PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptHHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 August 6.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC2917239
NIHMSID: NIHMS179251

Stored-trace reactivation in rat prefrontal cortex is correlated with down-to-up state fluctuation density

Abstract

Spontaneous reactivation of previously stored patterns of neural activity occurs in hippocampus and neocortex during non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep. Notable features of the neocortical local field potential during NREM sleep are high amplitude, low frequency thalamocortical oscillations including K-complexes, low-voltage spindles and high-voltage spindles. Using combined neuronal ensemble and local field potential recordings, we show that prefrontal stored-trace reactivation is correlated with the density of down-to-up state transitions of the population of simultaneously recorded cells, as well as K-complexes, and low voltage spindles in the local field potential. This result strengthens the connection between reactivation and learning as these same NREM sleep features have been correlated with memory. Although memory trace reactivation is correlated with low-voltage spindles, it is not correlated with high-voltage spindles, indicating that despite their similar frequency characteristics, these two oscillations serve different functions.

Keywords: memory, sleep, spindles, down states, K-complexes, learning

Introduction

Sleep reactivation of stored neuronal firing patterns expressed during a recent waking experience is hypothesized to underlie a gradual rearrangement of neocortical connections that allows some memories to become integrated into the brain’s general memory store (Marr, 1971; McClelland et al., 1995; Buzsáki, 1998). This hypothesis has been supported by experimental observations of stored-trace reactivation in multiple brain regions and in a variety of different species (Pavlides and Winson 1989; Wilson and McNaughton 1994; Kudrimoti et al. 1999; Dave and Margoliash 2000; Hoffman and McNaughton 2002; Peigneux et al. 2004). The link between memory reactivation and learning, however, remains experimentally unconfirmed.

Euston et al. (2007) used two statistical techniques to demonstrate reactivation in rat medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). “Explained variance” (EV) is based on the similarity between the firing rate correlation matrices in the ensemble of simultaneously recorded neurons during behavior and subsequent sleep (Kudrimoti et al., 1999). The other technique, template matching, identifies multi-neuronal firing sequences during waking behavior that recur during post-behavioral sleep (Louie and Wilson, 2001; Tatsuno et al., 2006). EV is a relatively robust measure, largely independent of both the temporal order in which neuronal patterns appear, and the speed of play back. Template matching captures the temporal order of cell-firing sequence replay, but is highly sensitive to both permutations of event order and fluctuations in playback time-scale.

Reactivation occurs predominantly during NREM sleep (Kudrimoti et al., 1999), which is characterized by high amplitude, low frequency oscillations of local field potentials (LFP) (Steriade, 2003). Although NREM sleep in rodents is not generally divided into different stages (but: Jarosiewicz et al., 2002; Jarosiewicz and Skaggs, 2004), distinctive electrographic features reflecting different underlying network dynamics can be identified. K-complexes, down-to-up state transitions (Steriade et al. 1993a and b), and the slow oscillation are different manifestations of the same neural population event (Amzica and Steriade 1997a and b) and are identifiable both in the LFP and in the sum of single spikes (see Fig. 1a and b). Additionally, despite occupying similar and sometimes overlapping frequency bands, two different kinds of sleep spindles can be differentiated (Kandel and Buzsáki, 1997). The amplitude of one is 3-5 times larger than the other (Buzsáki, 1991), and thus we refer to them as high voltage spindles (HVS: 7-8 Hz) and low voltage spindles (LVS: 6-20, primarily 10-20 Hz).

Fig. 1
K-complexes and down states

We investigated how reactivation strength in the mPFC is related to neural population events observed in the LFP and the sum of individual neuron activity. The motivation for this effort was two-fold: first, to determine when, during sleep, memory replay occurs and, second, to strengthen the link between memory-trace reactivation and memory consolidation. While reactivation has not yet been correlated with increased learning, several electrographic features of NREM sleep have been (Gais et al., 2002; Clemens et al., 2005; Eschenko et al., 2006; Marshall et al., 2006). A correlation between these same features and stored-trace reactivation strength would support the hypothesized connection between learning and sleep reactivation.

Methods

Subjects

Three male Brown Norway/Fisher 344 hybrid rats (7-9 months old at the time of surgery, 350-400 g) were housed in Plexiglas home cages, maintained on a reversed, 12:12 hr light-dark cycle. Training and recording sessions occurred during the dark portion of this cycle.

Data Acquisition

Neural recordings were obtained via a chronically implanted “hyperdrive” consisting of 12 independently manipulatable tetrodes. Each tetrode consisted of four, polyimide-coated, nichrome wires (diameter 14 μm, Kanthal Palm Coast, Palm Coast, FL) twisted together (McNaughton et al., 1983b; Reece and O’Keefe, 1989; Wilson and McNaughton, 1994). Hyperdrive construction was as described in Gothard et al. (1996) except that tetrodes were inserted into silica tubing (65 μm inner diameter, 125 μm outer diameter, Polymicro Technologies, Phoenix, AZ) for added rigidity, and secured with cyanoacrylate glue after insertion into the drive. About 4-5 mm of wire protruded, ensuring that only the tetrodes penetrated the brain and not the silica sheath. During recording sessions, the hyperdrive was connected to a unity-gain headstage (HS-54, Neuralynx, Bozeman, MT) that enabled low-noise transmission of neural data to the recording system. The headstage also contained an array of LED’s that could be detected by an overhead camera, enabling tracking of the position of the rat on the maze and during sleep at 60 frames/sec (30 frames/sec interleaved) with a pixel resolution of 640 × 480. All data were recorded using a Neuralynx Cheetah recording system running in combination with the computer. Single unit data from each tetrode were amplified, filtered between 0.6 and 6 kHz and digitized at 32 kHz. Video spatial resolution was approximately 3 pixels/cm. Twelve LFP traces were collected; one from a single channel on each tetrode. The LFP traces were amplified and filtered between 1 and 475 Hz with a 1 kHz or 2 kHz sampling rate.

Surgery and Electrode Placement

NIH guidelines and the University of Arizona IACUC approved protocols were followed for all surgical and behavioral procedures. Each rat was anesthetized with Isoflurane (1-2 % by volume in oxygen at a flow rate of 1.5-2.5 L/min), placed in a stereotaxic holder and injected with Penicillin G (30,000 units per hind leg i.m.). The skull was cleared of skin and fascia and craniotomies were opened for two stimulating electrodes targeting the Medial Forebrain Bundle, or MFB (rat 1: - 3.25 mm AP, 1.65 mm ML bilaterally, 8.5 mm ventral from dura; rat 2: -2.5 mm AP, 1.8 mm ML right, 8.5 mm ventral from dura and -4.0 mm AP, 1.5 mm ML right, 8.2 mm ventral from dura; rat 3: -2.5 mm AP, 1.8 mm ML right, 8.5 mm ventral from dura and -4.0 mm AP, 1.5 mm ML right, 8.2 mm ventral from dura) and a hyperdrive. Each stimulating electrode consisted of two, Teflon-coated, stainless steel wires (coated diameter 0.0045”, A-M Systems, Sequim, WA) twisted together with approximately 0.5 mm of insulation removed from one tip. The tips were separated by about 0.5 mm in the long axis. In rats 1 and 2 the hyperdrive was centered over the left mPFC at 3.0 (rat 1) or 2.9 (rat 2) mm AP, 1.3 mm ML, and angled at 9.5 degrees toward the midline. In rat 3, the hyperdrive actually consisted of two groups of electrodes, only half of which went into the frontal cortex. The other 6 electrodes were positioned over the hippocampus, but the data from these electrodes are not reported here. The six prefrontal tetrodes were centered over the left mPFC at 3.0 mm AP, 1.3 mm ML, and angled at 9.5 degrees toward the midline. It should be noted that the bundle of cannulae holding the electrodes, and therefore the electrodes themselves, extended about 1.4-2 mm in the anterior-posterior dimension, so that cells were sampled from a relatively wide region of the medial frontal cortex. Medial-lateral extent varied between 0.5 and 1.4 mm, depending on the geometry of the cannula bundle. Rats were returned to ad libitum feeding and allowed to recover for 3-4 days after surgery. Single units were recorded with respect to a reference electrode positioned deep in the mPFC. The tetrodes were moved gradually from 2000 to roughly 4000 μm from the brain surface, keeping all electrodes at the same depth as nearly as possible. For rats 1 and 2, a full set of behavior (see below) was obtained at a given depth and then all of the electrodes were moved down 80-120 μm to ensure a fresh group of cells. After that, tetrodes were moved only as necessary to obtain good recordings until the next group turning event. This procedure was repeated until electrodes reached a depth of 4000 μm (rat 1) or 3200 μm (rat 2). In rat 3 the prefrontal electrodes were lowered to depths between 1800 μm and 2400 μm and held constant throughout the experiment except for occasional movements to obtain more cells. All tetrode positioning was done after a given recording session, to allow the tetrodes at least 18 hours to stabilize prior to the next recording session.

After the conclusion of the experiments a histological analysis was performed to confirm that all electrodes were located in the medial precentral, anterior cingulate, and prelimbic cortex (nomenclature from Krettek and Price, 1977). A description of the analysis and pictures of the histological sections can be found in the Supplementary Materials (Fig. S1).

Reward

Following stimulation electrode placement, MFB stimulation was used as reinforcing reward (for a review and training techniques, see: Olds and Milner (1954), Olds and Fobes (1981), Liebman and Cooper (1989)). All stimulation used two wires, though not necessarily the two twisted wires in one stimulating electrode. The choice of electrodes was determined empirically based on the rat’s response. Despite the bilaterally implanted stimulation electrodes in rat 1, all stimulation was delivered exclusively on right side of the brain, contralateral to the hyperdrive. A range of stimulation parameters was explored using an operant conditioning chamber equipped to deliver MFB stimulation when the rat performed a nose poke. Stimulus parameters were selected based on the minimum net current needed to sustain repeated self-stimulation. The final selected MFB stimulation consisted of a train of 400 μs wide, 70 to 100 μA, diphasic current pulses, delivered at 150 Hz for 320-370 ms.

Behavioral Procedures

The behavioral task has been described in detail elsewhere (Euston and McNaughton, 2006; Euston et al., 2007) but is briefly described here. All rats were trained to find reward at one of the eight, equally-spaced reward zones on the edge of a 1.2 m circular arena. The correct zone was indicated by a flashing LED. Upon reaching the goal zone, reward was delivered and, after a brief delay (0.5-1.0 sec), the next zone was cued by a flashing LED. Onset of the LED was also accompanied by a brief 4 kHz tone. This training continued until each rat made direct trajectories to reward locations. A training session lasted 50-60 minutes.

Rats were initially trained to run to randomly chosen reward zones, but were subsequently switched to a specific sequence of zones. After a rat completed a sequence three times with guidance from LED cues (a “cued” block of sequences), a 5 s delay was inserted between the non-spatial, audio cue and the illumination of the cue light, providing time for the rat to move to the next reward location without the aid of the visual cue. Given the typical running speed of a rat, the vast majority of cue-delay trials in well-learned sequences were completed without the LED and are hence referred to as “non-cued” trials. After the rat completed the non-cued sequence three times, audio and visual cues were presented simultaneously, again, starting another cued block. Blocks of three, complete traversals of the sequence alternated between cued and non-cued throughout the duration of the recording session.

Rats were run in one of two different kinds of sequences; one eight elements long and the other six elements long. The eight element sequence contained two repeated segments in the shape of a “V”, followed by an alternating choice between two zones. This sequence was the same as that used by Euston and McNaughton (2006), where a thorough description can be found. The other sequence had six elements with no repeated segments. For each sequence, rats were trained until they reached asymptotic performance. Asymptotic performance was defined in the same way as in Bower et al. (2005); the animal completed 70% of the cue-delay trials with sufficient continuity of movement and accuracy. Asymptotic performance was usually reached within three days, using two training sessions per day. For rats 1 and 2, electrodes were then pushed down to acquire new cells and a new sequence was initiated. The new sequence was created by flipping (i.e. a mirror image) and rotating the original sequence so as to create a sequence novel to the rat. The flip ensured that the order of turns was reversed while the rotation ensured that a different configuration of places was rewarded. Rats 1 and 2 were presented with a simple 6-element non-repeating sequence for two days before starting each new repeat-element sequence. Rat 3 was trained on the same eight-element “V” sequence for all days reported here. All data presented in the present paper were gathered on days when the rat had reached asymptotic performance. The rats were run daily for one to four months.

At the start of the experiment, the rat was brought into the recording room and placed in a towel-lined bowl in the center of the recording arena. The headstage was attached to the hyperdrive. The signal gain and threshold for each electrode were manually set on the basis of visual inspection, data recording was initiated, and the rat was allowed to rest for 20-40 min in the towel-lined bowl (pre-task sleep, S1). The exact duration of the rest period was not systematically controlled. Following this rest period, the rat was moved from the bowl to the arena. The task phase (M1) began when the rat was placed on the behavioral platform. The rat was then returned to the towel-lined bowl and allowed to rest for another 20-40 min (post-task sleep, S2). Rats then ran another task phase (M2) and were given a final rest period (post-task sleep, S3).

Data Pre-processing

Artifact arising from MFB stimulation was removed from the single unit data by deleting all spikes recorded during the stimulation and those occurring within 20 ms of stimulation termination (i.e., the black-out window was stimulus duration + 20 ms). The LFP was not analyzed during behavioral epochs. Individual units were isolated offline (for details see Supplementary Materials); the end result was of a collection of timestamps associated with each detected action potential from a given cell. The LFP traces were low pass filtered at 100 Hz and down-sampled to 200 Hz.

Determination of Motionless Periods

Position during each video frame was extracted by fitting a circle to the ring of LEDs on the headstage in each video frame. The positions were then smoothed by convolution of both x and y position data with a normalized Gaussian function with standard deviation of 120 video frames (about 1.8 sec). After smoothing, the instantaneous velocity was found by taking the difference in position between subsequent video frames. An epoch during which the headstage velocity dropped below 0.78 pixels/second for more than 2 min was judged to be a period of motionlessness. This threshold was set based on visual examination of the velocity profiles in order to exclude frame by frame jitter and subtle readjustments of body position. In practice, each 30-40 min sleep epoch contained several motionless periods with lengths varying between 2 and roughly 30 min (mean = 18 min, standard deviation = 8.8 min).

Sleep Analysis Window

For each “sleep” epoch the periods during which the animal was motionless for at least two minutes (sleep blocks) were extracted. For sleep blocks that were longer than two minutes, a sliding window was established. The two minute window started at the beginning of the sleep block and moved forward in two minute increments so that none of the analysis windows overlapped. At each of these steps, all of the analysis parameters were calculated for that time window. Thus, every increment of the window has a corresponding data point. Experimental sessions lacking any sleep blocks were excluded from the analysis. For each data point, the EV and the number of template matches were also calculated over the same time period.

Digital Filtering

Fourth order Chebyshev Type 1 filters implemented using the Matlab Signal Processing Toolbox (The Mathworks, Natick, MA) were used for the digital filtering of both the local field potentials and the sum of the binned spike trains.

Detection of Down States

Down states are commonly recognized by the absence or near absence of spiking in the population of cells (Sirota and Buzsáki, 2005; Luczak et al., 2007; Hoffman et al., 2007). Consequently, down states were defined as times when all of the individual cells were silent for at least 100 ms. To find these times, all of the spike trains from populations of 50-150 simultaneously recorded cells within one session were binned into 50 ms (20 Hz) non-overlapping bins and all of the binned spike trains were summed. The incidences when this summed signal was zero for two or more consecutive points were counted as down states.

Detection of HVS

Although easily distinguished by eye, HVS epochs are difficult to detect automatically due to the presence of LVS in the same frequency band (LVS power is mostly in the 10-20 Hz range but can be as low as 6 Hz). During a bout of HVS a large number of cells are progressively recruited and become phase locked to the oscillation. Because of this powerful oscillatory entrainment, HVS epochs are associated with high amplitude deflections in the LFP. The depth of modulation in HVS is greater than the depth of modulation in LVS (Kandel and Buzsáki, 1997) and this was used to distinguish the two events. HVS epochs were identified by locating those periods of time when the dominant power in the LFP was in the spindle range and when the depth of signal modulation in the spindle range (as measured by the variance) was high.

Detection was accomplished by implementing the following procedure: 1) Two filtered versions of the LFP were made; one representing the K-complex frequency (2-6 Hz) and one representing the entire spindle range (6-20 Hz). 2) A 250 ms sliding window was used to evaluate the standard deviation of the spindle-filtered LFP over time. Starting at the beginning of the sleep block, the window was advanced 1/10 of its full extent (25 ms) until the end of the window reached the end of the LFP trace. In each increment of the 250 ms window, the standard deviation of the LFP filtered in the spindle frequency range was calculated over that window. The output of this step is referred to as the “standard deviation signal.” 3) To isolate the incidences of high variance, the points in the standard deviation signal that exceeded two times the standard deviation of that signal, were selected. 4) In order to eliminate false identification of K-complexes as HVS, the group of selected points was restricted to those falling during a time when the power in the spindle band was dominant. This was insured by averaging the windowed and filtered versions of the LFP over 500 ms and finding the times during which the magnitude of the power in the K-complex band was less than 80% of the magnitude of the power in the spindle band. 5) This process was performed independently for each of the LFP traces. For each of these, the number of peaks in a sleep block passing the criteria described above was summed. To quantify the duration of HVS in each 2 min sleep block, an “HVS score” was computed by averaging the number of peaks identified in all of the LFP traces from all of the recording electrodes during that sleep block. The HVS score does not indicate the number of continuous bouts of HVS, but rather the amount of time in each 2 min window that was occupied by HVS oscillations.

Other spindle detection algorithms (Siapas and Wilson, 1998; Schabus et al., 2004; Clemens et al., 2005; Eschenko et al., 2006) have relied on bandpass filtering and an amplitude threshold, as we do here. These algorithms do not, however, distinguish between LVS and HVS as was required for the present analysis. The standard deviation-based method was introduced as a convenient way to differentiate the two rhythms.

The bin width and power threshold values were subjective. However, these values were chosen to provide the optimal detection of sleep events and were therefore not truly arbitrary. Importantly, assignment of these values was made independently from the reactivation results and consistently applied across sessions and between subjects. A change in filtering parameters or thresholds would thus be expected to influence the number of HVS (or LVS or K-complex) epochs detected, but should not directly influence the relationship between sleep parameters and strength of reactivation, an assessment of which was the primary goal of this paper.

Detection of K-complexes

K-complexes and LVS both occur during times when the sum of single cell activity fluctuates between up states and down states. As can be seen in Fig. 1, K-complexes and down-to-up state transitions co-occur and are in fact different reflections of the same phenomenon (Amzica and Steriade, 1997b, 2002). The detection of K-complexes in the LFP was performed using the same steps that were used for HVS detection with a few exceptions. First, in step 2 the absolute change in amplitude of the signal, rather than the standard deviation, was used to identify an event. This was because the K-complex is a high-amplitude, biphasic event, the full extent of which generally fits within one window. A window with a large peak-to-peak difference, therefore, is very likely to be centered on a K-complex. In each window the absolute difference between the maximum amplitude and minimum amplitude was calculated and thus we refer to the output signal as the “difference signal”. Second, the difference signal was smoothed with a 10 point moving average filter and a peak finding algorithm was used to identify the time points corresponding to local maxima. Those peaks that were greater than two times the standard deviation of the difference signal and which also occurred during times of high K-complex power were selected (the ratio of spindle power to K-complex power was less than 80%). The number of peaks falling during each sleep block was summed and, again, the number of peaks averaged over all of the LFPs from all of the recording electrodes was used as a measure of the number of K-complexes. Unlike the HVS score, the K-complex score represents the number of events, and is not a measure of time.

Detection of LVS

The LVS detection method was the same as the HVS detection method, with two notable differences. First, in step 2, instead of the standard deviation, the power in the spindle band was used. Points exceeding two times the standard deviation of the binned spindle power were selected. Second, in step 4, the peaks were required to fall during times when the K-complex frequency was the dominant power in the signal. This effectively excluded all of the points that had already been designated as HVS and required that identified incidences of LVS co-occur with K-complexes. As before, the number of peaks occurring during each sleep block was averaged over all of the LFP traces from each of the tetrodes and this was used to represent the amount of time occupied by LVS.

Spindle Separation

In order to confirm that LVS and HVS are different populations of events, oscillatory activity in the spindle band of the LFP was further examined with respect to the dominant frequency and the depth of modulation, the two most distinguishing characteristics of LVS and HVS. For each rat, a session was selected in which a substantial amount of both LVS and HVS was identified. An LFP trace from one of the recording electrodes (randomly chosen) was binned (250 ms). The power and variance in the entire spindle band (6-20 Hz) as well as the power in the HVS (6-10 Hz) and LVS (10-20 Hz, capturing most of the LVS power) bands were calculated for each bin. The peaks of the spindle band, defined as the bins in which the power exceeded 2 times the standard deviation of the power in the 6-20 Hz band, were selected. The HVS power, the LVS power and the variance corresponding to these peaks were used to characterize the activity in the spindle band.

Regression Analysis

The data points for each of the measures and for the EV for the two sleep sessions following behavioral epochs (S2 and S3) were grouped and expressed as a z-score. The data points for each measure and for the EV were then collapsed across recording sessions and each measure was linearly fit to the EV using a least squares method. Fits were considered significant for a p-value <0.05.

Statistical Comparison of Sleep Features Before and After Behavior

In order to determine if there was a significant difference in the incidence of down states, K-complexes, LVS and HVS between the first (S1) and the last (S3) sleep sessions, a two-sample t-test was performed to test the hypothesis that the means were the same. For each session the score for each event (i.e. the number of down states) in S1 was divided by the number of 2 min windows in S1. Likewise, the score for each event in S3 was divided by the number of 2 min windows in S3. These normalized scores were pooled over all sessions. The equal mean hypothesis was tested at the 5% level.

Explained Variance

For the first sleep epoch, the behavioral epochs, and each of the sleep blocks pulled from the second and third sleep epochs, spike trains were binned into T non-overlapping intervals of 50 ms (for comments on binsize see Supplementary Materials), producing sequences of spike counts xi[t]. The normalized correlation C between each pair (ij) of spike trains was computed using the equation:

Cij=1/Tt=1Txi[t]xj[t]μiμjσiσj

derived from Pearson’s correlation coefficient, where μ is the mean and σ is the standard deviation of x[t]. Thus the same population of cells produced matrices of firing-rate correlations, corresponding to the pre-task sleep, the task, and each of the post-task sleep blocks. The correlation matrix is symmetric, so the lower off-diagonal elements are extracted, producing a vector of all pair-wise cell correlations. The separate correlation matrices are entered into the subsequent explained variance (EV) calculations, described next.

Any measure of reactivation must show that activity patterns induced during the task reappear during the subsequent sleep. In addition, activity patterns that existed during the first sleep epoch should be discounted because these were evidently not induced by the task experience. The EV measure is the square of the partial correlation of task and post-task sleep cell-pair correlations, partialing out any pre-existing correlations during pre-task sleep:

EV=rT,SbSa2=(rT,SbrT,SarSb,Sa(1rT,Sa2)(1rSb,Sa2))2

where T, Sa and Sb represent task, pre-task sleep, and post-task sleep periods (Kudrimoti et al., 1999). EV reflects the proportion of variability in the Sb sleep correlations that can be accounted for by task correlations after accounting for Sa sleep correlations. Any pre-task sleep replay of task activity (e.g., from a previous session) or indeed, any restriction on the allowable correlations due to connectivity constraints would tend to reduce the EV measure.

Template Matching

Each template was an N×M matrix representing the firing rate of N cells (rows) over M time bins (columns). Six or eight templates were generated, one for each segment of the sequence. A segment was defined as the time between arrival at one reward zone and arrival at the next reward zone, excluding times during which stimulation was delivered. Each row of the template comprised the spike counts from one cell within a series of 100 ms bins covering one segment, averaged over all repetitions of the sequence. Repetitions were first screened to eliminate segments that took inordinately long times (i.e., segments during which the rat was off-task). For each segment, any repetitions during which the traversal time exceeded four times the distance of the quartile from the median were excluded. For the remaining repetitions, each segment was scaled so that traversal time equaled the median time. The spikes from these scaled repetitions were then averaged in 100 ms bins. Note that median times were different for the different segments, meaning that the number of template bins varied between 13 and 42.

The bin width used in template matching was chosen for both behavioral and analytical reasons. Based on observation, a bin width of 100 ms effectively captures the rate of change of cellular firing during behavior. With smaller bins, a given cell’s firing rate may not change significantly from one bin to the next. With longer bins, temporal sequences across cells are no longer discernable. Bin width is also constrained by the need to account for temporal compression of replay. For a 100 ms template bin width at 6 times compression (as used in this paper), the target bin was 16.6 ms. In practice, 16 ms is near the lower limit of bin sizes which yields strong template correlations for this data. Finally, the 100 ms bin width has proven effective for detecting reactivation in a previously published paper (Euston et al., 2007).

Template matching was based on a subset of cells that were active during all phases of the experiment and that exhibited task-related firing rate changes. Any cells with less than 50 spikes in any given period (pre-task sleep, task, or post-task sleep) were excluded. Two additional criteria were applied to determine whether a cell should be included in the template matching analysis. The first was a firing rate criterion. Cells in which the average spike count exceeded five spikes during a given template (i.e., the sum of all spike counts within that cell’s row of the template exceeded five) were judged to have a sufficient spike count for that template. The second criterion was that the cell show strong task-related activity. Towards this end, each row of each template was first smoothed using a 5-point (i.e. five 100 ms bins) moving average. This removed high frequency random fluctuation of firing activity but left task-related slow modulation. The coefficient of variation (standard deviation divided by the mean, computed on the 13-42 values in a given row) was then computed for each cell that exceeded the firing rate criterion. A cell was judged to have significant task-related firing in a given template if its coefficient of variation exceeded 0.40. To be included in the template matching analysis, neurons needed to pass both the firing rate and coefficient of variation criteria on at least half of the templates (3 in the case of 6 templates and 4 in the case of 8 templates). This requirement resulted in a significant reduction in the number of cells; typically between one third and two thirds of the original number of cells were included in the template matching analysis. In addition, it was also confirmed that the template matching analysis was robust to variations of the threshold.

The spike activity of all included cells from a recording session is stored in an N × T spike-matrix Q (McNaughton, 1998), where rows correspond to N cells and columns to T discrete bins. The bin contents represent the number of spikes each cell fired during each time bin. In the limit of infinitely small bins, each row becomes a spike raster. As described above, the template matrix is an averaged firing pattern during each segment of the sequential task. A single template, X, is an N × M matrix, where M corresponds to the number of bins in the template. A target matrix Y, with the same dimensions as the template, is also selected from the Q matrix. In matrix form, both template matrix X and target matrix Y are represented as:

X=[x11x12:x1Mx21x22::::::xN1::xNM],Y=[y11y12:y1My21y22::::::yN1::yNM]

The template matching method seeks to calculate the similarity of these two matrices (Louie and Wilson, 2001). Based on our previous study on different measures of template match strength (Tatsuno et al., 2006), we used the standardized Pearson correlation coefficient measure. In this measure, each row of the X and Y matrices is standardized to zero-mean and unit variance by subtracting its row mean, xn¯ and yn¯, and dividing by the row standard deviation, σx,n and σy,n, respectively. xn¯, yn¯, σx,n and σy,n are defined as:

xn¯=1Mm=1Mxnm,ynm¯=1Mm=1Mynm,σx,n=1Mm=1M(xnmxn¯)2,σy,n=1Mm=1M(ynmyn¯)2

This normalization transforms the elements xnm and ynm to z-score variables wnm and znm through:

wnm=xnmxn¯σx,n,znm=ynmyn¯σy,n

By construction, mean firing rate differences among different rows is fully suppressed with this normalization. Therefore, this measure is sensitive to the firing order relationships among different neurons. The standardized Pearson measure is then defined using the 2-dimensional Pearson correlation coefficient, COR, for the normalized W and Z matrices,

COR=n=1Nm=1M(wnmw¯)(znmz¯)n=1Nm=1M(wnmw¯)2n=1Nm=1M(znmz¯)2

where the mean w and [z macron] are calculated as

w¯=1NMn=1Nm=1Mwnm,z¯=1NMn=1Nm=1Mznm

In a previous study (Euston et al., 2007) we determined that sleep replay occurred approximately 6 times faster than the speed of firing during behavior. Therefore, in this study, we modified the bin width of the target matrix to reflect a compression factor of 6. Examples of the template matching results can be found in the Supplementary Materials (Fig. S4).

To assess a template “match”, the z-score for each match was computed using a sampling distribution estimated from shuffled templates. For this analysis, the column vectors of the template matrix were randomly shuffled, and 100 shuffled templates were generated for each template matrix. The template match was then calculated for each shuffled template, and a sampling distribution was generated for each time point. The raw template match value at each time point was then converted to a z-score, and the z-score greater than 4 was marked as a template “match”. The number of template matches found for each of the templates during each 2 min window was summed. This number, the total number of template matches, was used as an alternative to explained variance to quantify the magnitude of reactivation.

Results

For these experiments, three rats were chronically implanted with 6 or 12 moveable tetrodes in the pre-frontal cortex. These tetrodes were used to record simultaneously the single unit firing and the local field potentials generated by the brain during active performance of a sequence running task. Based on the histologically reconstructed electrode tracks and the depths to which electrodes were lowered, all electrodes were confirmed to be located in the most ventral portion of medial precentral cortex, anterior cingulate, or prelimbic cortex.

Each recording session consisted of an initial 20-40 minute sleep epoch (S1), a 50 minute behavioral epoch in which the animal ran a trained sequence task (M1), an intermediate 20-40 minute sleep epoch (S2), another 50 minute behavioral epoch (M2), and a final 20-40 minute sleep epoch (S3). The experimental protocol was slightly modified for the third rat; the final sleep epoch was extended to approximately 120 minutes. Here, however, we are only using the first 40 minutes of this extended sleep period in order to make the results between animals more comparable.

The analysis was restricted to times when the rat was either sleeping or resting motionlessly. Video-tracker data were used to identify the times in the sleep epochs during which the animal was not moving. Previous analyses showed that the frequency of occurrence of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep during the first 40 minutes was extremely rare, and so the analyses focused on aspects of NREM sleep.

Characterization of sleep features in the LFP and single unit activity

In a plot of the LFP during a sleep session, it is clear that motion (wakefulness) is marked by low amplitude, high frequency activity. During motionlessness, on the other hand, the LFP is marked by high amplitude, low frequency activity. Likewise, motionlessness in the sum of individual spikes is indicated by times when all cells stop firing and the sum of spikes goes to zero (down states) (Fig. 2a); a phenomenon which is not observed during waking. Closer visual examination of the LFP during motionlessness reveals that this state is marked by high frequency, low amplitude oscillations punctuated by high amplitude, low frequency oscillations (Fig. 2b). Again, these states are not only visible in the LFP, they are also apparent in the sum of single unit activity.

Fig. 2
Examples of LFP and the sum of single cell activity during periods of motion and motionlessness

The bouts of high amplitude, low frequency oscillations can be further differentiated into periods of high voltage spindling and periods marked by K-complexes and low voltage spindles (Figs. 2b, ,3).3). This differentiation is based on frequency characteristics in the LFP, entrainment of spikes in the single unit activity, and the presence or absence of down/up states. In Fig. 3 the activity of a group of cells recorded simultaneously is compared to the LFP from the same electrodes during HVS (Fig. 3a) and K-complexes/LVS (Fig. 3b). It is easily demonstrated that HVS corresponds not only to a distinctive 7-8 Hz oscillation in the LFP, but also to a 7-8 Hz oscillation in the sum of spikes. Although the activity frequently reaches zero during the oscillation, down states, as defined above, do not occur during HVS.

Fig. 3
Example of HVS epoch and down state/K-complex/LVS epoch

The LFP frequencies associated with LVS tend to be higher and more distributed (10-20 Hz) (Fig 3b), although we have observed them to be as slow as 6 Hz. Unlike HVS, incidences of LVS are frequently coupled with K-complexes in the LFP and down/up states in the sum of spikes. Another important difference between HVS and LVS is that LVS oscillations do not recruit the entire population of cells such that they can be identified in the single unit activity (Fig. 3b). The fact that HVS oscillations entrain a much larger fraction of the cells relative to LVS presumably accounts for the much larger LFP peak-to-peak amplitude seen in HVS. This demonstrates the extent to which the population of cells becomes entrained to the HVS oscillation.

The different qualities of HVS and LVS are further illustrated in Fig. 4. Peaks in the spindle range (6 – 20 Hz) have a bi-modal distribution in both frequency and depth of modulation. This clustering shows that HVS and LVS represent two distinct populations of spindles, the functional implications of which should be evaluated separately.

Figure 4
Spindle distributions

K-complexes are strongly associated with down-state to up state transitions (Fig. 1, Fig. 3b) and thus, like HVS, are identifiable in the single unit activity. During those periods when K-complexes interspersed with low voltage spindles are observed in the LFP the sum of spikes fluctuates slowly between complete silence and strong activity (down/up states, Fig. 5). In fact, the distribution of the total spike activity transitions from uni-modal to bi-modal at the onset of K-complex activity in the LFP.

Fig. 5
Distribution of population activity states during waking and motionlessness

Relationship between sleep characteristics and memory reactivation

For analysis, the motionless periods were subdivided into two-minute, non-overlapping, windows, each of which defines a data point. “Sleep” epochs without any motionless periods were not analyzed. For each window, four sleep-characterization parameters were calculated; the number of down states, the number of K-complexes, the amount of LVS, and the amount of HVS. The EV (taking into account S1) over each window was also determined so that each four dimensional data point was matched to an EV value. A variable number of data points were computed for each session depending on the amount of time the animal remained motionless during that session. For the first rat, 29 sessions and 356 data points were analyzed, for the second rat, 3 sessions and 72 data points were analyzed, and for the third rat, 10 sessions and 196 data points were analyzed. On average, 98 cells (range: 66-142), 74 cells (range: 69-78), and 63 (range: 43-76) cells were recorded per session in rats 1, 2 and 3 respectively. In order to compare results across sessions, the EV measure and each of the four characterization parameters for each session were expressed as z-scores (means and standard deviations of the un-normalized parameters for each of the three rats can be found in Table 1). These parameters were subsequently evaluated for their ability to predict the magnitude of the EV using a least squares regression analysis. For the first two rats we also used a template matching method as an alternative measure of memory reactivation. The number of template matches occurring during each time window was tallied and a regression analysis for the characterization parameters was performed in the same way as for the EV. Because the template matching method is extremely computationally intensive and time consuming, the analysis was only run on a subset of the sessions for two of the rats.

Table 1
Un-normalized parameter means and standard deviations

In Fig. 6 the linear regression plots for the individual parameters are shown for each of the three rats. For all three of the rats the number of down states, the number of K-complexes, and the amount of LVS in a time window were significantly positively correlated with the EV value (p<0.05). In contrast, in all three rats, the amount of HVS was significantly, but weakly, inversely correlated with EV.

Fig. 6
Linear regression plots for EV

Fig. 7 shows the linear regression plots for each of the characterization parameters and the number of template matches in S3 for 7 sessions recorded in the first rat (first column), and for 3 sessions recorded in the second rat (second column). These results are consistent with the EV results for all sessions; the number of down states, K-complexes and the amount LVS are positively correlated with the number of template matches. The amount of HVS is weakly inversely correlated with the number of template matches in rat 2 and uncorrelated with the number of template matches in rat 1. Thus, both measures of reactivation produce similar results. That this should be the case is not surprising as the two measures are strongly correlated with each other (Fig. 7).

Fig. 7
Linear regression plots for template matching

Sleep features in pre- and post-behavioral sleep

A t-test was used to determine if the incidence of the different sleep characteristics was different between S1 and S3. As Table 2 shows, the number of down states, the number of K-complexes (except in rat 2), and the amount of LVS was significantly greater in S3 as compared to S1. In contrast, the amount of HVS was not significantly different between S1 and S3. This suggests that while down states, K-complexes and LVS are affected by the behavior, HVS is relatively unaffected. Thus, K-complexes and LVS, which are associated with reactivation, are much more numerous after a task known to induce memory replay but HVS, which is actually negatively associated with reactivation, does not increase in frequency.

Table 2
Incidence of events in S1 and S3

Relationship of sleep features to each other

The four characterization parameters are not linearly independent of each other, as shown in Table 3. As it has been previously suggested down states and K-complexes are different manifestations of the same event (Amzica and Steriade, 1997b, 2002), it is not surprising that these two measures are strongly correlated with each other. Because down states and K-complexes were extracted using completely independent methods (one based on LFP, the other on the sum of spikes from all cells), this correlation, as well as the analysis shown in Fig. 1, stands as confirmation of the observations of Amzica and Steriade (1997b). Our methods may have strengthened correlations between K-complex activity and LVS and weakened those between K-complexes and HVS because the presence of K-complex activity was used to discriminate between HVS and LVS. Hence, correlations between characterization parameters is inconclusive with respect to the relationship between K-complexes, HVS, and LVS. This fact does not detract, however, from our primary findings of a relationship between EV and K-complexes and LVS. The similar results for down-states, LVS, and K-complexes confirm that the methods used to extract these parameters, though computationally different, are, at least, internally consistent and therefore, more likely to be reliable.

Table 3
Event Correlations

Discussion

These results indicate that the reactivation of memory traces occurs preferentially during periods with a high density of down-states and associated K-complexes and low voltage spindles. This conclusion is supported by the positive correlation of the EV, a measure of reactivation strength, with the number of down states, K-complexes, and time spent in LVS. These three variables were also correlated with the number of template matches, an alternative measure of memory reactivation which is computed using entirely different principles from the correlation-based EV measure. In contrast, the amount of time spent in HVS was weakly inversely correlated with the magnitude of EV and the number of template matches.

In recent years a great deal of interest has been focused on NREM sleep as more evidence emerges to support its role in learning and memory. Long considered a quiescent state, it has now been demonstrated that during NREM sleep cortical neurons are actually quite active, although sensory inputs are dramatically diminished. Some of this activity can be attributed to reactivation of previously stored memory traces (Wilson and McNaughton, 1994; Qin et al., 1997; Hoffman and McNaughton, 2002; Ji and Wilson, 2007; Euston et al., 2007), a process which is thought to serve memory consolidation (Marr, 1971; McClelland et al., 1995; Buzsáki, 1998). Large amplitude, low frequency corticothalamic oscillations of the LFP are the hallmark of this sleep state (Steriade, 2003). The oscillations themselves, especially spindles, have been hypothesized to play an important role in memory (Steriade and Timofeev, 2003), a theory which is supported by a growing body of evidence (Gais et al., 2002; Eschenko et al., 2006). Our results reveal a correlation between memories representing recent experience in the prefrontal cortex and network down states, K-complexes, and low voltage spindling. Notably, replay is negatively correlated with high voltage spindling. These results shed light on which brain states are associated with memory replay and, when combined with other findings, suggest a correlation between memory replay and memory consolidation.

One of the important contributions of the present findings is to the sharpen the distinction between what we call high-voltage and low-voltage spindles. Two distinct types of spindles were described by Contreras and Steriade (1996; Contreras et al., 1997), a waxing and waning type and an exclusively waning type; in our terminology these correspond to HVS and LVS respectively. The authors described that LVS were grouped by the slow oscillation and were preceded by a depth positive EEG wave (K-complex). HVS gradually waxed by entraining more neurons and then desynchronized the same way. The maximum amplitude of the oscillation was dependent upon the level of background activity. The authors reported that, although both types of spindles occur in natural sleep, under ketamine-xylazine anesthesia only LVS is observed. Under barbiturate anesthesia, on the other hand, spontaneous LVS is not observed but spontaneous HVS is. This difference was ascribed to the different levels of corticothalamic network activity observed under the two drugs (barbiturate = low activity, ketamine-xylazine = high activity). The conclusion of the authors was that the cortex is very important in shaping and synchronizing thalamically generated spindles.

Both LVS and HVS are thalamically generated oscillations; damage to the reticular nucleus of the thalamus abolishes both phenomena (Buzsáki et al., 1988; Steriade et al., 1985). The frequency ranges of HVS and LVS are also close together and sometimes overlap; HVS predominantly falls within a narrow 7-8 Hz frequency band with several prominent harmonics, LVS covers a wider and higher frequency range, about 10-20 Hz, but may be as low as 6 Hz. The current source density profiles for HVS and LVS are also similar (Kandel and Buzsáki, 1997). Despite these similarities, however, the two oscillations can be qualitatively separated based on waveform shape, amplitude, duration, and the extent to which they entrain the population of cells. Figure 4 demonstrates that LVS and HVS can also be quantitatively separated. This study adds a possible functional distinction between the two oscillations as well: LVS is associated with memory trace replay and possibly memory consolidation, while HVS plays some other yet-to-be-determined role.

There are several reasons to believe the cortical spindling plays an important role in memory. Unfortunately, however, the distinction between HVS and LVS is not always clear in the literature. Delineating the two spindle types is quite important as our results show that the two oscillations have different implications concerning reactivation and memory. Therefore, the classification of LVS and HVS must be inferred where it is not explicitly stated.

In humans, increased spindle density in the 12-15 Hz band is correlated with learning a declarative memory task (Gais et al., 2002), and also with retention of verbal memories (Clemens et al., 2005). In rats, a corresponding increase in spindle power was observed after learning, and after relevant recall of a remote memory (Eschenko et al., 2006). Also in rats, stimulation of synapses that had been previously potentiated by tetanizing stimulation more reliably evoked 7-14 Hz spindles than stimulation of un-potentiated synapses (Werk et al., 2005).

Spindles in the neocortex (both 7-14 Hz and 12-18 Hz), which we have shown to be correlated with neocortical reactivation, have also been temporally correlated with hippocampal sharp waves (Siapas and Wilson, 1998; Sirota et al., 2003). Hippocampal sharp waves have also been correlated with cortical transitions from down to up states and with the slow oscillation (Battaglia et al., 2004). These correlations are interesting not only because hippocampal reactivation occurs preferentially during sharp waves (Wilson and McNaughton 1994; Kudrimoti et al. 1999), but also because suppression of sharp waves has been shown to impair spatial memory (Girardeau et al., 2009). These results, taken together, reinforce the connection between neocortical reactivation, hippocampal reactivation and learning, consistent with consolidation theories (McClelland et al., 1995). In humans, induction of the slow oscillation by trans-cranial stimulation increased the number of sleep spindles (8-12 Hz) after learning a paired-associate task (Marshall et al., 2006). Remarkably, stimulation of the slow oscillation also significantly enhanced memory retention. Thus, it appears that LVS and the slow oscillation (and therefore the K-complex) are involved in the neocortical-hippocampal dialogue in a manner which facilitates learning.

Our data are consistent with a growing body of literature suggesting that HVS may be associated with memory deficits. HVS activity in rats increases with age (Radek et al., 1994) while memory is known to decline (Burke and Barnes, 2006). In aged rats, density of HVS correlates with passive avoidance retention deficits (Aldinio et al., 1985) and spatial memory impairment (Radek et al., 1994). Alternatively, these results may simply reflect a decrease in the time spent in LVS, which, based on the above discussion, might also lead to memory deficits. Rates of LVS were not reported in these studies.

The results presented here are correlative and cannot be considered a proof; however, they are consistent with the postulate that stored-trace reactivation contributes to the establishment of long-term memory. Our data show that LVS, down-to-up state transitions, and K-complexes are positively correlated with reactivation. A separate body of literature has shown that these same sleep features are predictive of learning and memory (Gais et al., 2002; Clemens et al., 2005; Marshall et al., 2006; Eschenko et al., 2006). Our findings thus provide possible link between these two domains.

Approximately 50% of the inter-down state intervals occur within the 1-2 second range, which is also approximately the range of LVS duration. Therefore, although further analysis is required, it appears likely that reactivation actually occurs during the low voltage spindle oscillations themselves.

Our results, in addition to the studies cited above, indicate that the cortical activity state is a determining factor in when memory replay can occur. This conclusion is in agreement with the conclusion of Marshall et al. (2006) that the slow oscillation/K-complexes/network down state is causally involved in memory replay and consolidation. The reason for this causal link is unknown, but it is reasonable to speculate that the complete silencing of neocortical activity during downstates facilitates the retrieval of weakly biased attractors corresponding to the most recent memories. According to the idea that the sleeping cortex passes through complex sequences of states reflecting learned or innate asymmetries in its weight matrix, this ongoing activity may prevent the network from converging on recent memory states. Briefly silencing the activity may therefore enable the network to reset to the strongest attractor states, which would typically be the most recently established ones.

Supplementary Material

Supp1

Acknowledgments

We thank P. Lipa, R. Roop, G. Van Acker, J. Dees, L. Ash, A. Uprety, S. Van Rhoads, A. Egurrola, J. Wang, and J. Meltzer for help with data collection and analysis. This work was supported by grant NS020331 from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, by grant MH046823 from the National Institute of Mental Health and the Alberta Heritage Foundation for Medical Research.

References

  • Aldinio C, Aporti F, Calderini G, Mazzari S, Zanotti A, Toffano G. Experimental models of aging and quinolinic acid. Methods Find Exp Clin Pharmacol. 1985;7:563–8. [PubMed]
  • Amzica F, Steriade M. Cellular substrates and laminar profile of sleep K-complex. Neuroscience. 1997a;82:671–686. [PubMed]
  • Amzica F, Steriade M. The K-complex: Its slow (<1-Hz) rhythmicity and relation to delta waves. Neurology. 1997b;49:952–959. [PubMed]
  • Amzica F, Steriade M. The functional significance of K-complexes. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2002;6:139–149. [PubMed]
  • Battaglia FP, Sutherland GR, McNaughton BL. Hippocampal sharp wave bursts coincide with neocortical “up-state” transitions. Learn Mem. 2004;11:697–704. [PubMed]
  • Bower MR, Euston DR, McNaughton BL. Sequential-Context-Dependent Hippocampal Activity Is Not Necessary to Learn Sequences with Repeated Elements. J Neurosci. 2005;25:1313–1323. [PubMed]
  • Burke SN, Barnes CA. Neural plasticity in the ageing brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:30–40. [PubMed]
  • Buzsáki G, Bickford RG, Ponomareff G, Thal LJ, Mandel R, Gage FH. Nucleus basalis and thalamic control of neocortical activity in the freely moving rat. J Neurosci. 1988;8:4007–26. [PubMed]
  • Buzsáki G. Memory consolidation during sleep: a neurophysiological perspective. J Sleep Res. 1998;7(Suppl 1):17–23. [PubMed]
  • Buzsáki G. The thalamic clock: emergent network properties. Neuroscience. 1991;41:351–64. [PubMed]
  • Clemens Z, Fabó D, Halász P. Overnight verbal memory retention correlates with the number of sleep spindles. Neuroscience. 2005;132:529–35. [PubMed]
  • Contreras D, Destexhe A, Steriade M. Spindle oscillations during cortical spreading depression in naturally sleeping cats. Neuroscience. 1997;77:933–6. [PubMed]
  • Contreras D, Steriade M. Spindle oscillation in cats: the role of corticothalamic feedback in a thalamically generated rhythm. J Physiol. 1996;490(Pt 1):159–79. [PubMed]
  • Dave AS, Margoliash D. Song replay during sleep and computational rules for sensorimotor vocal learning. Science. 2000;290:812–6. [PubMed]
  • Eschenko O, Mölle M, Born J, Sara SJ. Elevated sleep spindle density after learning or after retrieval in rats. J Neurosci. 2006;26:12914–20. [PubMed]
  • Euston DR, Tatsuno M, McNaughton BL. Fast-forward playback of recent memory sequences in prefrontal cortex during sleep. Science. 2007;318:1147–50. [PubMed]
  • Euston DR, McNaughton BL. Apparent Encoding of Sequential Context in Rat Medial Prefrontal Cortex Is Accounted for by Behavioral Variability. J Neurosci. 2006;26:13143–13155. [PubMed]
  • Gais S, Mölle M, Helms K, Born J. Learning-dependent increases in sleep spindle density. J Neurosci. 2002;22:6830–4. [PubMed]
  • Girardeau G, Benchenane K, Wiener SI, Buzsaki G, Zugaro MB. Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples impairs spatial memory. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:1222–1223. [PubMed]
  • Gothard KM, Skaggs WE, Moore KM, McNaughton BL. Binding of hippocampal CA1 neural activity to multiple reference frames in a landmark-based navigation task. J Neurosci. 1996;16:823–35. [PubMed]
  • Hoffman KL, McNaughton BL. Coordinated reactivation of distributed memory traces in primate neocortex. Science. 2002;297:2070–3. [PubMed]
  • Hoffman KL, Battaglia FP, Harris K, MacLean JN, Marshall L, Mehta MR. The Upshot of Up States in the Neocortex: From Slow Oscillations to Memory Formation. J Neurosci. 2007;27:11838–11841. [PubMed]
  • Jarosiewicz B, McNaughton BL, Skaggs WE. Hippocampal Population Activity during the Small-Amplitude Irregular Activity State in the Rat. J Neurosci. 2002;22:1373–1384. [PubMed]
  • Jarosiewicz B, Skaggs WE. Level of Arousal During the Small Irregular Activity State in the Rat Hippocampal EEG. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91:2649–2657. [PubMed]
  • Ji D, Wilson MA. Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:100–7. [PubMed]
  • Kandel A, Buzsáki G. Cellular-synaptic generation of sleep spindles, spike-and-wave discharges, and evoked thalamocortical responses in the neocortex of the rat. J Neurosci. 1997;17:6783–97. [PubMed]
  • Krettek JE, Price JL. Projections from the amygdaloid complex to the cerebral cortex and thalamus in the rat and cat. J Comp Neurol. 1977;172:687–722. [PubMed]
  • Kudrimoti HS, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL. Reactivation of hippocampal cell assemblies: effects of behavioral state, experience, and EEG dynamics. J Neurosci. 1999;19:4090–101. [PubMed]
  • Liebman JM, Cooper SJ. The Neuropharmacological Basis of Reward. 1989.
  • Louie K, Wilson MA. Temporally structured replay of awake hippocampal ensemble activity during rapid eye movement sleep. Neuron. 2001;29:145–56. [PubMed]
  • Luczak A, Barthó P, Marguet SL, Buzsáki G, Harris KD. Sequential structure of neocortical spontaneous activity in vivo. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2007;104:347–352. [PubMed]
  • Marr D. Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1971;262:23–81. [PubMed]
  • Marshall L, Helgadóttir H, Mölle M, Born J. Boosting slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory. Nature. 2006;444:610–3. [PubMed]
  • McClelland JL, McNaughton BL, O’Reilly RC. Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol Rev. 1995;102:419–57. [PubMed]
  • McNaughton BL, Barnes CA, O’Keefe J. The contributions of position, direction, and velocity to single unit activity in the hippocampus of freely-moving rats. Exp Brain Res. 1983a;52:41–9. [PubMed]
  • McNaughton BL, O’Keefe J, Barnes CA. The stereotrode: a new technique for simultaneous isolation of several single units in the central nervous system from multiple unit records. J Neurosci Methods. 1983b;8:391–7. [PubMed]
  • McNaughton BL. The Neurophysiology of Reminiscence. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 1998;70:252–267. [PubMed]
  • Olds J, Milner P. Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1954;47:419–27. [PubMed]
  • Olds ME, Fobes JL. The central basis of motivation: intracranial self-stimulation studies. Annu Rev Psychol. 1981;32:523–74. [PubMed]
  • Pavlides C, Winson J. Influences of hippocampal place cell firing in the awake state on the activity of these cells during subsequent sleep episodes. J Neurosci. 1989;9:2907–18. [PubMed]
  • Peigneux P, Laureys S, Fuchs S, Collette F, Perrin F, Reggers J, Phillips C, Degueldre C, Del Fiore G, Aerts J, Luxen A, Maquet P. Are spatial memories strengthened in the human hippocampus during slow wave sleep? Neuron. 2004;44:535–45. [PubMed]
  • Qin Y, McNaughton BL, Skaggs WE, Barnes CA. Memory Reprocessing in Corticocortical and Hippocampocortical Neuronal Ensembles. Royal Society of London Philosophical Transactions Series B. 1997;352:1525–1533. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Radek RJ, Curzon P, Decker MW. Characterization of high voltage spindles and spatial memory in young, mature and aged rats. Brain Res Bull. 1994;33:183–8. [PubMed]
  • Reece ML, O’Keefe J. The tetrode: a new techniques for multi-unit extracellular recording. Society of Neuroscience Abstracts. 1989;15:1250.
  • Schabus M, Gruber G, Parapatics S, Sauter C, Klösch G, Anderer P, Klimesch W, Saletu B, Zeitlhofer J. Sleep spindles and their significance for declarative memory consolidation. Sleep. 2004;27:1479–85. [PubMed]
  • Siapas AG, Wilson MA. Coordinated interactions between hippocampal ripples and cortical spindles during slow-wave sleep. Neuron. 1998;21:1123–8. [PubMed]
  • Sirota A, Buzsáki G. Interaction between neocortical and hippocampal networks via slow oscillations. Thalamus Relat Syst. 2005;3:245–259. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Sirota A, Csicsvari J, Buhl D, Buzsáki G. Communication between neocortex and hippocampus during sleep in rodents. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2003;100:2065–2069. [PubMed]
  • Steriade M, Nunez A, Amzica F. A novel slow (<1 Hz) oscillation of neocortical neurons in vivo: depolarizing and hyperpolarizing components. J Neurosci. 1993a;13:3252–3265. [PubMed]
  • Steriade M, Nunez A, Amzica F. Intracellular analysis of relations between the slow (< 1 Hz) neocortical oscillation and other sleep rhythms of the electroencephalogram. J Neurosci. 1993b;13:3266–3283. [PubMed]
  • Steriade M, Deschenes M, Domich L, Mulle C. Abolition of spindle oscillations in thalamic neurons disconnected from nucleus reticularis thalami. J Neurophysiol. 1985;54:1473–1497. [PubMed]
  • Steriade M. The corticothalamic system in sleep. Front Biosci. 2003;8:d878–99. [PubMed]
  • Steriade M, Timofeev I. Neuronal plasticity in thalamocortical networks during sleep and waking oscillations. Neuron. 2003;37:563–76. [PubMed]
  • Tatsuno M, Lipa P, McNaughton BL. Methodological considerations on the use of template matching to study long-lasting memory trace replay. J Neurosci. 2006;26:10727–42. [PubMed]
  • Werk CM, Harbour VL, Chapman CA. Induction of long-term potentiation leads to increased reliability of evoked neocortical spindles in vivo. Neuroscience. 2005;131:793–800. [PubMed]
  • Wilson M, McNaughton B. Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science. 1994;265:676–679. [PubMed]