PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptHHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
Neurology. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 6.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC3071795
NIHMSID: NIHMS232388

Patients with MCI and N400 or P600 abnormalities are at very high risk for conversion to dementia

Abstract

Objective

We sought cognitive event-related potential (ERP) biomarkers of disease progression and subsequent conversion to dementia in mild cognitive impairment (MCI).

Background

Two ERP components, the P600 and N400, are sensitive to abnormal episodic/declarative memory and semantic processing. When congruous category-exemplars are repeated, smaller P600s (relative to initial presentation) are normally elicited. Repetitions of semantically incongruous words yield smaller N400 amplitude. In mild Alzheimer disease (AD), abnormalities of both the N400 and P600 repetition effects are present, suggesting a wide-spread failure of synaptic plasticity.

Methods

Patients with amnestic MCI (n = 32) were longitudinally studied annually with an ERP paradigm in which semantically congruous (50%) and incongruous target words are repeated 10 to 140 seconds after initial presentation. ERP data were analyzed to contrast MCI-to-AD converters (within 3 years) vs nonconverters, using split-plot analyses of variance.

Results

A statistically significant P600 congruous word repetition effect was found only in the nonconverter group (F = 9.9, p = 0.005 vs MCI converters). This effect correlated with verbal memory measures. Repetition of incongruous words produced a significant N400 amplitude attenuation (across right-hemisphere sites) in nonconverters, but not in converters. Patients with MCI with abnormal/reduced N400 or P600 word repetition effects had an 87 to 88% likelihood of dementia within 3 years while those with normal/spared N400 and P600 repetition effects had only an 11 to 27% likelihood.

Conclusions

Abnormalities of the P600 or N400 in mild cognitive impairment are associated with an increased risk of subsequent conversion to Alzheimer disease (AD). These event-related potential components may offer useful biomarkers for the detection and staging of very early AD.

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI), as originally proposed,1 is considered a transitional stage between normal aging and Alzheimer disease (AD). This is supported by the high rate of conversion1,2 and high prevalence of AD neuropathology.35 The criteria1 require history and objective evidence of memory difficulties, relative sparing of general cognition, and intact activities of daily living.

Cognitive ERPs, comprised of summated EPSPs and IPSPs,6 provide an excellent tool for measuring the timing of synaptic processing. A late positive component (LPC), a.k.a. the P600, has been implicated in both memory encoding and retrieval processes.7,8 In word list recognition experiments, a consistent finding has been larger late positivities (with left parietal maxima) for recognized words.9 In contrast, when semantically predictable words are repeated, P600 amplitudes are generally smaller (relative to initial).10,11 P600 generators have been identified in several regions important for episodic/declarative memory (i.e., hippocampus, entorhinal, cingulate, and ventral prefrontal cortex).1214 We have previously reported severely reduced P600 word repetition effects in circumscribed amnesia10 and mild AD.15

The N400 component may also be related to verbal learning/memory.16 N400 amplitude is proportional to semantic processing load and inversely proportional to semantic expectancy.17,18 Invasive electrophysiology (and magnetoencephalography) reveal N400 generators in the anterior fusiform,19 lateral parahippocampal gyri,19 and the superior temporal sulcus.14,20,21 Our studies of circumscribed amnesia10 showed relatively normal N400 effects (e.g., larger N400s to new than old incongruous words). In contrast, patients with mild AD generally have absent N400 word repetition effects.15

To seek biomarkers/predictors of subsequent conversion to AD, we conducted annual longitudinal ERP assessments in MCI. We hypothesized that 1) patients with MCI with poorer memory or subsequent conversion would show reduced P600 word repetition effects, and 2) MCI converters will exhibit pronounced N400 abnormalities (decreased repetition and congruity effects) as they develop incipient dementia and subtle impairments in semantic/linguistic processing.

METHODS

Design

Individuals with amnestic MCI were studied longitudinally (annually) with a cognitive ERP word-repetition paradigm that reliably elicits and modulates the N400 and P600 components. We studied each patient with MCI with annual ERP and clinical assessments until either conversion to dementia (with one ERP recording performed at or soon after conversion) or dropout.

Participants

Thirty-two patients with MCI (mean age = 74.8) were studied after providing informed consent according to the guidelines of the University of California, San Diego (UCSD) Human Research Protection Program. Participants were recruited primarily from the Shiley-Marcos Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center (ADRC) at UCSD. All were assessed at least annually with the Clinical Dementia Rating scale (CDR),22 neurologic examination, and neuropsychological testing. All participants had laboratory tests screening for treatable causes of cognitive impairment (e.g., B12, TSH), and a neuroimaging study (generally MRI) prior to enrollment, which revealed no cause for their memory deficit. All patients with MCI met Petersen criteria for MCI,1 now termed amnestic MCI,23 and did not meet criteria for dementia (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–IV)24 or AD.25 All had baseline CDR global scores of 0.5. See table 1 for demographic and selected neuropsychological data. Six patients with MCI (two at baseline/year 1) were on cholinergic or other pharmacologic treatments for AD during the interval of this report. Exclusion criteria included stroke, epilepsy, CNS-active medications (e.g., benzodiazepines), or neuropsychiatric conditions associated with cognitive deficits.

Table 1
Demographics and behavioral data in mild cognitive impairment groups

National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke–Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association criteria25 for probable/possible AD were used to define conversion, independent of, and blinded to, the ERP data. Thus, decline in two or more cognitive domains, and functional decline (per CDR,22 ADCS-MCI ADL, or FAQ26) were both required for conversion. Sixteen patients with MCI subsequently converted to AD within 3 years of their baseline ERP (mean interval to AD diagnosis = 1.6 ± 0.8 years).

The MCI stable group have all remained in the MCI stage for ≥3 years (mean follow-up = 4.9 ± 1.5 years). Six participants with MCI are not classified as either converters or stable due to either having ≤3 years of follow-up or technically inadequate ERP data.

ERP data from 14 elderly (mean age = 74.0) normal controls (NC), primarily from the ADRC at UCSD, are also in this report. Their baseline ERP data were published in a cross-sectional study,27 which included 11 patients with MCI in this report.

ERP paradigm/procedure

Between 19 and 32 channel EEG was recorded at 250 Hz (bandpass = 0.16 to 100 Hz, re-referenced off-line to linked mastoids10) while subjects performed a semantic judgment task. Auditory category statements were followed (~1 second later) by a visual target word (duration = 300 msec, visual angle 0.4°). After a 3-second pause, participants read the target word followed by yes/no, indicating if it fit the preceding category. Three blocks of 144 trials were recorded, each lasting 20 minutes.

The details of the experimental stimuli have been published previously.10 Half of the target words were semantically congruous (medium-typicality exemplars) and half were incongruous; half were new and half were repeats. Repeated targets followed the same category as on first presentation, with intervening time intervals ranging from ~10 to 140 s (1 to 14 trials later).

EEG was recorded from midline (Fz, Cz, Pz), and lateral (F7/F8, T5/T6, O1/O2) sites in the International 10–20 System28 and from sites which approximate Broca area (BL/BR), Wernicke area (WL/WR), and their right-hemisphere homologues, and Brodmann area 41 (41L/41R). ERPs were derived after off-line rejection of trials with eye movement (monitored by electro-oculography), or other artifacts (using a semiautomated computer algorithm).29 For the 26 patients with MCI (converters or stable) 34% of the trials were rejected across years 1 and 2.

ERP data were analyzed to contrast the MCI converters vs stable groups using mixed-design analyses of variance (ANOVA) with the factors of group, timepoint (year 1/year 2), condition (repetition or congruity), and electrode. Follow-up ANOVAs (e.g., done within group or timepoint) were performed where appropriate. The 300 to 550 msec and 500 to 800 msec latency windows, which consistently captured the N400 and P600 potentials across the participants, were chosen to quantify the N400 and P600 amplitude. Two-tailed p values of ≤0.05 (with Greenhouse-Geisser correction where appropriate) were considered significant.

RESULTS

Behavioral data

Both MCI subgroups performed near ceiling on the category decision task (table 1). ANOVA of the accuracy data (group × congruity × timepoint) showed a significant effect of timepoint (F = 7.26, p = 0.01), indicating a small decline in performance from year 1 to 2. There was no main effect of group or group × timepoint interaction.

ERP analyses

Congruous words: P600 repetition effects

In normal elderly subjects, repeated target words elicited smaller P600s than did their initial presentation (figure 1). A similar decrement of the P600 can be appreciated in the MCI stable group’s grand average ERPs for both year 1 and year 2. However, note that their congruous word repetition effect (shaded area) had a somewhat later onset latency than NC. The MCI converters, in contrast, show no P600 congruous word repetition effect: new and repeated target words elicited identical P600s at both year 1 and year 2 (when n = 6 had developed probable AD and n = 10 were still MCI).

Figure 1
Grand average event related potentials (ERPs) for normal old, mild cognitive impairment (MCI) stable, and MCI converter groups elicited by new (solid lines) and repeated (dotted lines) semantically congruous words at year 1 (left side) and year 2 (right ...

The longitudinal analysis of the P600 amplitude in MCI showed a main effect of repetition [F(1,24) = 16.1, p = 0.0005] with larger P600s to new than old target words across MCI participants. A group × repetition interaction was present [F(1,24) = 9.96, p = 0.005 across all channels; F = 18.4, p = 0.0002 across midline sites] indicating the MCI stable group had larger P600 repetition effects than MCI converters. Within group ANOVAs showed the MCI stable group had a significant P600 repetition effect [F(1,9) = 18.4, p = 0.002], while the MCI converters did not [F(1,15) = 0.27, p = 0.61].

Incongruous words: N400 repetition effects

In normal elderly, larger N400s (starting at 300 msec, peaks at 500 msec) were elicited by new than repeated incongruous words (figure 2). The MCI stable and MCI converters had similar N400 repetition effects at year 1, somewhat smaller than in NC, especially for the MCI converters (compare columns 1 and 3 in left side of figure 2). By year 2, the MCI converters group showed a complete loss of the N400 repetition effect, while the MCI stable group ERP resembled those of year 1.

Figure 2
Grand average event related potentials (ERPs) for normal old, mild cognitive impairment (MCI) stable, and MCI converter groups elicited by new (solid lines) and repeated (dotted lines) semantically incongruous words at year 1 (left side) and year 2 (right ...

The longitudinal analysis of the N400 amplitude showed a significant group × electrode interaction [F(14,336) = 4.2, p = 0.009] indicating the MCI stable group had more posteriorly distributed N400s than the MCI converters, who had an atypically anterior distribution (especially at year 2, see top right of figure 2). There was a marginal group × repetition interaction [F(1,24) = 3.14, p = 0.089] due to a relatively larger N400 repetition effect in the MCI stable group. Within-group ANOVAs showed a significant N400 repetition effect of the MCI stable group across the right hemisphere [F(1,9) = 9.47, p = 0.01], which was of marginal significance across all channels [F(1,9) = 4.33, p = 0.067]. By contrast, the MCI converters had a nonsignificant N400 repetition effect [F(1,15) = 0.30, p = 0.59 across the right hemisphere; F(1,15) F = 0.29, p = 0.60 across all channels]. Within-group ANOVA of the NC group (year 1 data) showed a highly significant effect of repetition [F(1,13) = 7.55, p = 0.016 across all channels]. The N400 repetition effect in MCI converters was slightly delayed and spatially restricted to far posterior channels (see year 1 topographies in figure 2).

New words: N400 congruity effects

The difference between the ERPs elicited by initial presentations of congruous vs incongruous words defines the N400 congruity effect. Semantically incongruous words elicited larger N400s than congruous words in all groups. The N400 congruity effect is normally maximal in right posterior channels (see the NC grand average ERP in figure e-1 on the Neurology® Web site at www.neurology.org). The MCI stable group’s N400 effect had a similar distribution, but was prolonged in duration (persists after 550 msec, unshaded in figure e-1). In year 1, the MCI converter group ERP resembled the MCI stable group. However, in year 2, the MCI converters showed a decreased N400 congruity effect (mainly due to increased N400 amplitude to congruous words).

ANOVA of the N400 amplitude to new words showed a group × congruity interaction [F(1,24) = 4.69, p = 0.04] with a larger congruity effect in MCI stable than in MCI converters. Within-group ANOVAs showed the MCI stable group had an N400 congruity effect [F(1,9) = 18.59, p = 0.002 across all channels], which was significant at year 1 (p = 0.004) and year 2 (p = 0.002). The MCI converters had a N400 congruity effect at year 1 [F(1,15) = 5.32, p = 0.036] but not across years 1 and 2 [F(1,15) = 2.88, p = 0.11].

Correlations between ERP word repetition effects and memory

Correlational analyses of the P600 repetition effect (new – old words, mean voltage difference at Pz, 500 to 800 msec) within MCI showed significant correlations with all the main CVLT measures of verbal learning and memory (r in 0.42 to 0.68 range, table 2). Very similar results were found for correlations with the P600 repetition effect at the vertex (Cz). The P600 repetition effect correlated with the CDR sum of boxes but not with the Mini-Mental State Examination score. In contrast, the N400 repetition effect (amplitude at T6) did not significantly correlate with verbal learning/memory or global measures. Educational level was inversely correlated with both P600 and N400 repetition effect amplitudes at year 1, suggesting that patients with MCI and higher education level might have more advanced AD pathology while still in the MCI stage.30

Table 2
Correlations of word repetition effects with verbal memory and global measures in mild cognitive impairment (year 1)

Prediction value/classification

Figure 3 plots the N400 and P600 repetition effect amplitudes at year 1 (dashed lines = 10th percentile cutoffs in normal elderly). Patients with MCI with either an abnormal N400 or P600 word repetition effect had a higher likelihood of progressing to AD within 3 years (positive predictive value [PPV] = 87% to 88%; table 3). Conversely, patients with MCI with normal N400 and P600 word repetition effects had a low likelihood of converting (PPV = 11 to 27% depending on which criteria were applied). The sensitivity of the ERP measures was high in MCI converters (81 to 94%), with relatively high specificity (79 to 86% in normal elderly). However, the sensitivity was only fair (58 to 65%) when applied to all patients with MCI. Between 81 and 89% of the MCI subjects could be correctly classified as converters vs stable by these ERP criteria. For comparison, the main CVLT measures classified between 65 and 77% (e.g., 20/26 with long delay cued recall) of patients with MCI correctly.

Figure 3
N400 and P600 word repetition effects (mean amplitude difference to new vs old words, in microvolts, at year 1)
Table 3
Sensitivity, specificity, positive predictive value, and discrimination for event-related potential criteria (abnormal vs normal)

DISCUSSION

These data demonstrate that in patients with amnestic MCI, abnormal ERP word repetition effects (P600 or N400) confer a high risk of conversion to AD. A total of 81% of the MCI converters had abnormal (<10th percentile) ERP word repetition effects at year 1. Over 80% of participants with MCI with abnormal ERP word repetition effects at baseline declined and satisfied criteria for dementia within 3 years.

The longitudinal group analyses showed that P600 abnormalities were already pronounced at baseline in MCI converters, with P600 repetition effects already near floor. In contrast, the MCI stable group demonstrated a P600 repetition effect, but with delayed latency relative to NC. Across MCI participants, smaller P600 word repetition effect amplitudes correlated with poorer verbal memory and advanced disease stage (CDR sum of boxes). This is consistent with many previous studies which have linked the P600 to declarative memory.31,32

In contrast to what we observed in patients with chronic amnesia (e.g., Korsakoff syndrome), MCI converters also showed abnormal N400 effects (loss of both the N400 repetition effect and N400 congruity effect). These abnormalities were present in some MCI converters at year 1, and became pronounced by year 2. Thus, the N400 (an index of semantic memory accessibility) appears sensitive to the transition from MCI to AD, when deficits in non-memory cognitive domains (e.g., language, problem solving) emerge. We believe these N400 abnormalities in MCI likely reflect subtle dysfunction of semantic memory processes. It is also noteworthy that the MCI converters show an atypically anterior N400 distribution, similar to mild AD.15

We are only aware of one published longitudinal ERP study of MCI, in which three annual auditory P300 assessments were obtained in elderly subjects with memory complaints.33 Thirty of these subjects received a final clinical diagnosis (2 years later) of MCI and 28 had a final diagnosis of AD. While P300 latency was delayed at baseline and 24 months in AD, no significant P300 abnormalities were found in MCI. In contrast, a cross-sectional study34 found a 30 msec delay of the auditory P300 latency in MCI. Here we focus on ERP repetition effects, not just the latency or amplitude of specific components.

We believe that cognitive ERPs deserve further consideration as a biomarker for very early AD. Specifically, the loss of ERP word repetition effects is very common in early AD15 and usually precedes dementia in MCI. ERPs have limited spatial resolution, but offer unsurpassed temporal resolution and several practical advantages (e.g., inexpensive, noninvasive). The well-recognized sensitivity of ERPs to subtle changes in speed of cognitive processing also supports their candidacy as a cost-effective biomarker for AD.

Current basic research suggests that amyloid oligomers are a strong candidate for being the initiating and causative factor for the amnesia characteristic of AD.35,36 Some animal AD models display impaired synaptic plasticity prior to the appearance of A-β containing plaques and appreciable brain atrophy.36,37 Thus, ERP word repetition effects could prove to be more sensitive to very early AD than volumetric MRI. Interestingly, the sensitivity of our ERP measures to MCI appears comparable to recent amyloid imaging studies (with PIB-PET) of MCI.38,39 Further research that concurrently measures these biomarkers in MCI cohorts is needed to test their relative sensitivity and to determine which measures provide complementary vs redundant information.

Supplementary Material

figure e-1

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

The authors thank Ralph Nowacki, Shaunna Morris, Lauren Keats, the Shiley-Marcos UCSD ADRC, and the late Leon Thal.

Supported by NIH grants R01 AG18442, R01 AG08313, and P50 AG05131. Marta Kutas was a Lady Davis Fellow at Hebrew University during the writing of this article.

GLOSSARY

AD
Alzheimer disease
ADRC
Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center
ANOVA
analyses of variance
CDR
Clinical Dementia Rating scale
CVLT
California Verbal Learning Test
ERP
event-related potential
LPC
late positive component
MCI
mild cognitive impairment
MMSE
Mini-Mental State Examination
PPV
positive predictive value
UCSD
University of California, San Diego.

Footnotes

Supplemental data at www.neurology.org

Disclosure: The authors report no conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

1. Petersen R, Smith G, Waring S, Ivnik R, Tangalos E, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 1999;56:303–308. [PubMed]
2. Grundman M, Petersen R, Morris J, et al. Rate of dementia of the Alzheimer type (DAT) in subjects with mild cognitive impairment [abstract] Neurology. 1996;46 suppl:A403.
3. Galvin J, Powlishta K, Wilkins K, et al. Predictors of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease and dementia: a clinicopathologic study. Arch Neurol. 2005;62:758–765. [PubMed]
4. Morris J, Storandt M, Miller J, et al. Mild cognitive impairment represents early-stage Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol. 2001;58:397–405. [PubMed]
5. Mufson E, Ma S, Dills J, et al. Loss of basal forebrain P75ntr immunoreactivity in subjects with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. J Comp Neurol. 2002;443:136–153. [PubMed]
6. Nunez P. Physical principles and neurophysiological mechanisms underlying event-related potentials. New York: Oxford University Press; 1990.
7. Paller K. Recall and stem-completion priming have different electrophysiological correlates and are modified differentially by directed forgetting. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1990;16:1021–1032. [PubMed]
8. Paller K, Kutas M, Mayes A. Neural correlates of encoding in an incidental learning paradigm. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1987;67:360–371. [PubMed]
9. Tendolkar I, Rugg M. Electrophysiological dissociation of recency and recognition memory. Neuropsychologia. 1998;36:477–490. [PubMed]
10. Olichney J, Van Petten C, Paller K, Salmon D, Iragui V, Kutas M. Word repetition in amnesia: electrophysiological measures of impaired and spared memory. Brain. 2000;123:1948–1963. [PubMed]
11. Van Petten C, Kutas M, Kluender R, Mitchiner M, McIsaac H. Fractionating the word repetition effect with event-related potentials. J Cogn Neurosci. 1991;3:131–150. [PubMed]
12. Fernández G, Effern A, Grunwald T, et al. Real-time tracking of memory formation in the human rhinal cortex and hippocampus. Science. 1999;285:1582–1585. [PubMed]
13. Guillem F, Rougier A, Claverie B. Short- and long-delay intracranial ERP repetition effects dissociate memory systems in the human brain. J Cogn Neurosci. 1999;11:437–458. [PubMed]
14. Halgren E, Baudena P, Heit G, Clarke J, Marinkovic K, Clarke M. Spatio-temporal stages in face and word processing. 1. Depth recorded potentials in the human occipital and parietal lobes. J Physiology. 1994;88:1–50. [PubMed]
15. Olichney J, Iragui V, Salmon D, Riggins B, Morris S, Kutas M. Absent event-related potential (ERP) word repetition effects in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Neurophysiol. 2006;117:1319–1330. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
16. Helmstaedter C, Grunwald T, Lehnertz K, Gleibner U, Elger C. Differential involvement of left temporolateral and temporomesial structures in verbal declarative learning and memory: Evidence from temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain Cogn. 1997;35:110–131. [PubMed]
17. Chwilla D, Brown C, Hagoort P. The N400 as a function of the level of processing. Psychophysiology. 1995;32:274–285. [PubMed]
18. Kutas M, Van Petten C, Kluender R. Psycholinguistics electrified II. In: Gernsbacher MA, Traxler M, editors. Handbook of Psycholinguistics. New York: Elsevier Press; 2005.
19. McCarthy G, Nobre A, Bentin S, Spencer D. Language-related field potentials in the anterior-medial temporal lobe: 1. Intracranial distribution and neural generators. J Neurosci. 1995;15:1080–1089. [PubMed]
20. Halgren E, Dhond R, Christensen N, Connolly J. N400-like magnetoencephalography responses modulated by semantic context, word frequency, and lexical class in sentences. NeuroImage. 2002;17:1101–1116. [PubMed]
21. Helenius P, Salmelin R, Service E, et al. Distinct time courses of word and context comprehension in the left temporal cortex. Brain. 1998;121:1133–1144. [PubMed]
22. Hughes C, Berg L, Danziger W, Coben L, Martin R. A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry. 1982;140:566–572. [PubMed]
23. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004;256:183–194. [PubMed]
24. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2000.
25. McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan E. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology. 1984;34:939–944. [PubMed]
26. Pfeffer R, Kurosaki T, Harrah C, Jr, Chance J, Filos S. Measurement of functional activities in older adults in the community. J Gerontol. 1982;37:323–329. [PubMed]
27. Olichney J, Morris S, Ochoa C, et al. Abnormal verbal event-related potentials in mild cognitive impairment and incipient AD. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002;73:377–384. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
28. Jasper H. Report to the committee on methods of clinical examination in electroencephalography. Appendix: The ten-twenty system of the International Federation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1958;10:371–375.
29. Iragui VJ, Kutas JM, Mitchiner M, Hillyard SA. Effects of aging on event-related brain potentials and reaction times in an auditory oddball task. Psychophysiology. 1993;30:10–22. [PubMed]
30. Stern Y, Alexander G, Prohovnik I, Mayeux M. Inverse relationship between education and parietotemporal perfusion deficit in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol. 1992;32:371–375. [PubMed]
31. Paller K, Kutas M. Brain potentials during memory retrieval provide neurophysiological support for the distinction between conscious recollection and priming. J Cogn Neurosci. 1992;4:375–391. [PubMed]
32. Rugg M, Mark R, Walla P, Schloerscheidt A, Birch C, Allan K. Dissociation of the neural correlates of implicit and explicit memory. Nature. 1998;392:595–598. [PubMed]
33. Gironell A, García-Sánchez C, Estévez-González A, Boltes A, Kulisevsky J. Usefulness of P300 in subjective memory complaints: a prospective study. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2005;22:279–284. [PubMed]
34. Golob E, Johnson J, Starr A. Auditory event-related potentials during target detection are abnormal in mild cognitive impairment. Clin Neurophysiol. 2001;113:151–161. [PubMed]
35. Selkoe D. Alzheimer’s disease is a synaptic failure. Science. 2002;298:789–791. [PubMed]
36. Lesné S, Koh M, Kotilinek L, et al. A specific amyloid-beta protein assembly in the brain impairs memory. Nature. 2006;440:352–357. [PubMed]
37. Rowan M, Klyubin I, Cullen W, Anwyl R. Synaptic plasticity in animal models of early Alzheimer’s disease. Phil Trans Soc Lond B. 2003;358:821–828. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
38. Kemppainen NM, Aalto S, Wilson IA, et al. PET amyloid [11C]PIB uptake is increased in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2007;68:1603–1606. [PubMed]
39. Rowe CC, Ng S, Ackermann U, et al. Imaging beta-amyloid burden in aging in dementia. Neurology. 2007;68:1718–1725. [PubMed]