Neurol. 1997;54:579–84. [PubMed]
19. Prys-Roberts C. Anaesthesia: a practical or impractical construct? Br J Anaesth. 1987;59:1341–5. [PubMed]
20. Palanca BJ, Mashour GA, Avidan MS. Processed electroencephalogram in depth of anesthesia monitoring. Curr Opin Anaesthesiol. 2009;22:553–9. [PubMed]
21. Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Practice parameters: determining brain death in adults. Neurology. 1995;45:1012–4. [PubMed]
22. Feshchenko VA, Veselis RA, Reinsel RA. Propofol-induced alpha rhythm. Neuropsychobiology. 2004;50:257–66. [PubMed]
23. Tinker JH, Sharbrough FW, Michenfelder JD. Anterior shift of the dominant EEG rhythm during anesthesia in the Java monkey: correlation with anesthetic potency. Anesthesiology. 1977;46:252–9. [PubMed]
24. Doyle PW, Matta BF. Burst suppression or isoelectric encephalogram for cerebral protection: evidence from metabolic suppression studies. Br J Anaesth. 1999;83:580–4. [PubMed]
25. Bergey GK. Refractory status epilepticus: is EEG burst suppression an appropriate treatment target during drug- induced coma? What is the Holy Grail? Epilepsy Curr. 2006;6:119–20. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
26. Claassen J, Hirsch LJ, Emerson RG, Mayer SA. Treatment of refractory status epilepticus with pentobarbital, propofol, or midazolam: a systematic review. Epilepsia. 2002;43:146–53. [PubMed]
27. Giacino JT, Ashwal S, Childs N, et al. The minimally conscious state: definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology. 2002;58:349–53. [PubMed]
28. Aldrete JA. The post-anesthesia recovery score revisited. J Clin Anesth. 1995;7:89–91. [PubMed]
29. Alkire MT, Hudetz AG, Tononi G. Consciousness and anesthesia. Science. 2008;322:876–80. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
30. Angel A. The G.L. Brown lecture. Adventures in anaesthesia. Exp Physiol. 1991;76:1–38. [PubMed]
31. Alkire MT, Haier RJ, Barker SJ, Shah NK, Wu JC, Kao YJ. Cerebral metabolism during propofol anesthesia in humans studied with positron emission tomography. Anesthesiology. 1995;82:393–403. [PubMed]
32. Fiset P, Paus T, Daloze T, et al. Brain mechanisms of propofol-induced loss of consciousness in humans: a positron emission tomographic study. J Neurosci. 1999;19:5506–13. [PubMed]
33. Purdon PL, Pierce ET, Bonmassar G, et al. Simultaneous electroencephalography and functional magnetic resonance imaging of general anesthesia. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1157:61–70. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
34. Velly LJ, Rey MF, Bruder NJ, et al. Differential dynamic of action on cortical and subcortical structures of anesthetic agents during induction of anesthesia. Anesthesiology. 2007;107:202–12. [Erratum, Anesthesiology 2008;108:175.] [PubMed]
35. Ferrarelli F, Massimini M, Sarasso S, et al. Breakdown in cortical effective connectivity during midazolam-induced loss of consciousness. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:2681–6. [PubMed]
36. Rudolph U, Antkowiak B. Molecular and neuronal substrates for general anaesthetics. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:709–20. [PubMed]
37. Hemmings HC, Jr, Akabas MH, Gold-stein PA, Trudell JR, Orser BA, Harrison NL. Emerging molecular mechanisms of general anesthetic action. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2005;26:503–10. [PubMed]
38. Bai D, Pennefather PS, MacDonald JF, Orser BA. The general anesthetic propofol slows deactivation and desensitization of GABA(A) receptors. J Neurosci. 1999;19:10635–46. [PubMed]
39. Shepherd G. The synaptic organization of the brain. 4. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998.
40. Propofol. Bedford, OH: Bedford Laboratories; 2008. (package insert). ( http://dailymed.nlm.nih.gov/dailymed/drugInfo.cfm?id=6337)
41. Devor M, Zalkind V. Reversible analgesia, atonia, and loss of consciousness on bilateral intracerebral microinjection of pentobarbital. Pain. 2001;94:101–12. [PubMed]
42. Parvizi J, Damasio AR. Neuroanatomical correlates of brainstem coma. Brain. 2003;126:1524–36. [PubMed]
43. Feldman JL, Del Negro CA. Looking for inspiration: new perspectives on respiratory rhythm. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:232–42. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
44. Kungys G, Kim J, Jinks SL, Atherley RJ, Antognini JF. Propofol produces immobility via action in the ventral horn of the spinal cord by a GABAergic mechanism. Anesth Analg. 2009;108:1531–7. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
45. Pontine (medial) and medullary (lateral) reticulospinal tracts. Laboratory 12: tract systems I. University of Pennsylvania School of Veterinary Medicine; 2006. ( http://cal.vet.upenn.edu/projects/neurology/lab12/lab12_rspt.htm.)
46. Dutton RP, Eckhardt WF, III, Sunder N. Total spinal anesthesia after interscalene blockade of the brachial plexus. Anesthesiology. 1994;80:939–41. [PubMed]
47. Durrani Z, Winnie AP. Brainstem toxicity with reversible locked-in syndrome after intrascalene brachial plexus block. Anesth Analg. 1991;72:249–52. [PubMed]
48. Penn RD, Savoy SM, Corcos D, et al. Intrathecal baclofen for severe spinal spasticity. N Engl J Med. 1989;320:1517–21. [PubMed]
49. Mourisse J, Gerrits W, Lerou J, van Egmond J, Zwarts MJ, Booij L. Electromyo-graphic assessment of blink and corneal reflexes during midazolam administration: useful methods for assessing depth of anesthesia? Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. 2003;47:593–600. [PubMed]
50. Nelson LE, Guo TZ, Lu J, Saper CB, Franks NP, Maze M. The sedative component of anesthesia is mediated by GABA(A) receptors in an endogenous sleep pathway. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:979–84. [PubMed]
51. Saper CB, Scammell TE, Lu J. Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and circadian rhythms. Nature. 2005;437:1257–63. [PubMed]
52. Morairty S, Rainnie D, McCarley R, Greene R. Disinhibition of ventrolateral preoptic area sleep-active neurons by adenosine: a new mechanism for sleep promotion. Neuroscience. 2004;123:451–7. [PubMed]
53. Correa-Sales C, Rabin BC, Maze M. A hypnotic response to dexmedetomidine, an alpha 2 agonist, is mediated in the locus coeruleus in rats. Anesthesiology. 1992;76:948–52. [PubMed]
54. Jorm CM, Stamford JA. Actions of the hypnotic anaesthetic, dexmedetomidine, on noradrenaline release and cell firing in rat locus coeruleus slices. Br J Anaesth. 1993;71:447–9. [PubMed]
55. Huupponen E, Maksimow A, Lapinlampi P, et al. Electroencephalogram spindle activity during dexmedetomidine sedation and physiological sleep. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. 2008;52:289–94. [PubMed]
56. Lydic R, Baghdoyan HA. Sleep, anesthesiology, and the neurobiology of arousal state control. Anesthesiology. 2005;103:1268–95. [PubMed]
57. Osman NI, Baghdoyan HA, Lydic R. Morphine inhibits acetylcholine release in rat prefrontal cortex when delivered systemically or by microdialysis to basal forebrain. Anesthesiology. 2005;103:779–87. [PubMed]
58. Heinricher MM, Morgan MM. Supra-spinal mechanisms of opioid analgesia. In: Stein C, editor. Opioids in pain control: basic and clinical aspects. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press; 1999. pp. 46–69.
59. Cesselin FBJ, Bourgoin S, et al. Spinal mechanisms of analgesia. In: Stein C, editor. Opioids in pain control: basic and clinical aspects. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press; 1999. pp. 70–95.
60. Stein C, Machelska H, Binder W, Schafer M. Peripheral opioid analgesia. Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2001;1:62–5. [PubMed]
61. Hug CC., Jr Does opioid “anesthesia” exist? Anesthesiology. 1990;73:1–4. [PubMed]
62. Hilgenberg JC. Intraoperative awareness during high-dose fentanyl–oxygen anesthesia. Anesthesiology. 1981;54:341–3. [PubMed]
63. Meuret P, Backman SB, Bonhomme V, Plourde G, Fiset P. Physostigmine reverses propofol-induced unconsciousness and attenuation of the auditory steady state response and bispectral index in human volunteers. Anesthesiology. 2000;93:708–17. [PubMed]
64. Lepousé C, Lautner CA, Liu L, Gomis P, Leon A. Emergence delirium in adults in the post-anaesthesia care unit. Br J Anaesth. 2006;96:747–53. [PubMed]
65. Funk W, Hollnberger H, Geroldinger J. Physostigmine and anaesthesia emergence delirium in preschool children: a randomized blinded trial. Eur J Anaesthesiol. 2008;25:37–42. [PubMed]
66. Chase MH. Confirmation of the consensus that glycinergic postsynaptic inhibition is responsible for the atonia of REM sleep. Sleep. 2008;31:1487–91. [PubMed]
67. Lavigne GJ, Sessle BJ, Choiniere M, Soja PJ, editors. Sleep and pain. Seattle: IASP Press; 2007.
68. Steriade M, Timofeev I. Neuronal plasticity in thalamocortical networks during sleep and waking oscillations. Neuron. 2003;37:563–76. [PubMed]
69. Schiff ND. Central thalamic contributions to arousal regulation and neurological disorders of consciousness. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1129:105–18. [PubMed]
70. Schiff ND, Plum F. The role of arousal and “gating” systems in the neurology of impaired consciousness. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2000;17:438–52. [PubMed]
71. Schiff ND, Giacino JT, Kalmar K, et al. Behavioural improvements with thalamic stimulation after severe traumatic brain injury. Nature. 2007;448:600–3. [PubMed]
72. Alkire MT, McReynolds JR, Hahn EL, Trivedi AN. Thalamic microinjection of nicotine reverses sevoflurane-induced loss of righting reflex in the rat. Anesthesiology. 2007;107:264–72. [PubMed]
73. Schiff ND, Posner JB. Another “Awakenings” Ann Neurol. 2007;62:5–7. [PubMed]
74. Brefel-Courbon C, Payoux P, Ory F, et al. Clinical and imaging evidence of zolpidem effect in hypoxic encephalopathy. Ann Neurol. 2007;62:102–5. [PubMed]
75. Chen L, Savio Chan C, Yung WH. Electrophysiological and behavioral effects of zolpidem in rat globus pallidus. Exp Neurol. 2004;186:212–20. [PubMed]
76. Fulton SA, Mullen KD. Completion of upper endoscopic procedures despite paradoxical reaction to midazolam: a role for flumazenil? Am J Gastroenterol. 2000;95:809–11. [PubMed]
77. Tung A, Tadimeti L, Caruana-Montaldo B, et al. The relationship of sedation to deliberate self-extubation. J Clin Anesth. 2001;13:24–9. [PubMed]
78. Mhuircheartaigh RN, Rosenorn-Lanng D, Wise R, Jbabdi S, Rogers R, Tracey I. Cortical and subcortical connectivity changes during decreasing levels of consciousness in humans: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study using propofol. J Neurosci. 2010;30:9095–102. [PubMed]
79. Steriade M, Nuñez A, Amzica F. A novel slow (< 1 Hz) oscillation of neo-cortical neurons in vivo: depolarizing and hyperpolarizing components. J Neurosci. 1993;13:3252–65. [PubMed]
80. Steriade M, Contreras D, Curro Dossi R, Nuñez A. The slow (< 1 Hz) oscillation in reticular thalamic and thalamocortical neurons: scenario of sleep rhythm generation in interacting thalamic and neocortical networks. J Neurosci. 1993;13:3284–99. [PubMed]
81. Ching S, Cimenser A, Purdon P, Brown EN, Kopell NK. Thalamocortical model for a propofol-induced alpha rhythm associated with loss of consciousness. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010 (in press) [PubMed]
82. Steriade M, Amzica F, Contreras D. Cortical and thalamic cellular correlates of electroencephalographic burst-suppression. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1994;90:1–16. [PubMed]
83. Koenig MA, Kaplan PW, Thakor NV. Clinical neurophysiologic monitoring and brain injury from cardiac arrest. Neurol Clin. 2006;24:89–106. [PubMed]
84. Stecker MM, Cheung AT, Pochettino A, et al. Deep hypothermic circulatory arrest: I. Effects of cooling on electroencephalogram and evoked potentials. Ann Thorac Surg. 2001;71:14–21. [PubMed]
85. Yamatogi Y, Ohtahara S. Early-infantile epileptic encephalopathy with suppression-bursts, Ohtahara syndrome: its overview referring to our 16 cases. Brain Dev. 2002;24:13–23. [PubMed]
86. Maksimow A, Särkelä M, Långsjö JW, et al. Increase in high frequency EEG activity explains the poor performance of EEG spectral entropy monitor during S-ketamine anesthesia. Clin Neurophysiol. 2006;117:1660–8. [PubMed]
87. Tsuda N, Hayashi K, Hagihira S, Sawa T. Ketamine, an NMDA-antagonist, increases the oscillatory frequencies of alpha-peaks on the electroencephalographic power spectrum. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. 2007;51:472–81. [PubMed]
88. Blumenfeld H, Taylor J. Why do seizures cause loss of consciousness? Neuroscientist. 2003;9:301–10. [PubMed]
89. Seamans J. Losing inhibition with ketamine. Nat Chem Biol. 2008;4:91–3. [PubMed]
90. Olney JW, Newcomer JW, Farber NB. NMDA receptor hypofunction model of schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res. 1999;33:523–33. [PubMed]
91. Reboso Morales JA, Ketamine González Miranda F. Rev Esp Anestesiol Reanim. 1999;46:111–22. (In Spanish.) [PubMed]
92. Oye I, Paulsen O, Maurset A. Effects of ketamine on sensory perception: evidence for a role of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1992;260:1209–13. [PubMed]
93. Sinner B, Graf BM. Ketamine. Handb Exp Pharmacol. 2008;182:313–33. [PubMed]
94. Lydic R, Baghdoyan HA. Ketamine and MK-801 decrease acetylcholine release in the pontine reticular formation, slow breathing, and disrupt sleep. Sleep. 2002;25:617–22. [PubMed]
95. Giacino JT, Kalmar K, Whyte J. The JFK Coma Recovery Scale–Revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2004;85:2020–9. [PubMed]
96. Monk TG, Weldon BC, Garvan CW, et al. Predictors of cognitive dysfunction after major noncardiac surgery. Anesthesiology. 2008;108:18–30. [PubMed]
97. Kelz MB, Sun Y, Chen J, et al. An essential role for orexins in emergence from general anesthesia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:1309–14. [PubMed]
98. Franks NP. General anaesthesia: from molecular targets to neuronal pathways of sleep and arousal. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:370–86. [PubMed]
99. Ren J, Ding X, Funk GD, Greer JJ. Ampakine CX717 protects against fentanyl-induced respiratory depression and lethal apnea in rats. Anesthesiology. 2009;110:1364–70. [PubMed]
100. Watson SL, Watson CJ, Baghdoyan HA, Lydic R. Thermal nociception is decreased by hypocretin-1 and an adenosine A1 receptor agonist microinjected into the pontine reticular formation of Sprague Dawley rat. J Pain. 2010;11:535–44. [PMC free article] [PubMed]