PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of biolettershomepageaboutsubmitalertseditorial board
 
Biol Lett. Oct 23, 2010; 6(5): 630–632.
Published online Mar 17, 2010. doi:  10.1098/rsbl.2010.0159
PMCID: PMC2936150
Higher reproductive skew among birds than mammals in cooperatively breeding species
Nichola J. Raihani1,2* and Tim H. Clutton-Brock2
1Institute of Zoology, Zoological Society of London, Regent's Park, London NW1 4RY, UK
2LARG, Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EJ, UK
*Author for correspondence (nichola.raihani/at/ioz.ac.uk).
Received February 17, 2010; Accepted February 25, 2010.
While competition for limited breeding positions is a common feature of group life, species vary widely in the extent to which reproduction is shared among females (‘reproductive skew’). In recent years, there has been considerable debate over the mechanisms that generate variation in reproductive skew, with most evidence suggesting that subordinates breed when dominants are unable to prevent them from doing so. Here, we suggest that viviparity reduces the ability of dominant females to control subordinate reproduction and that, as a result, dominant female birds are more able than their mammal counterparts to prevent subordinates from breeding. Empirical data support this assertion. This perspective may increase our understanding of how cooperative groups form and are stabilized in nature.
Keywords: reproductive skew, cooperative breeding, dominance, subordinate, animal societies, sociality
In cooperatively breeding species, where individuals receive assistance in the production of young (Brown 1987), reproduction may be shared more or less equitably among the females in a group (Keller & Reeve 1994; Magrath et al. 2004). Although several theoretical models providing adaptive explanations for variation in skew have been developed (see Johnstone & Cant 2009 for a review), empirical tests of these models are relatively rare. Moreover, most empirical studies of reproductive skew have focused on testing the predictions of skew models rather than assessing the validity of the underlying assumptions, which is problematic as small perturbations to the assumptions of the model can strongly affect the predicted outcome (Hodge 2009).
Existing reproductive skew models make distinct assumptions regarding the level of control attributed to individuals within the group. Determining which set of assumptions is likely to best reflect reality is crucial in order to interpret empirical data within the most appropriate theoretical framework. Transactional models assume that one individual in the group has complete control over reproduction but shares reproduction, either to prevent others from leaving (Vehrencamp 1979, 1983) or to prevent others from evicting it (Johnstone & Cant 1999). Compromise models, on the other hand, assume that no individual has full control over reproduction. Instead, the extent of reproductive skew is the outcome of a conflict between dominants and subordinates in which neither is able to enforce their own optimum (Clutton-Brock 1998; Reeve et al. 1998). Compromise models were proposed as a more plausible alternative to transactional models, on the grounds that attributing complete control over reproduction to one individual is unrealistic (Clutton-Brock 1998; Nonacs 2007; Johnstone & Cant 2009). However, rigid empirical evidence supporting the key assumption of compromise models—that subordinates are more likely to breed when dominants are unable to prevent them from doing so—is scarce.
Here, we suggest that the ability of dominant females to control subordinates might depend on whether the species is oviparous or viviparous and that this might generate variation in reproductive skew. For example, in birds, a single female could monopolize breeding by preventing other females from accessing the nest during the laying period (e.g. Dickinson et al. 1996; Whittingham et al. 1997). In contrast, internal gestation in mammals may make it harder for a single female to prevent others from breeding. The extent to which female birds and mammals are able to control subordinate reproduction might then influence the extent of reproductive sharing, with subordinate female mammals being more likely to breed than subordinate female birds. We test this hypothesis using data from 58 cooperatively breeding species.
Data from a total of 58 cooperatively breeding species (37 bird species from 18 families, 21 mammal species from 12 families) were available (see the electronic supplementary material for more information). Reproduction was said to have been monopolized when the dominant female was the mother of more than 95 per cent of offspring born into a group, although the model was also checked using 90 per cent and 99 per cent, respectively, as the threshold values. To control for repeated effects on the distribution of the data, we carried out a linear mixed model (LMM) with ‘family’ included as a random term. The response term was binary (1 = dominant female monopolises reproduction; 0 = reproduction shared among more than one female in the group) and the model was fitted to a binomial error distribution with a logit-link function. ‘Taxon’ (bird/mammal) was included as the sole explanatory term.
Dominant female birds are more likely than dominant female mammals to monopolize breeding, by producing more than 95 per cent of the offspring within a group (LMM: χ21,57 = 11.4, p = 0.0007; figure 1). Similar differences are present if the threshold value is increased to 99 per cent (LMM: χ21,57 = 8.96, p = 0.003); or reduced to 90 per cent (LMM: χ21,57 = 4.91, p = 0.03). The significant effect of family was controlled for in all three models (95% threshold: χ2 = 4.25, p = 0.039; 99% threshold: χ2 = 3.93, p = 0.047; 90% threshold: χ2 = 4.34, p = 0.037).
Figure 1.
Figure 1.
The mean proportion (±s.e.m.) of bird and mammal species in which one female produces >95% of all offspring.
Models of reproductive skew make distinct assumptions about the extent to which dominants are able to prevent subordinate breeding. Here, using data from 58 cooperatively breeding species, we show that skew among female mammals is lower than among female birds and suggest that this variation may be attributed to asymmetries in control. Specifically, we propose that internal gestation (rather than egg-laying) makes it harder for female mammals to prevent other females from breeding. This finding better supports the assumptions of compromise models (where dominants have limited control over subordinates) than those of transactional models.
There are several reasons why the mode of reproduction might be expected to influence the control that dominant females can exert over subordinates. Oviparity in birds means that females can often prevent subordinates from breeding by defending access to a nest (Dickinson et al. 1996; Whittingham et al. 1997). Conversely, viviparity in mammals makes it more difficult for a single female to monopolize breeding as she must often police several females rather than defending a single breeding site. This typically involves preventing subordinates from conceiving, evicting pregnant subordinates or killing their offspring (e.g. Creel & Waser 1997; Clutton-Brock et al. 1998). A dominant female that evicts a subordinate faces not only energetic costs and the risk of injury but also reduces the pool of available carers to help with her own breeding attempts. This is not the case for birds, where dominant females can remove any eggs she suspects were laid by subordinates without evicting the perpetrators (e.g. Bertram 1979). Although birds risk making recognition errors when tossing eggs, the same risks are also faced by mammals that commit infanticide. Indeed, the cost of inadvertently killing one's own offspring may be substantially increased in mammals compared with birds by increased investment during the gestation period and, in some species, infanticide only occurs when the female can be sure that any pups she kill are not her own (e.g. Digby 1995; Clutton-Brock et al. 1998).
Data from bird species where males do the bulk of incubation support the idea that the extent of female control influences reproductive sharing within the group. Where males incubate the clutch, females are less able to maintain exclusive access to the nest (Haydock & Koenig 2002). As such, joint nesting (where multiple females lay in the same nest) is more common in species with male-biased incubation (Vehrencamp 2000). In some species, subordinate females occasionally breed by laying their eggs in a separate nest to the dominant female (Williams 2004; Berg 2005). Although such plural nesting may be harder for dominant females to prevent, it is relatively rare: just four of the 37 (10.8%) bird species in our dataset were plural-nesting species, while 29.7 per cent were joint-nesting species. The occurrence of plural nesting may be limited by food and habitat availability (Brown 1987; Hatchwell & Komdeur 2000). Furthermore, secondary females' nests are often less successful than those of dominants (Curry 1988; Williams 2004; Berg 2005), perhaps owing to interference by dominants (Curry 1988) and because secondary females commonly receive little or no assistance from auxiliaries (Langen & Vehrencamp 1999). This is not the case for mammals, where evidence suggests that helpers do not preferentially feed the dominant female's offspring (e.g. Clutton-Brock et al. 2001).
Although variation in the mode of reproduction seems to be a viable explanation for variation in skew among birds and mammals, there is a plausible alternative. It is possible that dispersal patterns affect the availability of unrelated breeding partners for subordinates and, consequently, the extent to which reproduction is shared among group members. Specifically, female-biased dispersal in birds (Greenwood 1980) might mean that subordinate females rarely have access to unrelated males on the natal territory and so rarely attempt to co-breed alongside the dominant female (see Koenig et al. (2009) for a discussion of incest avoidance and reproductive skew). A subordinate female mammal, on the other hand, may regularly encounter and mate with unrelated males that either visit or immigrate into her group (e.g. Young et al. 2007). Thus, rather than being harder to control, subordinate female mammals may simply have more opportunities to breed. Although we cannot refute this alternative, we would argue that incest avoidance falls short as an explanation for the variation in skew observed between birds and mammals. Firstly, although inbreeding is typically avoided, this is not universally so (e.g. McRae 1996; Koenig & Haydock 2004; Spottiswoode & Møller 2004). Secondly, female birds may conduct extra-territorial forays to find preferred mates, meaning that they are not constrained to mate only with the males in their home territory (e.g. Double & Cockburn 2000). To conclude, although dispersal patterns and concomitant incest avoidance do not seem to account fully for variation in skew between birds and mammals; more empirical data to assess the link between incest avoidance and reproductive sharing would certainly be useful.
An understanding of the mechanisms that generate skew is essential if we are fully to grasp how cooperative groups form and are maintained in nature. Although variation at the level of the group or species may often be attributed to ecological, social and genetic parameters (Clutton-Brock 1998; Reeve et al. 1998; Faulkes & Bennett 2001), higher level taxonomic variation in reproductive skew may well be explained by differences in reproductive physiology. The extent to which dominant females can control subordinate reproduction might then be expected to have important, broad-scale effects on intra-group conflict, life-history traits and population dynamics in cooperatively breeding birds and mammals.
Acknowledgements
We thank Mike Cant, Sinead English, Sarah Hodge and Seirian Sumner for useful discussion and for reading earlier versions, and two excellent referees for their comments which improved the quality of this manuscript. N.R. is funded by the Zoological Society of London.
  • Berg E. C. 2005. Parentage and reproductive success in the white-throated magpie-jay, Calocitta formosa, a cooperative breeder with female helpers. Anim. Behav. 70, 375–385 (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.11.008)
  • Bertram B. C. R. 1979. Ostriches recognise their own eggs and discard others. Nature 279, 233–234 (doi:10.1038/279233a0) [PubMed]
  • Brown J. L. 1987. Helping and communal breeding in birds. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
  • Clutton-Brock T. H. 1998. Reproductive skew, concessions and limited control. Trends Ecol. Evol. 13, 288–292 (doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(98)01402-5) [PubMed]
  • Clutton-Brock T. H., Brotherton P. N. M., Smith R., McIlrath G. M., Kansky R., Gaynor D., O'Riain M. J., Skinner J. D. 1998. Infanticide and expulsion of females in a cooperative mammal. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265, 2291–2295 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0573) [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Clutton-Brock T. H., Brotherton P. N. M., O'Riain M. J., Griffin A. S., Gaynor D., Kansky R., Sharpe L., McIlrath G. M. 2001. Contributions to cooperative rearing in meerkats, Suricata suricatta. Anim. Behav. 61, 705–710 (doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1631)
  • Creel S. R., Waser P. M. 1997. Variation in reproductive suppression among dwarf mongooses: interplay between mechanisms and evolution. In Cooperative breeding in mammals (eds Solomon N. G., French J. A., editors. ), pp. 150–170 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Curry R. L. 1988. Group structure, within-group conflict and reproductive tactics in cooperatively breeding Galapagos mockingbirds, Nesomimus parvulus. Anim. Behav. 36, 1708–1728 (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(88)80111-8)
  • Dickinson J. L., Koenig W. D., Pitelka F. A. 1996. Fitness consequences of helping behaviour in the western bluebird. Behav. Ecol. 7, 168–177 (doi:10.1093/beheco/7.2.168)
  • Digby L. 1995. Infant care, infanticide, and female reproductive strategies in polygynous groups of common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 37, 51–61 (doi:10.1007/BF00173899)
  • Double M., Cockburn A. 2000. Pre-dawn infidelity: females control extra-pair mating in superb fairy-wrens. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 267, 465–470 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1023) [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Faulkes C. G., Bennett N. C. 2001. Family values: group dynamics and social control of reproduction in African mole-rats. Trends Ecol. Evol. 16, 184–190 (doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(01)02116-4) [PubMed]
  • Greenwood P. J. 1980. Mating systems, philopatry and dispersal in birds and mammals. Anim. Behav. 28, 1140–1162 (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80103-5)
  • Hatchwell B. J., Komdeur J. 2000. Ecological constraints, life-history traits and the evolution of cooperative breeding. Anim. Behav. 59, 1079–1086 (doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1394) [PubMed]
  • Haydock J., Koenig W. D. 2002. Reproductive skew in the polygynadrous acorn woodpecker. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 7178–7183 (doi:10.1073/pnas.102624199) [PubMed]
  • Hodge S. J. 2009. Understanding variation in reproductive skew: directions for future empirical research. In Reproductive skew in vertebrates (eds Hager R., Jones C. B., editors. ), pp. 439–466 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Johnstone R. A., Cant M. A. 1999. Reproductive skew and the threat of eviction: a new perspective. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 266, 275–279 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0633)
  • Johnstone R. A., Cant M. A. 2009. Models of reproductive skew: outside options and the resolution of reproductive conflict. In Reproductive skew in vertebrates (eds Hager R., Jones C. B., editors. ), pp. 3–23 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Keller L., Reeve H. K. 1994. Partitioning of reproduction in animal societies. Trends Ecol. Evol. 9, 98–103 (doi:10.1016/0169-5347(94)90204-6) [PubMed]
  • Koenig W. D., Haydock J. 2004. Incest and incest avoidance. In Ecology and evolution of cooperative breeding in birds (eds Koenig W. D., Dickinson J. L., editors. ), pp. 142–156 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Koenig W. D., Shen S. F., Krakauer A. H., Haydock J. 2009. Reproductive skew in avian societies. In Reproductive skew in vertebrates (eds Hager R., Jones C. B., editors. ), pp. 227–264 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Langen T. A., Vehrencamp S. L. 1999. How white-throated magpie-jay helpers contribute during breeding. Auk 116, 131–140.
  • Magrath R., Johnstone R. A., Heinsohn R. G. 2004. Reproductive skew. In Ecology and evolution of cooperative breeding in birds (eds Koenig W. D., Disckinson J. L., editors. ), pp. 157–176 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • McRae S. B. 1996. Family values: costs and benefits of communal nesting in the moorhen. Anim. Behav. 52, 225–245 (doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0169)
  • Nonacs P. 2007. Tug-of-war has no borders: it is the missing model in reproductive skew theory. Evolution 61, 1244–1250 (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00092.x) [PubMed]
  • Reeve H. K., Emlen S. T., Keller L. 1998. Reproductive sharing in animal societies: reproductive incentives or incomplete control by dominant breeders? Behav. Ecol. 9, 267–278 (doi:10.1093/beheco/9.3.267)
  • Spottiswoode C., Møller A. P. 2004. Genetic similarity and hatching success in birds. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 271, 267–272 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2605) [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Vehrencamp S. L. 1979. The roles of individual kin and group selection in the evolution of sociality. In Handbook of behavior and communication (eds Marler P., Vandenbergh J. G., editors. ), pp. 351–394 New York, NY: Plenum Press.
  • Vehrencamp S. L. 1983. Optimal degree of skew in cooperative societies. Am. Zool. 23, 327–335.
  • Vehrencamp S. L. 2000. Evolutionary routes to joint-female nesting in birds. Behav. Ecol. 11, 334–344 (doi:10.1093/beheco/11.3.334)
  • Whittingham L. A., Dunn P. O., Magrath R. D. 1997. Relatedness, polyandry and extra-group paternity in the cooperatively-breeding white-browed scrubwren (Sericornis frontalis). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 40, 261–270 (doi:10.1007/s002650050341)
  • Williams D. A. 2004. Female control of reproductive skew in cooperatively breeding brown jays (Cyanocorax morio). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 55, 370–380 (doi:10.1007/s00265-003-0728-7)
  • Young A. J., Spong G., Clutton-Brock T. 2007. Subordinate male meerkats prospect for extra-group paternity: alternative reproductive tactics in a cooperative mammal. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 1603–1609 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.0316) [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Articles from Biology Letters are provided here courtesy of
The Royal Society