PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptNIH Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
Neuroimage. Author manuscript; available in PMC Jan 15, 2012.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC2997191
NIHMSID: NIHMS247958
Differential effects of age and history of hypertension on regional brain volumes and iron
Karen M. Rodrigue,1 E. Mark Haacke,2 and Naftali Raz3
1 School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX, 75235
2 Department of Radiology, Wayne State University School of Medicine, Detroit, MI, 48202
3 Institute of Gerontology and Department of Psychology, Wayne State University, Detroit, MI, 48202
fax508-256-5689 tel. 313-577-2297 ; nraz/at/wayne.edu
Aging affects various structural and metabolic properties of the brain. However, associations among various aspects of brain aging are unclear. Moreover, those properties and associations among them may be modified by age-associated increase in vascular risk. In this study, we measured volume of brain regions that vary in their vulnerability to aging and estimated local iron content via T2* relaxometry. In 113 healthy adults (19–83 years old), we examined prefrontal cortex (PFC), primary visual cortex (VC), hippocampus (HC), entorhinal cortex (EC), caudate nucleus (Cd), and putamen (Pt). In some regions (PFC, VC, Cd, Pt) age-related differences in iron and volume followed similar patterns. However, in the medial temporal structures, volume and iron content exhibited different age trajectories. Whereas age-related volume reduction was mild in HC and absent in EC, iron content evidenced significant age-related declines. In hypertensive participants significantly greater iron content was noted in all examined regions. Thus, iron content as measured by T2* may be a sensitive index of regional brain aging and may reveal declines that are more prominent than gross anatomical shrinkage.
Keywords: MRI, aging, brain, iron, vascular risk, susceptibility weighted imaging
Aging, even in healthy individuals, is associated with significant brain changes. One of the best established changes in the aging brain is differential brain shrinkage that is prominent in the polymodal association cortices, striatum, and cerebellum and minimal in the primary sensory cortices (Fjell et al., 2009, Pfefferbaum et al., 1998; Raz et al., 2005; for a review see Raz & Rodrigue, 2006; Resnick et al., 2003). However, age effects on the brain are not limited to gross neuroanatomy but appear at multiple levels of analyses, including neurochemistry, vascular function, and neural connectivity (Bäckman et al., 2006; Hedden & Gabrieli, 2004; Madden et al., 2009; see Raz and Kennedy, 2009 for reviews). Furthermore, all aspects of brain aging are sensitive to numerous physiological, environmental, and genetic factors. The relationship among structural and metabolic aspects of brain aging and the role of physiological and neurochemical modifiers remains unclear.
One of the most prominent metabolic differences associated with brain aging is alteration of iron homeostasis (Andrews & Schmidt, 2007). Although indispensable in diverse physiological and neurochemical processes, iron is a potent agent of oxidative stress and neurodegeneration (Zecca et al., 2004). In the brain, iron is present in several forms. Ferric heme iron in the hemoglobin molecule makes deoxyhemoglobin paramagnetic (Pauling & Coryell, 1936) and creates local inhomogeneity detectable through magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) via change in T2* relaxation times (Ogawa et al., 1990). Thus, differences in T2* can indicate heme iron content. In addition to heme iron in blood that dynamically alters T2*, the brain contains non-heme iron predominantly sequestered in ferritin (Schenk & Zimmerman, 2004). Brain distribution of non-heme iron is uneven and its highest concentration is in the motor nuclei (Hallgren & Sourander, 1958; Haacke et al., 2005). Regional non-heme iron content increases with age (Hallgren & Sourander, 1958; Schenk & Zimmerman, 2004), and in the aging brain there is another contributor of non-heme iron that can alter T2*: iron-rich amyloid plaques (Collingwood et al., 2008; Quintana et al., 2006).
In a comparison of adults with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and mild cognitive impairment to normal controls, Rombouts, Scheltens, Kuijer, & Barkhof (2007) found significant reduction of T2* in the hippocampus, posterior cingulate gyrus and precuneus, parietal lobe, insula and putamen of AD patients. However, due to image filtering and smoothing, the validity of regional interpretation in this study is unclear. Recently, Sullivan and her colleagues used field dependent relaxation rate measures of T2* in a small sample of older adults, and observed significant associations between the T2* values in the basal ganglia and thalamus and cognitive and motor task performance (Sullivan, et al., 2009). Notably, the regions examined in that study included neither neocortex nor hippocampus.
Few studies applied T2* relaxometry to investigation of the aging brain (Haacke et al., 2005). In early attempts, age-related reduction in T2* signal was observed in the entorhinal cortex (EC) of older adults (Small et al., 1999; 2002), and interpreted as reduction of resting metabolism. However, in those studies, signal intensity, not true T2* values, were measured from a single slice. Other studies found age-related declines in T2* in superior frontal gyrus, hippocampus and entorhinal cortex (Raz et al., 2007a), striatum (Siemonsen, et al. 2008; Cherubini, et al., 2009; Haacke et al., in press), globus pallidus (Aquino et al., 2009; Pfefferbaum et al., 2009), and the thalamus (Haacke et al., in press). Although T2* may not be the ideal in vivo index of brain iron (Haacke et al., 2005), it is nonetheless a sufficiently robust correlate of iron content (Ordidge et al., 1994; Martin et al., 1998; 2008, Péran et al., 2009; Duyn et al., 2007; Yao et al., 2009; Shmueli et al., 2009). Although R2* (=1/T2*) is the sum of spin-spin relaxation rates, R2 (=1/T2), and the inhomogeneity-induced relaxation rate R2′ (=1/T2′), in regions with very little myelin the reciprocal of R2* but not R2, correlate highly with iron concentration across a wide range of field strengths (Péran et al., 2009; Yao et al., 2009). From these data, one can conclude that shortening of T2* is a good proxy for increased iron content. Despite this good correlation, others have suggested that the addition of phase information improves estimates of iron concentration in the basal ganglia (Pfefferbaum et al., 2009). Moreover, the correlations between age and T2* in the basal ganglia is driven almost entirely by iron-sensitive T2′ and not by T2 (Siemonsen et al., 2008).
An important factor not addressed in the reviewed studies of T2* in the aging brain, is the influence of vascular risk. Hypertension, one of the most common vascular risk factors, negatively affects brain structure (Burgmans et al., 2010; DeCarli et al., 1995; Giannaros et al., 2006; Raz, et al., 2003; Salerno et al., 1992). It is also linked to cerebral microbleeds (CMB), which contain paramagnetic hemosiderin (Harrison & Arosio, 1996; Viswanathan & Chabriat, 2006). Thus, hypertension may have an additional impact on T2* beyond the effects of aging.
In summary, prior studies suggest that through T2* relaxometry, regional differences in heme and non-heme iron content can be detected in normal aging and age-related cognitive disorders. However, regional distribution of T2* values, their relation to age and to structural properties affected by aging, and the role of vascular risk in those associations remain unclear. The goals of the present study were threefold. First, we investigated age differences in iron content indexed by T2* values across several brain regions that differ in their sensitivity to aging. Second, we compared the pattern of age differences in local T2* values to the pattern of volume shrinkage in the corresponding regions and evaluated the relationship between two types of indices of brain aging. Third, we sought to determine whether a vascular risk factor such as hypertension could affect the strength of association between age and regional indices of iron content.
2.1 Participants
The sample consisted of 113 participants recruited through media advertisements and paid for their participation. About 61% of the participants were from a previous preliminary study (Raz et al., 2007a). Persons with a history of cardiovascular, neurological or psychiatric disease, use of anti-seizure medication, anxiolytics, or antidepressants, head trauma with loss of consciousness for more than five minutes, thyroid problems, diabetes mellitus, drug and alcohol problems were excluded from the study. Persons with metal implants and dental prostheses that could affect image quality and T2* values were not included in the study. Participants had at least high school education, were native English speakers and consistent right-handers with Edinburgh Handedness Questionnaire score of at least 75 (Oldfield, 1971). To screen for dementia and depression we used the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE, Folstein, et al., 1975, a cut-off of 26), and Center for Epidemiology Studies Depression Scale (CES-D, Radloff, 1977; a cut-off of 15). See Table 1 for sample demographics.
Table 1
Table 1
Demographic characteristics of the sample and comparison of men and women.
Seventeen participants who reported a diagnosis of hypertension were taking antihypertensive medications: calcium channel blockers – one participant; angiotensin-converting enzyme inhibitors – two; angiotensin receptor II antagonist – one; beta-blockers – two; potassium-sparing diuretics – three; and eight participants took a combination of at least two of these medications. The hypertensive participants were significantly older than their normotensive peers (61.65 years vs. 52.59 years), t= −3.11, p = .003, but had similar education level (t= −1.18, ns) and MMSE (t (24) = −.38, ns). Mean systolic blood pressure for the hypertensive participants (137.85 mm Hg) exceeded that of the normotensive participants (126.71 mm Hg, t = 2.99, p < .005), whereas diastolic pressure did not (81.09 mm Hg vs. 77.88 mm Hg, t = 1.91, p = .07). All participants provided written informed consent at the beginning of the study and were debriefed after its completion.
2.2 Procedure
Blood Pressure Measurement
Blood pressure was measured by a mercury sphygmomanometer (Country Technology, Inc., Gays Mills, WI, model 12–525) with a brachial cuff on three separate occasions in participants seated in a comfortable chair in a climate-controlled room. The values were averaged across occasions.
MRI acquisition protocol
Images were acquired on a 1.5 Tesla Siemens Sonata scanner. Regional T2* measurements were obtained from a multi-echo 3D Gradient-Recalled Echo (GRE) sequence with 48 axial slices, echo time (TE) = 10, 20, 30, 40, 50, 60, 70, 80 ms; repetition time (TR) = 100 ms; field of view (FOV) = 256 × 256 mm2; bandwidth (BW) =170 Hz/pixel, flip angle (FA) = 30°, and voxel size= 1 × 1 × 2 mm3. Thus, the 8-echo data were collected in a single high-BW sequence, which further reduced problems associated with motion. Even-number echoes were flow-compensated in the read direction. Sequence duration was 15:22 min. For regional volumetry we acquired a magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MP-RAGE) sequence with 144 coronal slices, TE = 3.93 ms, TR = 800 ms, inversion time (TI) = 420 ms, FOV = 192 mm × 192 mm, BW= 130 Hz/pixel, acquisition matrix = 256 × 256, FA = 20°, and voxel size = 0.75 × 0.75 × 1.5 mm3. An 8-channel head coil was used for all sequences, thus considerably improving signal to noise ratio.
T2* Relaxometry
The raw data from the multi-echo GRE sequences were analyzed with locally designed software (SPIN; Signal Processing in NMR; Detroit). Images were sorted by echo time. T2* maps were created by fitting the logarithm of the data for each echo on a pixel-by-pixel basis. We used a linear fit on the logarithm of the data, with the number of echoes varying from pixel to pixel depending on the local signal. To reduce error, the signal from longer echoes that fell below two standard deviations of the noise was excluded from the fitting. The noise was calculated from 0.8 times the mean of the Rayleigh noise distribution.)
T2* values were sampled after interpolating the data by a factor of two in both in-plane dimensions of the transverse plane. The short-echo image (TE=10 ms) was used to anatomically identify the ROIs: hippocampal formation (HC), entorhinal cortex (EC), primary visual (calcarine) cortex (VC), superior frontal gyrus (SFG), caudate (Cd) and putamen (Pt). To ensure that roughly the same number of pixels was sampled from each region, an oval-shaped probe the size of 24–36 contiguous pixels (depending on ROI size) was placed within each ROI on the short TE image and copied onto the T2* map in the identical region to obtain the T2* mean and standard deviation values. To avoid partial volume artifacts, the operator was cautious not to place the sampling probe close to CSF. Test-retest reliability was assessed for one operator (KR) on 10 images (for each ROI) on two separate occasions, one week apart. All test-retest reliabilities (ICC3, Shrout & Fleiss, 1979) equaled or exceeded 0.89. The ROIs were determined with the aid of anatomical atlases (primarily Duvernoy, 1999) and were defined and measured as follows.
The T2* values in the head of the hippocampus (ICC3 = .90) were measured on four contiguous slices from the first slice on which it was apparent in the axial plane. This encompassed the extent of the anterior portion of the HC, which was chosen because of its reported sensitivity to aging (Hackert, et al., 2002). Entorhinal cortex T2* (ICC3 = .91) was measured on three contiguous slices beginning with the first slice of the HC measurement. T2* was measured in the gray matter lining of the calcarine sulcus (primary visual cortex, ICC3 = 0.89) on three contiguous slices, beginning on the last slice where the superior colliculi remained visible. The superior frontal gyrus T2* (ICC3 = 0.96) was measured on six consecutive axial slices beginning on the first slice where the AC-PC line is visible. The T2* values for the caudate nucleus (ICC3 = 0.94) and the putamen (ICC3 = 0.93) were measured on four consecutive slices from the first slices on which they appeared axially. Figure 1 illustrates ROI demarcation. All T2* measurements were obtained from the left and right hemispheres, separately and averaged.
Figure 1
Figure 1
Definition of the regions of interest (ROI), indicated in red. Each ROI was marked on the T1-weighted image (first echo, TE = 10 ms, the a. panel throughout the figure), and copied to the T2* map (indexed as b. in the figure). T2* values that were computed (more ...)
Volumetry
The MP-RAGE images were reformatted to correct for head tilt, pitch, and rotation using the orthogonal tool in Analyze software (Biomedical Imaging Resource, Mayo Clinic College of Medicine). All images were aligned along the anterior and posterior commissure line (see Raz, et al., 2004 for details). The hippocampal measurements were performed on images orthogonal to the long axis of the hippocampal formation. Aligned images were re-sliced into 0.5×0.5×0.5 mm3 contiguous sections.
Images were displayed on a 21″ monitor and magnified 2. Each ROI was traced manually with a stylus on a 21″ LCD digitizing tablet (Wacom Cintiq 21UX), see details in Raz et al., 2004. Six regions of interest (ROI) were manually traced and measured on the coronal plane: HC, EC, Cd, Pt, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC), and VC. All ROI areas were measured with the ROI tool in the Analyze software, and volumes were computed by multiplying the sum of areas by the slice thickness. Intracranial volume (ICV) was used to correct for differences in head and body size via a linear equation: Volumeadji = Volumerawi - b(ICVi - Mean ICV) for each subject i. In this equation, Volumeadji is adjusted ROI volume, Volumerawi is the raw ROI volume, b is the slope of the ROI volume regression on ICV, and Mean ICV is the sample mean of the intracranial volume.
Reliability of the regional volumetric measures was assessed by an intraclass correlation formula that presumes random selection of raters, ICC(2) (Shrout & Fleiss, 1979). Four trained and reliable raters obtained a minimum ICC(2) ≥ 0.90 on each region traced.
ROI Demarcation and Tracing Rules
Prefrontal Cortex (PFC)
The PFC was measured on 24 to 36 coronal slices located within 40% of the distance between the genu of the corpus callosum and the frontal pole. After the first slice, every 3rd slice (1.5mm) is measured resulting in a sampling of 8 to 12 total slices for this ROI. The PFC ROI includes superior, middle, and inferior frontal gyri and covers mainly Brodmann areas 9 and 46, and parts of areas 8, 10 and 45. The superior and inferior boundaries of the PFC are the most dorsomedial point of the cortex and the lateral orbital sulcus, respectively. The reliability of this measure is ICC(2) = 0.97.
Hippocampus (HC)
HC volume was measured on a series of 19 to 25 slices aligned perpendicular to the long axis of the right hippocampus. The mammillary bodies define the rostral boundary of the HC, and the slice showing the fornices rising from the fimbria mark the caudal boundary. Care was taken not to include the amygdala in this ROI. The reliability of this measure is ICC(2) = 0.94.
Entorhinal Cortex (EC)
In tracing this region, the rules developed by Insausti et al. (1998) were followed with minor modifications. The EC was traced on every third slice after the first slice spanning a total of 15–18 0.5mm thick slices (beginning with the first slice on which the anterior commissure is visible and continuing to one slice caudal to the posterior limit of the gyrus intralimbicus. The medial bank of the collateral sulcus defined the ventro-lateral boundary. The reliability of this measure is ICC(2) = .93.
Visual (Calcarine) Cortex (VC)
The volume of the VC was estimated as the volume of the cortical ribbon lining the calcarine sulcus. This sulcus appears as the most ventromedial sulcus in the temporal-occipital cortex on the coronal slice that is mid-vermis or immediately caudal to mid-vermis. It is measured on the anterior 50% of the coronal slices between the mid-vermal slice and the occipital pole. The inferior and superior boundaries of this ROI are defined as the point at which the opening of the sulcus occurs. At this point, a line is drawn horizontally so that no cortex (dorsal or ventral) outside of the calcarine sulcus is included, confining this ROI to BA 17. VC is measured on a total of 15–18 0.5mm slices, sampling every 3rd slice after the initial slice. The reliability of this measure is ICC(2) = 0.94.
Caudate Nucleus (Cd)
The volume of the head and the body of the caudate were estimated from 15–20 coronal slices. The most rostral slice was the one on which the caudate first appeared; usually lateral to the lateral ventricles. On the one to three sections on which the nucleus accumbens was visible, a diagonal line was drawn from the most inferior tip of the internal capsule to the lateral ventricle. The caudate was traced on every other slice (interslice distance 3 mm) until no longer visible. The reliability of this measure was ICC(2) = 0.95.
Putamen (Pt)
The volume of the putamen was measured from 15 to 20 coronal slices (every other slice at the inter-slice distance of 3 mm). The operator began tracing on the most rostral slice on which the putamen was visible and continued to the most caudal slice on which it could be detected. The external capsule demarcated the putamen laterally throughout the ROI. The dorsal boundary of the putamen was the white matter. The internal capsule delineated the medial border until reaching the anterior commissure. Past that point, the globus pallidus became the medial border of the putamen. The limen insulae, optic radiations, the amygdala, the temporal horn, and the anterior commissure served as the ventral boundary of the putamen on various slices. The reliability of this measure was ICC(2) = 0.93.
Intracranial vault (ICV)
The auto-trace function in the Analyze program was used to measure ICV. Measurements were taken from 9 slices across the axial plane, 6 mm apart, which encompasses the majority of the cranium excluding only the base of the brain where the cranium is not yet fully apparent and vertex of the brain. A seed point was selected on the edge of the cranium and the auto trace scale was manually adjusted until the entire cranium is outlined by the Analyze ROI tool. The reliability of this measure is ICC(2) = 0.99. Illustrations of all ROIs are displayed in Figure 1.
Statistical analyses
The data were examined for the undue influence of outliers. The general linear model approach was used to test all hypotheses in this study. Age (centered at the sample mean) was a continuous independent variable, whereas sex and hypertension diagnosis were categorical factors. Region of interest (ROI) was a categorical six-level repeated measures (within-subjects) factor. Volume and T2* were used as the dependent variables in separate analyses. All interactions among the predictors were tested but discarded if found non-significant (using a conservative p > 0.15 criterion), after which reduced models were tested. Significant age effects for ROIs were examined with linear and polynomial regressions. Strength of associations between age and brain variables were compared by testing correlations with Stieger’s test for related correlations (Steiger, 1980).
3.1 Age-related regional differences in T2*
The analysis revealed a significant main effect of age on T2*: F(1, 110) = 93.50, p < 0.001. Advanced age was associated with shorter T2* across the sampled ROIs, with the correlation between age and mean T2* r = 0.68, p < 0.001. No effect of sex or its interaction with other variables was found. The within-subjects main effect of ROI was significant and reflected significant regional differences in T2* values: F(5, 550) = 122.55, p < 0.001.
Notably, a significant Age × ROI interaction, F(5, 550) = 6.28, p < .001, indicated that the magnitude of age differences in T2* varied across the examined regions. Univariate post-hoc tests showed that all regions, except the primary visual cortex, evidenced a significant negative association with age. Table 3 summarizes regression analyses by ROI and scatter plots in Figures 24 depict the association of T2* with age by region. Examination of overlap between confidence intervals of the regression slopes presented in Table 3 shows the steepest age-related decline was found in the hippocampal formation, which differed significantly from all other ROIs except the caudate nucleus. The caudate nucleus and the putamen showed similar age differences, exceeding the effects of age on T2* in prefrontal and entorhinal cortices, which did not differ from each other. Primary visual cortex presented significantly shallower (and nonsignificant) age slopes than all other regions examined in this study.
Table 3
Table 3
Univariate regressions of regional T2* on age
Figure 2
Figure 2
Volume and T2* as a function of age in the hippocampus and the entorhinal cortex.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Regressions of volume and T2* on age in the caudate nucleus and putamen.
To test for nonlinear effects of age on T2*, a quadratic term was added to the regression for each ROI. Analyses revealed a significant quadratic effect only for the superior frontal gyrus T2* (see Table 4). As presented in Figure 3, T2* shortening in that region was apparent from young to middle-age, after which the estimated decline reached a plateau, although an overall increase in variability is apparent in the older adults. Examination of the scatter suggests, however, that the observed nonlinearity is likely to reflect inhomogeneity of variance across the age range with increased variability of T2* observed among the older participants.
Table 4
Table 4
Linear and quadratic effects of age on T2*
Figure 3
Figure 3
Regression of volume and T2* on age in the neocortical regions: prefrontal and occipital. The correlation coefficient for the superior frontal gyrus (a part of the lateral prefrontal cortex) T2* is for the combination of linear and quadratic components. (more ...)
3.2 Vascular risk and age-related T2* differences
Effects of Hypertension Diagnosis
To determine if diagnosis of hypertension affected the age-related differences in T2*, the analyses were first rerun with removal of the 17 diagnosed hypertensive participants. The pattern of results remained unchanged. However, due to the vastly unequal size of hypertensive and normotensive groups and the resulting small statistical power to detect differences between them, the hypertensive participants were matched on age, sex, race, and education to 17 normotensive participants in the study to determine if vascular risk played a role in age-related T2* differences, see Table 5 for details on matching.
Table 5
Table 5
Sample demographics for matched hypertension analyses
Results of the GLM analyses indicated a significant main effect of Hypertension diagnosis, F(1, 30) = 11.45, p < .002, beyond the main effect of age, F(1,30) = 6.36, p = .02. There was no difference among ROIs in the effect of Hypertension: ROI × Hypertension interaction F < 1, ns. The differences in regional T2* means between normotensive and hypertensive groups are displayed below in Figure 5.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Regional T2* differences between hypertensive and normotensive participants: Group means and standard errors of the means.
3.3 Regional brain volume and age
To examine the effect of age on regional brain volume, a mixed general linear model was fitted to the data, with Age, entered as a continuous predictor and Sex and Hypertension entered as categorical predictors. ROIs (HC, EC, PFC, VC, Cd, Pt) formed a six-level repeated-measures factor. Regional brain volume was a dependent variable.
The analyses revealed a significant main effect of Age on volume, F(1, 108) = 28.15, p < .001, i.e., advanced age was associated with smaller regional brain volumes across the ROIs. The main effect of Hypertension was not significant, F < 1. A within-subjects effect of ROI, F(5, 540) = 1336.06, p < .001, and the ROI × Age interaction, F(5, 540) = 10.69, p < .001, were significant, indicating that the association of regional volumes with age varied across the ROIs. There was also a significant main effect of Sex, F(1, 108) = 4.23, p < .05. However, these effects were qualified by a significant ROI × Age × Sex interaction, F(5, 540) = 3.91, p < .002, which indicated that the age differences in regional brain volumes varied across regions and between the sexes. Univariate post-hoc analyses revealed differential effects of age on regional brain volume for women and men, with women showing greater age-related differences in the hippocampal formation, prefrontal cortex and caudate nucleus than men. Men showed no significant age differences in those regions.
To assess the effect of hypertension diagnosis on regional brain volume, we repeated GLM analyses previously conducted on T2* values, in the group of 34 demographically matched hypertensive and normotensive controls. In contrast to the T2* analyses, we found neither group differences by Hypertension diagnosis nor ROI × Hypertension interaction (all F < 1).
3.4 Age-related differences in T2* versus age differences in volume
To test whether age differences in regional volumes and in T2* differed in magnitude, we compared the correlations between each region’s volume and age with age-T2* correlations for the same or equivalent region using Steiger’s Z* statistic (Steiger, 1980). The results show that with the exception of the medial-temporal regions, all ROIs showed equal strength of age differences for T2* and volume. For HC, the association between age and T2* (r = −0.54) was significantly stronger than that between HC volume and age (r = −0.37), whereas for the EC, the difference between a significant association of T2* and age (r = −0.37) and nonsignificant correlation between EC volume and age (r = −0.15) reached only a trend level (p = 0.075).
3.5 Associations between regional T2* and volume
We assessed a possible contribution of regional T2* to explaining age differences in regional brain volumes in a series of univariate analyses. In those analyses, Age, Sex, Hypertension diagnosis and regional T2* served as predictors of the corresponding brain volume for each ROI. The analysis for the entorhinal cortex revealed a significant Age × T2* interaction, F(1, 107) = 3.80, p = 0.05, indicating that only older adults (65 years or older, N = 43) showed a positive association between entorhinal T2* and volume, with smaller volumes corresponding to shorter T2*, t(42)= 2.38, p= 0.02. For other regions, the effect of regional T2* on brain volume depended on sex. In the HC, a trend for Sex × T2* interaction was noted: F(1, 107) = 3.53, p = 0.06. A significant positive association between the hippocampal volume and HC T2*, was observed only for women: F(1, 72) = 4.35, p = .04. In the basal ganglia, Age × Sex × T2* interactions were found for both Cd, F(1, 104) = 4.10, p = .05 and Pt, F(1, 104) = 6.20, p = .01. For Pt, the sex effect was due to an Age × T2* interaction for men only, F(1, 32) = 3.87, p = .06, with older men only showing a positive association between T2* and volume. For the Cd, the interaction was also due to the presence of an Age × T2* effect among men, but not among women. However, when one outlier (a 66 year old male with the largest Cd volume in the sample, 11.09 cm3, Studentized residual = 3.98) was removed the higher order interaction effect became nonsignificant. In the neocortical regions, primary visual and prefrontal, there were no significant contributions of T2* to local volume variance.
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to examine in vivo the relationship between regional brain iron content and age-related differences in regional brain volumes in healthy adults. An excellent correspondence between the T2* values and regional concentrations of non-heme iron (from Table 3, Haacke et al., 2005), supports the validity of T2* as an index of iron content (see Figure 6). Although we observed age-related increase in iron content, the regional pattern of T2* shortening differed from that of age-related shrinkage.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Calculated regional T2* correspond well to concentrations of iron (C) found in postmortem material (medians from multiple studies summarized in Haacke et al., 2005, Table 3). Note, that because there are no reports on the entorhinal cortex iron content, (more ...)
In the neostriatum and the prefrontal cortex, age-related differences in volume and iron content were of similar magnitude, and no age differences were found in the primary visual cortex by either method. In contrast, within medial temporal lobe, age-related differences in T2* exceeded the differences in volume. It is unclear, whether this reflects increased sensitivity of T2* to aging, a response to age-related changes, or an early indication of incipient pathology.
T2* is affected by local content of two types of iron: heme, mainly blood-born, and non-heme, mainly sequestered, in ferritin within glia and endothelium and in transferrin within the neurons (Andrews & Schmidt, 2007; Benarroch, 2009; Haacke, et al., 2005). Thus, the differences in T2* may stem from alterations of cerebral microvascular dynamics and tissue oxygenation and/or from the changes in non-heme iron homeostasis.
Non-heme iron is distributed unevenly throughout the brain (Haacke et al., 2005; Hallgren & Sourander, 1958; Morris et al., 1992), and its regional contents increase with age (Hallgren & Sourander, 1958). Concentration of iron-binding proteins increases with age in the frontal white matter, basal ganglia, and substantia nigra (Bartzokis et al., 2007; Connor et al., 1995; Zecca, et al., 2001). Although, in contrast to the basal ganglia, non-heme iron content in the neocortex is relatively low (Hardy et al., 2005; Ogg, 1999), direct experimental manipulations of iron content show that the influence of deoxyhemoglobin and its heme iron on neocortical T2* is negligible in comparison to that of ferritin, i.e. non-heme iron (Fukunaga et al., 2010; Lee et al., 2010). It is possible, however, that the role of heme iron in cortical T2* increases with age because of changes in cortical vasculature. The findings by several research groups (Asslani et al., 2009; Heo et al., 2010; Small et al, 2004) suggest that for the medial temporal lobe this may be the case. However, because that work was limited to the hippocampal formation and the entorhinal cortex, it is still unclear whether their findings generalize across neocortical regions.
In comparison to the neocortex and the striatum, the medial temporal regions present additional difficulty in interpretation of T2* differences, because the they may contain iron trapped in the amyloid plaques (Bartzokis, et al., 2007; Collingwood et al., 2008; Quintana et al., 2006). Indeed, hippocampal ferritin content is elevated with age (Bartzokis, et al. 2007). Postmortem (Price et al., 2009) and in vivo (Bourgeat et al., 2010; Rowe et al., 2007) studies show significant amyloid load in 22–34% of cognitively normal persons (for review see Rodrigue, et al., 2009). Amyloid burden in the inferior temporal cortex correlates inversely with hippocampal volume (Bourgeat et al., 2010); limbic and neocortical plaque counts obtained postmortem are associated with suppressed memory scores measured before death (Price et al., 2009). It is possible therefore, that differential age effects on T2* of the medial temporal structures reflect elevation in the amyloid plaque load therein. Early accumulation of amyloid plaques involves only the entorhinal and perirhinal cortices, with later proliferation into the hippocampus and subsequent involvement of the neocortical sites (Braak & Braak, 1991, 1997). It is noteworthy in that respect that we observed a significant association between EC volume and T2* only in the older participants, and no such association was found in the neocortical regions that are not expected to contain a significant number of plaques. This pattern suggests the possibility that amyloid plaques in the EC significantly contribute to T2* shortening in that structure. However, only concurrent assessment of brain iron content and amyloid burden in vivo can test that proposition.
The third goal of the current study was to investigate the modifying effect of hypertension on T2*. We found that groups of hypertensive individuals carefully matched to their normotensive peers evidenced comparatively shortened T2* (i.e., greater iron presence) in all examined regions. Thus, hypertension may be associated with increased content of heme and non-heme iron contents beyond the normal effects of age. The addition of a vascular risk factor on top of the normal effects of aging resulted in two distinct findings. First, the hypertensive group displayed shorter T2* than the control group. Second, the differentiated pattern of brain aging (as measured by iron content inferred from T2* measures) was lost in the hypertensive group. Thus, this study replicates and extends previous findings which suggest that vascular risk exacerbates brain aging and is associated with an anterior-to-posterior gradient of regional decline (Raz et al., 2007b), and extends this pattern from the structural level to the level of brain iron homeostasis. Notably, all hypertensive participants were treated well with anti-hypertensive agents. It is unclear, however, what if any effect different antihypertensive medication might have had on brain iron content. This important question should be addressed in future studies with larger number of participants undergoing various types of anti-hypertensive therapy.
Although the presence of non-heme iron may partly explain the effect of hypertension, T2* variations across the regions may also reflect differences in deoxyhemoglobin content in the local vasculature. Hypertension is associated with increased vascular resistance (Seals et al., 2006), and a particular vulnerability to hypoperfusion (Bang et al., 2008; Dai et al., 2008; Nobili et al, 1993) and ischemia (Ay et al., 2005; Baumbach & Heistad, 1988; Yao et al., 1991; Schwartz et al., 2007), which may account for a significant share of the T2* shortening. Although most of the hypertensive participants were taking anti-hypertensive medications that could improve cerebral perfusion (Bertel et al., 1987; Efimova et al., 2008), such improvement was likely to be only partial (Nobili et al., 1993). Notably, the negative effect of hypertension on age-related differences was observed in regional T2* values but not in regional volumes. Thus, it is possible that T2* is a more sensitive index of brain aging than regional volumes and that changes in T2* due to iron accumulation precede local shrinkage.
Finally, age-related shortening of T2* may reflect hemosiderin in CMB. Although CMB are relatively common in the general population of elderly (Greenberg et al., 2009), it is unlikely that many are present in this selective sample. In a sample with less stringent exclusion criteria, only 11% of healthy participants had CMB and none had more than three (Ayaz et al., 2010). Nonetheless, as CMB are more likely in persons with hypertension (Ochi et al., 2009; Sun et al, 2009; Viswanathan & Chabriat, 2006) they could have contributed to the observed T2* differences between hypertensive and normotensive participants. Even in that case, however, CMB would be observed in the regions that, with the exception of neostriatum, were not sampled in this study: deep white matter, thalamus, cerebellum, and brainstem (Kato et al., 2002).
T2* maps used in this study included no phase information and thus cannot distinguish between iron and other inorganic materials, such as calcium (Chavhan et al., 2009). In normal adults, calcification is common in the pineal, choroid plexus, and leptomeninges (Vigh et al., 1998) as well as in the basal ganglia. In this study, only in the latter could calcification confound the effect attributed to iron. In light of the excellent correspondence between regional iron concentration values and the observed T2*, this confound unlikely.
Some previous studies (Siemonsen et al. 2008) indicate that because T2* is a combination of T2 (spin-spin relaxation) and T2′ (inhomogeneity effect), the latter may be a better index of iron content. Because T2 values were not available in this study, we could not compute T2′. However, age differences in T2* and T2′ in gray matter (e.g., striatum) are equivalent (Siemonsen et al., 2008) and it is unlikely that use of T2′ would alter the results.
This study has additional limitations. First, we excluded the participants with many common age-related pathologies and risk factors (e.g., heart disease and diabetes). Thus, our sample is not representative of aging in the general population. Second, the relatively low field strength employed in this study could have been insufficient for detection of more subtle differences in T2* that are now possible with higher-field magnets, which in spite of the added susceptibility artifacts, afford a substantially greater contrast-to-noise ratio (Duyn et al., 2007; Shmueli et al., 2009).
4.1. Conclusions
In conclusion, the results of this study show that regional iron concentration is sensitive index of brain aging. In longitudinal study, in vivo assessment of regional content of brain iron may shed light on the origins of age-related regional brain shrinkage. Moreover, altered iron metabolism may mediate exacerbation of brain aging by vascular risk factors. The mechanisms underpinning the relative contribution of various types of iron to age-related T2* shortening remain to be determined. Developing more sensitive and specific MRI methods of estimating brain iron will allow a better understanding of processes that link structural and metabolic aging of the brain. The results reported here underscore the importance of closely monitoring age-related changes in vascular health, as they can be modified by behavioral and pharmacological means (Nilsson, 2008).
Table 2
Table 2
Pearson correlations among age and regional T2* and the variables’ means and standard deviations
Acknowledgments
The work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (grants R37 AG-011230 to NR, training grant T32 HS-013819 to KR via the Institute of Gerontology), and a Dissertation Award from the American Psychological Association (to KR). We thank Hanzhang Lu for valuable comments.
Footnotes
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
  • Andrews NC, Schmidt PJ. Iron homeostasis. Ann Rev of Physiol. 2007;69:69–85. [PubMed]
  • Aquino D, Bizzi A, Grisoli M, Garavaglia B, Bruzzone MG, Nardocci N, Savoiardo M, Chiapparini L. Age-related iron deposition in the basal ganglia: Quantitative analysis in healthy subjects. Radiology. 2009;252:165–172. [PubMed]
  • Asllani I, Habeck C, Borogovac A, Brown TR, Brickman AM, Stern Y. Separating function from structure in perfusion imaging of the aging brain. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:2927–2935. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Ay H, Koroshetz WJ, Vangel M, Benner T, Melinosky C, Zhu M, Menezes N, Lopez CJ, Sorensen AG. Conversion of ischemic brain tissue into infarction increases with age. Stroke. 2005;36:2632–2636. Epub 2005 Nov 3. [PubMed]
  • Ayaz M, Boikov AS, Haacke EM, Kido DK, Kirsch WM. Imaging cerebral microbleeds using susceptibility weighted imaging: One step toward detecting vascular dementia. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2010;31:142–148. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Bäckman L, Nyberg L, Lindenberger U, Li SC, Farde L. The correlative triad among aging, dopamine, and cognition: current status and future prospects. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30:791–807. [PubMed]
  • Bang OY, Saver JL, Alger JR, Starkman S, Ovbiagele B, Liebeskind DS. UCLA Collateral Investigators. Determinants of the distribution and severity of hypoperfusion in patients with ischemic stroke. Neurology. 2008;71:1804–1811. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Bartzokis G, Tishler TA, Lu PH, Villablanca P, Altshuler LL, Carter M, Huang D, Edwards N, Mintz J. Brain ferritin iron may influence age- and gender-related risks of neurodegeneration. Neurobiol Aging. 2007;28:414–423. [PubMed]
  • Baumbach GL, Heistad DD. Cerebral circulation in chronic arterial hypertension. Hypertension. 1988;12:89–95. [PubMed]
  • Benarroch EE. Brain iron homeostasis and neurodegenerative disease. Neurology. 2009;72:1436–1440. [PubMed]
  • Bertel O, Marx BE, Conen D. Effects of antihypertensive treatment on cerebral perfusion. Am J Med. 1987;82:29–36. [PubMed]
  • Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 1991;82:239–259. [PubMed]
  • Braak H, Braak E. Frequency of stages of Alzheimer-related lesions in different age categories. Neurobiol Aging. 1997;18:351–357. [PubMed]
  • Bourgeat P, Chételat G, Villemagne VL, Fripp J, Raniga P, Pike K, Acosta O, Szoeke C, Ourselin S, Ames D, Ellis KA, Martins RN, Masters CL, Rowe CC, Salvado O. AIBL Research Group. Beta-amyloid burden in the temporal neocortex is related to hippocampal atrophy in elderly subjects without dementia. Neurology. 2010;74:121–127. [PubMed]
  • Burgmans S, van Boxtel MPJ, Gronenschild EHBM, Vuurman EFPM, Hofman P, Uylings HBM, Jolles J, Raz N. Multiple indicators of age-related differences in cerebral white matter and the modifying effects of hypertension. NeuroImage. 2010;49:2083–2093. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Chavhan GB, Babyn PS, Thomas B, Shroff MM, Haacke EM. Principles, techniques, and applications of T2*-based MR imaging and its special applications. Radiographics. 2009;29:1433–1449. [PubMed]
  • Cherubini A, Péran P, Caltagirone C, Sabatini U, Spalletta G. Aging of subcortical nuclei: microstructural, mineralization and atrophy modifications measured in vivo using MRI. Neuroimage. 2009;48:29–36. [PubMed]
  • Collingwood JF, Chong RK, Kasama T, Cervera-Gontard L, Dunin-Borkowski RE, Perry G, Pósfai M, Siedlak SL, Simpson ET, Smith MA, Dobson J. Three-dimensional tomographic imaging and characterization of iron compounds within Alzheimer’s plaque core material. J Alzheimers Dis. 2008;14:235–245. [PubMed]
  • Connor JR, Snyder BS, Arosio P, Loeffler DA, LeWitt P. A quantitative analysis of isoferritins in select regions of aged, parkinsonian, and Alzheimer’s diseased brains. J Neurochem. 1995;65:717–724. [PubMed]
  • Dai W, Lopez OL, Carmichael OT, Becker JT, Kuller LH, Gach HM. Abnormal regional cerebral blood flow in cognitively normal elderly subjects with hypertension. Stroke. 2008;39:349–354. Epub 2008 Jan 3. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • DeCarli C, Murphy DG, Tranh M, Grady CL, Haxby JV, Gillette JA, Salerno JA, Gonzales-Aviles A, Horwitz B, Rapoport SI. The effect of white matter hyperintensity volume on brain structure, cognitive performance, and cerebral metabolism of glucose in 51 healthy adults. Neurology. 1995;45:2077–2084. [PubMed]
  • Duvernoy HM. The Human Brain: Surface, Three-Dimensional Sectional Anatomy with MRI, and Blood Supply. 2. NY: Springer; 1999.
  • Duyn JH, van Gelderen P, Li TQ, de Zwart JA, Koretsky AP, Fukunaga M. High-field MRI of brain cortical substructure based on signal phase. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:11796–11801. [PubMed]
  • Efimova IY, Efimova NY, Triss SV, Lishmanov YB. Brain perfusion and cognitive function changes in hypertensive patients. Hypertens Res. 2008;31:673–678. [PubMed]
  • Fjell AM, Westlye LT, Amlien I, Espeseth T, Reinvang I, Raz N, Agartz I, Salat DH, Greve DN, Fischl B, Dale AM, Walhovd KB. High consistency of regional cortical thinning in aging across multiple samples. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:2001–2012. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiat Res. 1975;12:189–198. [PubMed]
  • Fukunaga M, Li TQ, van Gelderen P, de Zwart JA, Shmueli K, Yao B, Lee J, Maric D, Aronova MA, Zhang G, Leapman RD, Schenck JF, Merkle H, Duyn JH. Layer-specific variation of iron content in cerebral cortex as a source of MRI contrast. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:3834–3839. [PubMed]
  • Gianaros PJ, Greer PJ, Ryan CM, Jennings JR. Higher blood pressure predicts lower regional grey matter volume: Consequences on short-term information processing. Neuroimage. 2006;31:754–765. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Greenberg SM, Vernooij MW, Cordonnier C, Viswanathan A, Al-Shahi Salman R, Warach S, Launer LJ, Van Buchem MA, Breteler MM. Microbleed Study Group. Cerebral microbleeds: a guide to detection and interpretation. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8:165–174. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Haacke EM, Cheng NYC, House MJ, Liu Q, Neelavalli J, Ogg RJ, Khan A, Ayaz M, Kirsch W, Obenaus A. Imaging iron stores in the brain using magnetic resonance imaging. Mag Res Imaging. 2005;23:1–25. [PubMed]
  • Haacke EM, Miao Y, Liu M, Habib CA, Liu T, Yang Y, Lang Z, Hu J, Wu J. Correlation of change in R2* and phase with putative iron content in deep gray matter of healthy adults. J Mag Res Imaging. in press. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Hackert VH, den Heijer T, Oudkerk M, Koudstaal PJ, Hofman A, Breteler MM. Hippocampal head size associated with verbal memory performance in nondemented elderly. Neuroimage. 2002;17:1365–1372. [PubMed]
  • Hallgren B, Sourander P. The effect of age on the non-haemin iron in the human brain. J Neurochem. 1958;3:41–51. [PubMed]
  • Hardy PA, Gash D, Yokel R, Andersen A, Ai Y, Zhang Z. Correlation of R2 with total iron concentration in the brains of rhesus monkeys. J Mag Res Imaging. 2005;21:118–127. [PubMed]
  • Harrison PM, Arosio P. The ferritins: molecular properties, iron storage function and cellular regulation. Biochim Biophys Acta. 1996;1275:161–203. [PubMed]
  • Hedden T, Gabrieli JD. Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;2:87–96. [PubMed]
  • Heo S, Prakash RS, Voss MW, Erickson KI, Ouyang C, Sutton BP, Kramer AF. Resting hippocampal blood flow, spatial memory and aging. Brain Res. 2010;1315:119–127. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Insausti R, Juottonen K, Soininen H, Insausti AM, Partanen K, Vainio P, Laakso MP, Pitkanen A. MR volumetric analysis of the human entorhinal, perirhinal, and temporopolar cortices. Am J Neuroradiol. 1998;19:659–671. [PubMed]
  • Kato H, Izumiyama M, Izumiyama K, Takahashi A, Itoyama Y. Silent cerebral microbleeds on T2*-weighted MRI: correlation with stroke subtype, stroke recurrence, and leukoaraiosis. Stroke. 2002;33:1536–1540. [PubMed]
  • Lee J, Hirano Y, Fukunaga M, Silva AC, Duyn JH. On the contribution of deoxy-hemoglobin to MRI gray-white matter phase contrast at high field. Neuroimage. 2010;49:193–198. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Madden DJ, Bennett IJ, Song AW. Cerebral white matter integrity and cognitive aging: contributions from diffusion tensor imaging. Neuropsychol Rev. 2009;19:415–435. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Martin WR, Roberts TE, Ye FQ, Allen PS. Increased basal ganglia iron in striatonigral degeneration: in vivo estimation with magnetic resonance. Can J Neurol Sci. 1998;25:44–47. [PubMed]
  • Martin WR, Wieler M, Gee M. Midbrain iron content in early Parkinson disease: a potential biomarker of disease status. Neurology. 2008;70(16 Pt 2):1411–147. [PubMed]
  • Morris CM, Candy JM, Oakley AE, Bloxham CA, Edwardson JA. Histochemical distribution of non-haem iron in the human brain. Acta Anatomica. 1992;144:235–257. [PubMed]
  • Nilsson PM. Early vascular aging (EVA): consequences and prevention. Vasc Health Risk Manag. 2008;4:547–552. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Nobili F, Rodriguez G, Marenco S, De Carli F, Gambaro M, Castello C, Pontremoli R, Rosadini G. Regional cerebral blood flow in chronic hypertension. A correlative study. Stroke. 1993;24:1148–1153. [PubMed]
  • Ochi N, Tabara Y, Igase M, Nagai T, Kido T, Miki T, Kohara K. Silent cerebral microbleeds associated with arterial stiffness in an apparently healthy subject. Hypertens Res. 2009;32:255–260. [PubMed]
  • Ogawa S, Lee TM, Nayak AS, Glynn P. Oxygenation-sensitive contrast in magnetic resonance image of rodent brain at high magnetic fields. Magn ResonMed. 1990;14:68–78. [PubMed]
  • Ogg RJ, Langston JW, Haacke EM, Steen RG, Taylor JS. The correlation between phase shifts in gradient-echo MR images and regional brain iron concentration. Mag Res Imaging. 1999;17:1141–1148. [PubMed]
  • Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113. [PubMed]
  • Ordidge RJ, Gorell JM, Deniau JC, Knight RA, Helpern JA. Assessment of relative brain iron concentrations using T2-weighted and T2*-weighted MRI at 3 Tesla. Magn Reson Med. 1994;32:335–341. [PubMed]
  • Pauling L, Coryell CD. The Magnetic Properties and Structure of the Hemochromogens and Related Substances. ProcNatl Acad Sci USA. 1936;22:159–163. [PubMed]
  • Péran P, Cherubini A, Luccichenti G, Hagberg G, Démonet JF, Rascol O, Celsis P, Caltagirone C, Spalletta G, Sabatini U. Volume and iron content in basal ganglia and thalamus. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:2667–2675. [PubMed]
  • Pfefferbaum A, Sullivan EV, Rosenbloom MJ, Mathalon DH, Lim KO. A controlled study of cortical gray matter and ventricular changes in alcoholic men over a 5-year interval. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:905–912. [PubMed]
  • Pfefferbaum A, Adalsteinsson E, Rohlfing T, Sullivan EV. MRI estimates of brain iron concentration in normal aging: comparison of field-dependent (FDRI) and phase (SWI) methods. NeuroImage. 2009;47:493–500. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Price JL, McKeel DW, Jr, Buckles VD, Roe CM, Xiong C, Grundman M, Hansen LA, Petersen RC, Parisi JE, Dickson DW, Smith CD, Davis DG, Schmitt FA, Markesbery WR, Kaye J, Kurlan R, Hulette C, Kurland BF, Higdon R, Kukull W, Morris JC. Neuropathology of nondemented aging: presumptive evidence for preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30:1026–1036. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Quintana C, Bellefqih S, Laval JY, Guerquin-Kern JL, Wu TD, Avila J, Ferrer I, Arranz R, Patino C. Study of the localization of iron, ferritin, and hemosiderin in Alzheimer’s disease hippocampus by analytical microscopy at the subcellular level. J Struct Biol. 2006;153:42–54. [PubMed]
  • Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psych Meas. 1977;1:385–401.
  • Raz N, Kennedy KM. A systems approach to age-related change: Neuroanatomical changes, their modifiers, and cognitive correlates. In: Jagust W, D’Esposito M, editors. Imaging the Aging Brain. New York, NY: Oxford UP; 2009. pp. 43–70.
  • Raz N, Lindenberger U, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Head D, Williamson A, Dahle C, Acker JD. Regional Brain Changes in Aging Healthy Adults: General Trends, Individual Differences, and Modifiers. CerebCortex. 2005;15:1676–1689. [PubMed]
  • Raz N, Rodrigue KM. Differential aging of the brain: Patterns, cognitive correlates and modifiers. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30:730–748. [PubMed]
  • Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Acker JD. Hypertension and the brain: Vulnerability of the prefrontal regions and executive functions. Behav Neurosci. 2003;17:1169–1180. [PubMed]
  • Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Haacke EM. Brain aging and its modifiers: Insights from in vivo neuromorphometry and susceptibility weighted imaging. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2007a;1097:84–93. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Acker JD. Vascular health and longitudinal changes in brain and cognition in middle-aged and older adults. Neuropsychology. 2007b;2:149–157. [PubMed]
  • Raz N, Gunning-Dixon F, Head D, Rodrigue KM, Williamson A, Acker JD. Aging, sexual dimorphism, and hemispheric asymmetry of the cerebral cortex: Replicability of regional differences in volume. Neurobiol Aging. 2004;25:377–396. [PubMed]
  • Resnick SM, Pham DL, Kraut MA, Zonderman AB, Davatzikos C. Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging studies of older adults: A shrinking brain. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:3295–3301. [PubMed]
  • Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Park DC. Beta-amyloid deposition and the aging brain. Neuropsychol Rev. 2009;19:436–450. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Rombouts SA, Scheltens P, Kuijer JP, Barkhof F. Whole brain analysis of T2* weighted baseline FMRI signal in dementia. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28:1313–1317. [PubMed]
  • Rowe CC, Ng S, Ackermann U, Gong SJ, Pike K, Savage G, Cowie TF, Dickinson KL, Maruff P, Darby D, Smith C, Woodward M, Merory J, Tochon-Danguy H, O’Keefe G, Klunk WE, Mathis CA, Price JC, Masters CL, Villemagne VL. Imaging beta-amyloid burden in aging and dementia. Neurology. 2007;68:1718–1725. [PubMed]
  • Salerno JA, Murphy DG, Horwitz B, DeCarli C, Haxby JV, Rapoport SI, Schapiro MB. Brain atrophy in hypertension. A volumetric magnetic resonance imaging study. Hypertension. 1992;20:340–348. [PubMed]
  • Schenck JF, Zimmerman EA. High-field magnetic resonance imaging of brain iron: Birth of a biomarker? NMR Biomed. 2004;17:433–445. [PubMed]
  • Schwartz GL, Bailey KR, Mosley T, Knopman DS, Jack CR, Jr, Canzanello VJ, Turner ST. Association of ambulatory blood pressure with ischemic brain injury. Hypertension. 2007;49:1228–1234. [PubMed]
  • Seals DR, Moreau KL, Gates PE, Eskurza I. Modulatory influences on ageing of the vasculature in healthy humans. Exp Gerontol. 2006;41:501–507. [PubMed]
  • Shmueli K, de Zwart JA, van Gelderen P, Li TQ, Dodd SJ, Duyn JH. Magnetic susceptibility mapping of brain tissue in vivo using MRI phase data. Magn Reson Med. 2009;62:1510–1522. [PubMed]
  • Shrout PE, Fleiss JL. Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing raters reliability. Psychol Bull. 1979;86:420–428. [PubMed]
  • Siemonsen S, Finsterbusch J, Matschke J, Lorenzen A, Ding XQ, Fiehler J. Age-dependent normal values of T2* and T2′ in brain parenchyma. Am J Neuroradiol. 2008;29:950–955. [PubMed]
  • Small SA, Perera GM, DeLaPaz R, Mayeux R, Stern Y. Differential regional dysfunction of the hippocampal formation among elderly with memory decline and Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol. 1999;45:466–472. [PubMed]
  • Small SA, Tsai WY, DeLaPaz R, Mayeux R, Stern Y. Imaging hippocampal function across the human life span: Is memory decline normal or not? Ann Neurol. 2002;51:290–295. [PubMed]
  • Small SA, Chawla MK, Buonocore M, Rapp PR, Barnes CA. Imaging correlates of brain function in monkeys and rats isolates a hippocampal subregion differentially vulnerable to aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:7181–7186. [PubMed]
  • Steiger JH. Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychol Bull. 1980;87:245–251.
  • Sullivan EV, Adalsteinsson E, Rohling T, Pfefferbaum A. Relevance of iron deposition in deep gray matter brain structures to cognitive and motor performance in healthy elderly men and women: Exploratory findings. BrainImaging and Behavior. 2009;3:167–175. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Sun J, Soo YO, Lam WW, Wong KS, Zeng JS, Fan YH. Different distribution patterns of cerebral microbleeds in acute ischemic stroke patients with and without hypertension. Eur Neurol. 2009;62:298–303. [PubMed]
  • Vígh B, Szél A, Debreceni K, Fejér Z, Manzano e Silva MJ, Vígh-Teichmann I. Comparative histology of pineal calcification. Histol Histopathol. 1998;13:851–870. [PubMed]
  • Viswanathan A, Chabriat H. Cerebral microhemorrhage. Stroke. 2006;37:550–555. [PubMed]
  • Yao B, Li TQ, Gelderen P, Shmueli K, de Zwart JA, Duyn JH. Susceptibility contrast in high field MRI of human brain as a function of tissue iron content. Neuroimage. 2009;44:1259–1266. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Yao H, Sadoshima S, Ooboshi H, Sato Y, Uchimura H, Fujishima M. Age-related vulnerability to cerebral ischemia in spontaneously hypertensive rats. Stroke. 1991;22:1414–1418. [PubMed]
  • Zecca L, Gallorini M, Schunemann V, Trautwein AX, Gerlach M, Riederer P, Vezzoni P, Tampellini D. Iron, neuromelanin and ferritin content in the substantia nigra of normal subjects at different ages: Consequences for iron storage and neurodegenerative processes. J Neurochem. 2001;76:1766–1773. [PubMed]
  • Zecca L, Youdim MB, Riederer P, Connor JR, Crichton RR. Iron, brain ageing and neurodegenerative disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:863–873. [PubMed]