PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptNIH Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
Pediatrics. Author manuscript; available in PMC Jul 2, 2010.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC2895811
NIHMSID: NIHMS210753

Preventive Interventions Addressing Underage Drinking: State of the Evidence and Steps Toward Public Health Impact

Abstract

The epidemiological features of underage drinking and evidence of its social, health, and economic consequences suggest compelling reasons for the development and dissemination of effective preventive interventions. To clarify the nature and extent of the current evidence base on preventive interventions addressing underage drinking, a review of the literature was conducted through extensive searches of the research literature on outcome evaluations, existing reviews of this body of outcome research (N = 25), and summary reports of evidence on specific interventions. More than 400 interventions were identified and screened, and the evidence for 127 was reviewed. Criteria for the evaluation of evidence were established for intervention studies with alcohol-specific outcome measures for 3 developmental periods (<10, 10–15, and 16 to ≥20 years of age). Ultimately, 12 interventions met criteria for “most promising” evidence and 29 met criteria for “mixed or emerging” evidence. Conducting this review revealed clear advances in the number of evidence-based interventions available and the quality of outcome research; however, much work remains to achieve greater public health impact through evidence-based interventions. This work should consider (1) the great need for intervention research related to understudied developmental phases, intervention domains (eg, family, school, community, and media), and populations (eg, early tweens, late teens, young adults not attending college, and nonmajority populations); (2) the critical importance of addressing key issues in research design and methods (eg, limited longitudinal studies, replication studies, and dissemination research); and (3) the need for improved consistency in application of evidence and reporting standards. Finally, we recommend the application of emerging consumer-oriented and community-participatory models for intervention development and research, designed to increase the likelihood of “real-world” public health impact through improved translation of intervention science into practice.

Keywords: interventions, alcohol, youth, evidence

Many of the published reports on outcomes of preventive interventions addressing underage drinking open with statements on the broad scope of the problem, based on epidemiological data and results from studies of health, and economic consequences of underage drinking. It is eminently clear from such findings that the magnitude of the problem is great. In this country, lifetime prevalence rates of alcohol use among eighth-, 10th-, and 12th-graders are 41.0%, 63.2%, and 75.1%, respectively.1 Prevalence rates for past 30-day use are substantial, at 17.1%, 33.2%, and 47.0% for the 3 grade levels. Of considerable concern are the levels of more-problematic types of use, including binge drinking and drunkenness. For example, the past 30-day rates of drunkenness among eighth-, 10th-, and 12th-graders in 2005 were 6.0%, 17.6%, and 30.2%, respectively.1 It is noteworthy that these problematic levels of alcohol use occur worldwide.2

There is extensive literature on the social, health, and economic consequences of underage drinking.36 To begin, the single greatest mortality risk of underage drinking is traffic crashes7; adolescents who indulge in heavy drinking are more likely to engage in risky driving behaviors.8,9 Underage drinking also is a major factor in both unintentional and intentional injury deaths.7 Furthermore, adolescents who drink heavily are at increased risk for development of physical health problems, during adolescence and subsequently.10 Among the major health problems are those associated with an increased likelihood of unprotected sexual activity.8,9,11 Underage drinking also is associated with a range of mental health and other behavioral problems, including depression and suicidality,4,12,13 delinquent behaviors,14 and violence, including rapes,15 as well as poorer academic performance.16,17 The costs of underage drinking are estimated to be more than $62 billion, although estimates are wide-ranging18,19 and a comprehensive, definitive, economic analysis remains to be performed.7

Perhaps the single most important point to be made about underage drinking is that there can be substantial, lifelong consequences that take a tremendous toll on individuals, families, communities, and society as a whole. Emerging evidence suggests that heavy drinking may have significant lasting effects on brain structure and function that adversely affect positive youth and young adult development.20,21 Notably, early onset of alcohol use is associated with problematic substance use in later adolescence and an increased likelihood of alcohol-related disorders in adulthood. For example, individuals who initiate drinking before 15 years of age are 4 times as likely to develop alcohol dependence as are those who wait until ≥21 years of age; each additional year of delayed drinking reduces the likelihood of dependence by 14%.22 The adult alcohol use disorders that are rendered more likely by underage drinking are associated with serious health problems and substantial negative economic impact.7

The prevalence rates and problematic consequences of underage drinking warrant a comprehensive public health approach, firmly grounded in evidence-based preventive interventions and policy-making. From a public health perspective, there are many challenges in addressing the underage drinking problem in this country. Our view is that a major challenge is the design and testing of interventions across developmental stages for a wide range of subpopulations, interventions designed to reduce risk factors and to promote protective factors that delay initial use and lower rates of binge drinking and other forms of alcohol abuse. This includes the need for a range of effective interventions and policies, including comprehensive community-level interventions. A related challenge is the widespread dissemination of interventions demonstrating effectiveness, as addressed below.23,24

This review responds to recommendations for addressing the prevention of underage drinking in the recent Surgeon General’s Call to Action on Preventing Underage Drinking.6 In addition, this review is written as a companion piece to the exhaustive review by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Underage Drinking Initiative Steering Committee of the developmental antecedents and consequences of underage drinking for 3 developmental periods (<10 years of age, 10–15 years of age, and 16 to ≥20 years of age) presented in this supplement. It was considered essential to have a current comprehensive review of the evidence on interventions addressing underage drinking (both prevention and treatment) for each of these 3 developmental periods. Our review was conducted in the context of numerous existing reviews of the literature on a range of preventive interventions and reports summarizing evidence-based interventions. Indeed, for purposes of the current review, we uncovered 25 reviews or meta-analyses of the literature addressing the more-general topic of substance-related interventions. Those most relevant to the present review were published since the middle 1990s and capture the findings and conclusions of earlier reviews, as well as extending them. Among those published since the middle 1990s, most reviews were directed toward preventive interventions targeting the full range of preventive intervention outcomes, such as those targeting a broad range of substance use, as well as risk and protective factors for substance use,2532 other problem behaviors such as violence and antisocial behavior,33,34 mental health,35,36 or positive youth development.37 Only 2 relatively recent reviews focused exclusively on alcohol misuse,38,39 and 1 had a relatively narrower focus on primary prevention and long-term outcomes.38

None of the reviews and meta-analyses met all of the criteria for the current review. That is, there were no reviews that had all 4 of the following: (1) exclusive focus on alcohol outcomes or effects on primary risk factors for problematic alcohol use among youths, (2) classification of reviewed evidentiary literature on specific interventions on the basis of levels of evidence, (3) inclusion of all developmental periods, and (4) inclusion of all types of interventions, beyond primary or universal interventions, such as in the present review. Moreover, most reviews on interventions targeting alcohol outcomes did not pay special attention to (and weigh more heavily) evidence from intervention outcome studies that had conducted follow-up evaluations beyond intervention posttests or beyond the time point at which the primary core components of the intervention were delivered, as did the current review. One exception was the systematic review of primary preventions for alcohol misuse among young people by Foxcroft et al.38 This systematic review did examine a number of interventions also evaluated for the current review; however, because of the differences in the inclusion/exclusion criteria considered and because of the application of different evaluation criteria, conclusions drawn by the 2 reviews are somewhat different.

As described below, a second type of “review” that provides the context for the present review is a summary report of evidence-based interventions. There has been a proliferation of such summaries over the past 10 years, including those by the Blueprints for Violence Prevention,40 Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration National Registry of Evidence-Based Programs and Practices,41 and the US Department of Education Safe and Drug-Free Schools Program.42 A summary of 12 of these reports has been completed (www.colorado.edu/cspv/blueprints). For the most part, these summary reports state their selection criteria and then provide synopses of the selected interventions, including their evidence base. Like the current review, they typically cover interventions targeting all developmental periods. None, however, focus exclusively on alcohol outcomes. Notably, work is underway through the What Works Repository43 to create a classification framework that will allow comparison of interventions from all extant model intervention summary reports, using a common frame of reference. The criteria for classification in the What Works Repository were among those considered in determining the classification criteria for the current review, as described below.

The objectives of this review are threefold. The first objective is to highlight the compelling reasons for greater attention to evidence-based preventive interventions addressing underage drinking. The second objective is to provide a review of alcohol-targeting interventions with evidence of efficacy or effectiveness, for interventions involving 3 age groups (<10, 10–15, and 16 to ≥20 years of age). The third objective is to discuss key findings and their implications from a public health perspective, including coverage of needed areas of intervention, critical research issues, standards of evidence, and future directions in achieving greater public health impact through preventive interventions.

METHODS

Intervention Selection Criteria

Type of Intervention

The scope of interest for this review included universal (for everyone in an eligible population), selective (for those who are members of population subgroups at higher risk), and indicated (for those with existing risk factors or conditions that identify them as being individually at risk) prevention interventions.44 With this scope in mind, interventions that entailed treatment for youths who already showed an alcohol-related disorder were excluded.

Target Population Age

The review was organized around the age groups targeted by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Underage Drinking Initiative, to the extent possible. That is, as noted earlier it focused on interventions targeting 3 age groups (<10, 10–15, and 16 to ≥20 years of age). Reviews addressing the age 16 to ≥20 group considered interventions targeting high school students and noncollege populations beyond high school; interventions directed toward college-attending populations were excluded because comprehensive reviews specifically directed toward that population had already been conducted. Larimer and Cronce,27 for example, provided an excellent review of interventions directed toward college students.

To elaborate, Larimer and Cronce27 reviewed universal, selective, and indicated interventions for college students, implemented with individuals, small groups, or classrooms, or delivered by mail and computer/Internet, from 1999 through 2006. Although their review uncovered >1000 studies, only 42 met the inclusion criteria (≥1 active individual intervention condition, a drinking behavior outcome, a control condition, prospective random assignment to conditions, 70% participant retention, 6-month follow-up period, and >25 individuals per condition). Interventions were categorized into 3 groups, that is, (1) educational/awareness-building (information/knowledge programs, values clarification, and normative reeducation), (2) cognitive-behavioral skills-based (expectancy challenge programs, self-monitoring, multicomponent alcohol skills training, and general life skills training), and (3) motivational feedback-based (brief motivational interventions and mailed or computerized motivational feedback). Briefly, the review indicated empirical support for multicomponent, skills-based interventions, and in-person, mailed, or computer motivational interventions that provided respondents with personalized feedback about drinking perceptions and tendencies. In contrast, no support was found for general life skills training, values-clarification programs, or informational or knowledge-based programs delivered alone; there was only limited support for expectancy challenge programs (1 study, with male participants only; female participants experienced iatrogenic effects). The review thus highlights the extensive research on preventive interventions conducted with college samples to date.

Outcomes of Interest

For youths ≥10 of age, interventions were included only if the intervention studies incorporated outcome measures of alcohol use or abuse. Interventions were excluded if their outcome assessment included only measures of illegal drug use, smoking, or broad indices of substance or drug use but not direct measures of alcohol use. (When only broad substance abuse indexes were reported, an attempt was made to contact the research team to assess whether other analyses had been conducted to disaggregate findings regarding effects on alcohol use.) In other words, interventions that broadly targeted and measured illegal drug (but not alcohol) use, smoking, sexuality, or health promotion were excluded. If, however, prevention or health promotion programs showed multiple effects that included alcohol-specific measures, then the programs were included in the review.

It is noteworthy in this context that an exception to the alcohol-specific measure requirement was made for interventions that were directed toward policy, law, or environmental changes. In such cases, if the relevant outcome study measured an action that was the logical consequence of alcohol use or abuse behavior (eg, alcohol-related traffic incidents among adolescents), then the study was included. In the case of outcomes concerning alcohol-related harm, we remained cognizant of the fact that harm-related measures could show positive outcomes even in the absence of evidence for decreases in alcohol use.

Because of the relative absence of alcohol use among most children <10 years of age, different outcome measure-related selection criteria were used for interventions designed for this age group. In the case of children <10 years of age, interventions related to key risk factors predicting later alcohol use also were reviewed. On the basis of a review of the relevant etiologic literature, the primary alcohol risk outcome considered was early aggressive behavior, because it is the only risk factor (other than parental alcoholism) that has consistently shown a relationship with early initiation of underage drinking.4550 Early aggressive behaviors include direct aggression, fighting, and hitting, as well as behaviors defined by a broader construct often called either externalizing behavior problems or conduct problems, as reported by teachers, parents, observers, and peers.

Types of Intervention Literature Reviewed

As noted earlier, 3 types of literature were reviewed, to ensure that all relevant evidence on specific interventions was uncovered, including studies of specific interventions, reviews of the outcomes literature (particularly systematic reviews that focused on evidence concerning individual interventions), and summary reports of the evidence on specific interventions produced by agencies conducing evidence-based intervention reviews. First, given the quality assurance inherent in the peer review process, the focus was on refereed professional journals, which were searched via available databases; peer-reviewed research was weighted most heavily. The search of databases included Science Citation Index Expanded, PsycINFO, Medline, and the Social Science Citation Index. For example, >400 abstracts concerning interventions targeting the developmental period of 10 to 15 years of age were reviewed. Additional relevant books and book chapters also were reviewed. Second, literature reviews and meta-analyses (N = 25) were used, such as those cited above,38,51,52 among others.

Third, relevant Internet sources were checked, such as the Web pages of the National Institutes of Health, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, American Psychological Association, Department of Education, Center for the Study and Prevention of Violence (University of Colorado), Society for Prevention Research, Early Career Preventionists’ Network, Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning, and individual armed services branches (for the age group of 16 to ≥20 years of age). These sources were cross-checked against the core group of interventions yielded by the first 2 types of reviews, to identify and to secure articles addressing additional relevant interventions. For each new document obtained, the reference list was reviewed against the list of identified interventions, to avoid omissions.

From these collective sources, a set of core interventions was identified for inclusion in this report. When necessary, the originating research team was contacted during the review process, to address specific questions or to review the information for accuracy. Initially, the review led to the identification and screening of >400 interventions, 127 of which seem to show at least some evidence concerning the desired outcomes. Among those, 41 met the criteria discussed below and thus are included in this report (18 for <10 years of age, 13 for 10–15 years of age, and 10 for 16 to ≥20 years of age).

Intervention Evaluation Criteria

A set of criteria was devised to evaluate interventions uncovered through the literature search process described above. The approach had 3 levels of evidence, that is, (1) most promising, (2) mixed or emerging, and (3) insufficient evidence or no evidence of effects. For an intervention to be considered among the relatively most promising, it was required that 6 criteria or sets of criteria be met through examination of an interventions research base, as follows. The first criterion concerned experimental design, that is, either a randomized trial design or a quasiexperimental design that used an adequate comparison group. The second criterion entailed sample specification, that is, the sample for which outcomes were measured and its behavioral and social characteristics must have been specified. The third criterion concerned outcome assessments, that is, preintervention, postintervention, and follow-up findings must have been included. The need for follow-up findings was considered essential, given the frequently observed dissipation of positive posttest results. We set the criterion that follow-up data must be reported ≥6 months beyond a posttest assessment or ≥6 months beyond the time point at which the primary core components of the intervention were delivered, for examination of the duration and stability of intervention effects. The fourth criterion concerned effects observed, that is, there was a measurable difference in alcohol or alcohol-related outcomes in statistical significance testing. The fifth criterion involved additional quality-of-evidence criteria, that is, evidence that 7 quality-of-evidence criteria consistent with those of the National Registry of Evidenced-based Programs and Practices53 were met, including (1) reliability of outcome measures, (2) validity of outcome measures, (3) pretest equivalence, (4) intervention fidelity, (5) analysis of missing data, (6) degree and evaluation of sample attrition, and (7) appropriate statistical analyses. The sixth criterion concerned manualization, with a written manual that specified the target population and procedures to be used in the intervention, except in the case of law- or policy-focused interventions (eg, minimum drinking age law).

The original plan for this review called for a strong evidence category for programs that met the additional criteria of consistent follow-up impact on alcohol use and independent replication of effects. Sufficiently few programs met these additional criteria that this category of strong evidence was dropped.

Because we concluded that a precise metric for classification of interventions on the basis of their outcome research was problematic (eg, scoring the 6 criteria or sets of criteria with appropriate weighting of individual criteria; see a discussion of issues with scoring systems in the next section), classification was based on our overall judgment, after careful consideration of how well all specified criteria were met. All interventions that failed to meet the criteria for most promising evidence were considered for classification as mixed or emerging. Those that did not meet the criteria for mixed or emerging received no more attention in this evaluation. In addition to how well the criteria delineated above were met, key considerations in classifying interventions as mixed or emerging were as follows.

First, there was a mixture of positive and null intervention condition main effects across studies of a given intervention or across alcohol-related measures within an intervention outcome study. In the case of mixed results, we made a judgment regarding whether a preponderance of evidence favored inclusion. When the preponderance of evidence favored inclusion, the mixed results are noted in the summary tables. Second, there were positive alcohol-related findings but also methodologic limitations that diminished confidence in the validity of reported positive findings to the point that classification as most promising evidence was considered inappropriate. Related methodologic limitations are noted in the summary tables. Third, there were no intervention condition main effects but there were positive effects for a subsample (eg, a high-risk subsample). In such cases, we considered whether possible confounds with subsample analyses were addressed (eg, a higher level of intervention dosage or exposure among a high-risk subsample), as well as whether there was an examination of any subgroups beyond the 1 in which findings were observed (eg, low-risk), for which there could be negative effects. In cases in which parallel positive subgroup findings were reported in subsequent studies, the evidence was considered to be relatively stronger, even if confounding could not be ruled out definitively. Fourth, all studies in which the intervention occurred at <10 years of age and data showed an impact only on the risk factor of aggression and not on later alcohol use were classified as emerging.

Earlier it was noted that interventions directed toward policy, law, or environmental changes warranted special consideration of their alcohol-related outcomes. A review of the literature on interventions involving raising the minimum drinking age and zero-tolerance laws also suggested special attention to criteria for their classification in 1 of the 3 categories designated above. For both of these types of law-based interventions, there were no studies identified that met all of the criteria discussed earlier for classification as most promising or mixed or emerging evidence (eg, regarding experimental design, outcome assessments, effects observed, and the quality-of-evidence criteria consistent with those of the National Registry of Evidenced-based Programs and Practices). In this context, it is important to note that an inherent limitation in the research on policy, law, or environmental types of interventions is that randomized designs with the criteria discussed above are sometimes not feasible. Therefore, we considered studies that had quasiexperimental designs, including longitudinal data collection and multiple data collection points ≥6 months before and after the implementation of the law or policy,54 that had suitable comparison groups,55 or that examined the policies by contrasting multiple school districts.5658

Taking into consideration all relevant design and inference issues, the body of evidence on laws raising the drinking age warranted consideration of classification in the mixed or emerging evidence category. Other laws and policy-focused interventions also were considered, such as mandated server training regarding alcohol crashes,59 alcohol pricing,60 and laws on blood alcohol concentrations of 0.08%.61,62 These were not included in our final report for one or more reasons, that is, the study was not focused on 16- to 20-year-old subjects (eg, mandated training), it assessed outcomes other than the accepted alcohol measures described earlier, or it did not meet the other modified criteria we applied to evaluations of the efficacy of policy, law, and environmental interventions.

The reasons why most interventions were classified as having insufficient or no evidence of effects were wide-ranging. Among the criteria that were least frequently met, and thus led to this classification, were <6 months of follow-up data (as defined above), insignificant effects, weak experimental design, and failure to use alcohol-specific measures (among interventions that originally seemed to show at least some positive evidence concerning substance-related outcomes).

SUMMARY DESCRIPTION OF INTERVENTIONS AND SUPPORTING EVIDENCE

Interventions classified as having the most promising or mixed or emerging evidence are summarized in Tables 1 to to6.6. Detailed analyses of evidence for each of the intervention categories are well beyond the scope and space constraints of the present report. Rather, tabular summary descriptions of all interventions and supportive evidence are provided. Tables 1 and and22 review interventions for subjects ≤10 years of age, Tables 3 and and44 review interventions for those 10 to 15 years of age, and Tables 5 and and66 review those for ages 16 to ≥20 years of age. Each intervention that had sufficient evidence was first categorized as most promising versus mixed or emerging. Each intervention was then designated by type (universal, selective, or indicated) and domain (school, family, community, workplace, or multicomponent). Information is provided on sample size, time of data collection, age/grade level, ethnicity, and urban/rural or other location (where available). Summary results are presented with key citations and Web sites that provide additional information. For mixed or emerging interventions, there is a brief description of the reason for this designation (eg, the type of mixed findings or the methodologic shortcomings).

TABLE 1
Interventions for Children <10 Years of Age With Most Promising Evidence on Alcohol Outcomes
TABLE 2
Interventions for Children <10 Years of Age With Mixed or Emerging Evidence on Alcohol Risk or Alcohol Outcomes
TABLE 3
Interventions for Youths 10 to 15 Years of Age With Most Promising Evidence on Alcohol Outcomes
TABLE 4
Interventions for Youths 10 to 15 Years of Age With Mixed or Emerging Evidence on Alcohol Outcomes
TABLE 5
Interventions for High School Students or Older Participants (16 to ≥20 Years of Age) With Most Promising Evidence on Alcohol Outcomes
TABLE 6
Interventions for High School or Older Participants (16 to ≥20 Years of Age) With Mixed or Emerging Evidence on Alcohol or Alcohol Consequence-Related Outcomes

OVERVIEW OF KEY FINDINGS AND IMPLICATIONS

Topics Covered

The extensive review of the evidence for interventions addressing underage drinking suggested several topics for discussion. Collectively, these topics frame both the summary of our key findings and their salient implications. The first of these topics is the “coverage” of the evidence base or how well it addressed all phases of the 3 developmental periods addressed, intervention domains, and the full range of populations that could benefit from intervention. Additional topics concern the state of the art in intervention research that produced the evidence base, including key research issues and standards of evidence, as well as standards for reporting research in the professional literature.

Coverage of Needed Areas of Intervention Evidence

The review of effective preventive interventions for underage drinking illuminates both the strong scientific advances that have been made in the field of prevention of alcohol use in underage populations in certain areas and the need for better coverage in others. Here our focus is on coverage with respect to intervention domain, developmental phase, and population. Readers are referred to reports by Offord et al63 and others64 for discussion of the relative advantages of the different types of preventive interventions (eg, universal, selective, indicated, or tiered).

Offord et al63 delineated key advantages and disadvantages of universal, selective (or targeted), and indicated (or clinical) interventions, indicating important trade-offs to consider among them. To conduct the suggested trade-off analyses, it is necessary to have data on (1) the prevalence and costs of the problem the intervention addresses, (2) the effectiveness of the intervention, (3) the extent to which the intervention reaches those who need it, (4) the quality of implementation of the intervention (particularly compliance), and (5) intervention costs. As an illustration of related trade-off analyses presented in the article, a universal intervention would likely be a better choice than an indicated or clinical intervention alone when the condition it addresses is highly prevalent, the costs of that condition are high, the intervention is relatively inexpensive, and the intervention has been proven to be effective. In general, Offord et al63 suggested a strategy that entails implementation of effective universal interventions, followed by selective interventions for those who are not sufficiently helped by the universal interventions, and entry into clinical services for those not benefiting from the selective interventions (often referred to as a tiered strategy). The authors concluded by recommending that an optimal mixture of interventions become available.

There are numerous ways to summarize intervention findings, that is, according to developmental periods (<10, 10–15, or 16 to ≥20 years of age), domains (family, school, workplace, community policy/environmental, or multiple domains), or targeted populations. Here we discuss areas where evidence-based intervention is relatively stronger or weaker by focusing on coverage of developmental phases within key domains, with additional attention to coverage of special populations and culturally-based population subgroups or nonmajority populations.

Family-focused interventions delivered in the infant and preschool years have focused primarily on building healthy parent-children relationships, decreasing aggressive behavior, and building children’s social and cognitive competence for the transition to school (eg, The Incredible Years and Triple-P programs). These interventions have shown reductions in children’s aggressive behavior in the short term, whereas only 1 preschool program has shown effects on reduced use of alcohol in the teen years (Nurse Family Partnership). With few exceptions, these early family interventions have evidence limited to the risk precursor of later alcohol use (aggressive behavior).

Although family-focused interventions are prevalent before school entry, there have been fewer family-focused interventions that have been implemented with elementary school-aged children and tested for efficacy, especially those targeting “tweens” during the later elementary school years. A number of family or family-school integrated interventions during the elementary school years, however, have shown effects on either delayed initiation or reduction in alcohol use in adolescence (eg, Linking the Interests of Families and Teachers, Seattle Social Development Project, Raising Healthy Children, and the Preventive Treatment Program). The family interventions that target the period of 10 to 15 years of age and meet the qualifying criteria described above seem to have considerable promise, consistent with the conclusion of the Cochrane systematic review.38 Independent of the targeted developmental phase, these interventions typically address a range of risk and protective factors originating in the family, including child monitoring, parent-child bonding or affective quality, effective discipline, and parental involvement in child activities (eg, Strengthening Families Program: For Parents and Youth 10–14, Guiding Good Choices, and Family Matters). Both small group-format and home-based interventions have been developed; small group interventions have shown relatively stronger evidence. Although no family-based interventions have shown effectiveness with young people 16 to ≥20 years of age who are not college-bound, findings with the college-bound population indicate their potential effectiveness.65

There have been significant advances in the field of school-based prevention. Related findings indicate that such interventions can reduce early initiation of alcohol use and progression of use in the young adolescent and adolescent years. Furthermore, a number of interventions for younger children have shown significant reductions in aggression and disruption, the primary risk precursors of early alcohol use (eg, I Can Problem Solve, Promoting Alternative Thinking Strategies, Second Step, and Good Behavior Game). Interventions that have shown effects typically address the following: role-playing that provides practice in the use of new skills, a broad focus on life skills, support to improve emotional regulation, a focus on positive peer relationships and, with youths, provision of accurate norms for alcohol and substance use, plus instruction in peer refusal skills.

Most elementary school interventions have shown effects only on the risk precursor of aggressive behavior and not on alcohol use. Elementary school interventions have focused primarily on building social competencies and reducing aggressive behaviors. Although a few classroom intervention trials have monitored their samples through the middle-school period and demonstrated effects on alcohol use (eg, Classroom Centered Intervention), most studies have not been funded for a sufficient period to demonstrate whether there are direct effects on alcohol use.

It is noteworthy that we could find no interventions meeting the aforementioned criteria for efficacy or effectiveness that focused on early alcohol use and that provided prevention curricula in the later elementary school years (grades 3 through 5), just before the transition to middle school. Also, although numerous interventions exist that have shown effects on the delay of initiation of use during the middle and early high school periods, there was only 1 intervention that could be classified as most promising66 and 1 that could be classified as mixed or emerging67 in reducing the rate of drinking during the high school years. The latter was limited in that it focused only on high school football players and not on the general population. Given the high rates of binge drinking reported by US high school students, this is an area in need of substantial attention.

Multidomain interventions focus on ≥2 different domains of the child’s or youth’s life (among individual, family, school, worksite, community/environmental, and policy domains). By intervening in multiple domains, it has been hypothesized that the effects of preventive approaches might be maximized (eg, Midwestern Prevention Project and Project Northland). Not surprisingly, such interventions are more likely to occur with less-mobile and -independent younger or middle schoolchildren than with those in high school or older. It is noteworthy that most of the effective interventions in the younger age group used multidomain models (eg, Linking the Interests of Families and Teachers, Fast Track, Seattle Social Development Project, Raising Healthy Children, and Preventive Treatment Program). Although such interventions may be somewhat more difficult to implement with adolescents, this area of research requires additional effort. One promising model for developing multidomain interventions is to combine 2 different interventions with proven efficacy that focus on the separate domains of school and family.24,68 A recent interesting finding concerning 1 of the most-promising multidomain interventions reviewed (Project Northland; see Table 3) was that analyses of components across domains suggested that the relatively strongest effects on tendency toward alcohol use were shown for the parent program component.69

The literature on prevention research often has differentiated preventive interventions (which are usually curricular and teach skills) from policy, law, or environmentally focused interventions (eg, media, regulations, or enforcement). Although there has been much discussion of policy- and environment-level interventions, we were not able to locate any effective policy inventions for children below 16 or 17 years of age; no evidence-based policy interventions that have been shown to delay the initiation of alcohol use or to reduce its early use before the age of high school graduation seem to exist.

We were able to find 2 relatively effective interventions that focused on decreasing sales to minors, increasing identification checks by vendors, or reducing community tolerance of underage purchasing and consumption of alcohol.70,71 Studies of these interventions provided only mixed or emerging evidence, either because of failure to measure specific alcohol use outcomes (or direct logical consequences of use) or because too few communities were studied to allow definitive statements regarding the generalizability of findings. Although media-based interventions have been devised to address drug use (with mixed results)72 and they have been incorporated into multidomain interventions,73 no stand-alone media interventions targeting alcohol use and showing strong evidence could be found. Future research in this area is warranted, especially considering the literature on mass media influences on underage drinking.7476

Concerning the effects of laws raising the minimum drinking age and zero-tolerance laws, the evidence from studies with quasiexperimental designs suggests that minimum legal drinking age laws can reduce rates of underage drinking,58 single-vehicle night time car accidents,55 and fatalities.77 The preventive effects from studies examining the minimum drinking age laws were not completely consistent, however. For example, some studies noted that drinking levels among 18- to 19-year-old students on college campuses remained high after enactment of underage drinking laws 78,79; in other cases, rates of accidents and fatalities remained the same after the change in law.80,81 In addition, the issue of whether drinking was not reduced as a result of these laws but there was a change in where teens drank and how they obtained alcohol has been raised.82 Although our conclusions are consistent with those of other reviews, that the minimum legal drinking age laws seem to have a preventive effect,83,84 these interventions were included in the review as having mixed or emerging evidence, considering the criteria discussed above.

Overall, it is worth underscoring the point there is very limited research on interventions that specifically target emerging alcohol use among late elementary school-aged children, as well as those targeting high school students or young people in the age range of 16 to ≥20 years who are not currently in college. There is very limited intervention research specifically focusing on children in the later elementary school years, despite the indicators for it, as articulated by a number of prevention researchers.85,86 In addition, our review of high school and post–high school interventions focused on the following areas: school-based, community-based, armed services, primary health care settings, alcohol and driving safety, and workplace-based. Despite the broad range of areas investigated and the numerous studies examining the causes of consumption in this developmental period, there were few theory-driven interventions targeted toward young people. Data from large national surveys have consistently indicated that high levels of consumption and misuse of alcohol tend to occur between 17 and 25 years of age.87 There are few non–college-based interventions targeted to this age range. Therefore, there remains a large disconnect between those who are consuming alcohol at high rates and the efforts being undertaken to reduce such practices. Because approximately one half of US individuals in the age range of 18 to 21 years are not attending a 4-year college, future work with this population is greatly needed.

Addressing optimal coverage of evidence-based interventions requires consideration of the optimal mixture of the universal, selective, and indicated types of interventions, as well as the potential role of tiered interventions, wherein universal-level interventions are used as a point of entry to selective interventions, which in turn are used to direct participants toward indicated-level interventions, which are potentially beneficial.63,88,89

Need for Additional Coverage of Cultural Adaptations and Special Populations

It was encouraging to discover a number of interventions with promising or emerging evidence that were designed to be culturally competent for minority populations, that were implemented with other understudied populations (eg, rural), or that otherwise addressed cultural adaptations (eg, Keepin’ It REAL). There is, however, a clear need to strengthen the cultural competency of interventions, as well as the need to develop additional culturally specific interventions in some cases. In addition, there is a need to demonstrate the generalizability of findings of already proven, evidence-based models across cultural groups; some related efforts to date have produced mixed results.90 As part of this process, it will be important to differentiate surface-structure changes (changes in wording, pictures, and stories to represent culturally relevant models) from deep-structure changes (actual changes in the skills, attitudes, cognitions, or policies that may be necessary with different cultural groups).

Key Issues in Current Intervention Research

Overview

It is evident there has been increasing attention to research methods, with attendant improvements in study design and analysis (eg, hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling for studies with cluster randomization and hierarchical data structures). The use of randomized, clinical trials has been crucial to legitimizing prevention efforts by creating greater credibility for the outcomes observed. The current review, however, points to a number of important gaps and other issues to be addressed in future research on the prevention of underage drinking. Most of these issues cut across all types and domains of prevention programs.

Limited Longitudinal Study

The first issue is a need for rigorous studies with longitudinal data that track both the initiation and growth of alcohol use (and abuse) over time. Numerous studies that might have the promise of preventing underage drinking could not be reviewed in this article because they reported only data obtained shortly after completion of the primary intervention. Furthermore, even among those studies that met the criterion of having ≥6 months of follow-up data, there were very few that had extensive, regular, longitudinal data collection that allowed examination of longer-term effects, the possibility of either decay or growth of effects, or the longer-term public health significance of the findings. Also, because the growth of initiation follows a different trajectory and timeline than does heavy use or binge drinking, it is essential to study both of these processes across early to later adolescent periods.

Specificity in Logical Models

The second issue concerns the studies that did have longitudinal findings. There were sometimes mixed results among multiple outcomes from a single wave of data, as well as mixed results across multiple waves of data. Given the rapid changes in the use of alcohol during adolescence, it is not surprising that there is some inconsistency in results across time. Intervention researchers and developers, however, need to specify more clearly the logic of their intervention models and to differentiate more fully the objectives and interventions designed to achieve them, including delay of initiation of use, prevention of regular use or binge drinking, harm reduction, and prevention of alcohol abuse and addiction. It is clear that some interventions, especially those that are universal, may have very specific objectives that follow from the intervention logical model.

Specificity in Self-Reported Outcome Measures and Related Issues

The third issue concerns the need for prevention trial reports to provide more-specific evidence on measures of alcohol use. In a number of instances, programs that might be effective were not reviewed here because the only measures reported were broad omnibus measures of substance abuse, with no specific analyses that differentiated use of alcohol, marijuana, or other illegal substances. Considering that some programs may be quite effective for some substances and not for others, reporting of outcome for each substance used is necessary. It is not sufficient to examine a broad substance abuse index, if the field is to have a better understanding of alcohol use, differentiated from use of other drugs. In addition, the field would benefit from additional work on the validity of self-reports, including setting-specific effects on reporting.91

Limited Replication Study

As indicated earlier in this review, there is a great need for independent replications of the intervention outcome studies reviewed, as well as for standards guiding replication studies.23,92 Independent replications of the interventions reviewed were very rare. The literature specifies different types of replications (eg, exact, scientific, conceptual, and systematic) and discusses their applicability across different phases of prevention research.92 Systematic replications, which entail the study of the effects of systematic variations of intervention procedures, for example, are especially important to consider. As an illustration, a replication study of a school-based intervention93 suggested that an alternative to teacher implementers might be less effective. In addition to the clear need for more replication research, there is a great need to address other issues in this type of research, such as how much difference in intervention content is allowable for a study to be considered a replication study,94 and to develop a set of standards to guide replication research.24,93

Limited Study of Active Ingredients or Core Components and Outcome Mediators

Earlier discussion of the domains of the interventions reviewed discussed multicomponent interventions. Another issue that needs to be addressed concerns the type of intervention that covers >1 domain, such as 1 having components at the family, school, and community levels, like the Midwestern Prevention Program.95 It may be important to assess which components of these interventions are producing the observed effects, considering the capacity and resources they require for effective implementation. A number of approaches to the identification of core or active ingredients have been discussed in the literature, including dismantling designs and factorial designs,96 along with modeling of outcome mediators. Outcome mediator modeling is used to identify key mechanisms of effects by examining which components of interventions (components that target specific, mediating risk or protective factors) account for substantial proportions of the variance in the targeted alcohol-related outcomes.97 Complementing mediational analyses are dose-response evaluations that examine how the dose level of each component of a multicomponent intervention affects outcomes and relative contributions to those outcomes. These types of analyses are especially helpful in determining whether individual components are differentially effective; multicomponent dose-response analyses also can evaluate whether there are synergistic effects among components.69

In the context of considering which intervention components contribute most substantially to targeted outcomes, it also should be noted that some multicomponent interventions have a clear logical model that calls for the multiple components and their synergy; dismantling designs might be especially useful in testing whether the multicomponent models are in fact necessary to achieve positive effects. This issue is rendered more salient by reviews suggesting that single-component, family interventions are among the most effective.38

Limited Economic Analyses

Economic analyses of any kind were conducted with only a limited number of interventions reviewed; even fewer evaluated economic benefits specific to alcohol outcomes.98 Economic analyses99 conducted to date (by the Washington State Institute for Public Policy, for example) clearly indicate the potential cost-effectiveness and cost benefits of a number of preventive interventions. Although a detailed analysis of this issue and its importance is beyond the scope of this report, these types of analyses specific to alcohol-related cost savings would greatly benefit broader dissemination of effective interventions, as discussed below. For example, the decision to adopt an evidence-based intervention can be greatly influenced by the availability of supportive economic analysis data.64

Limited Study of Factors That Moderate Effects

Especially in the case of universal interventions, there is a need to confirm whether intended effects for general populations are achieved across the risk spectrum represented by participating individuals. In cases where benefits to participants are not uniform, the intervention design should be modified100102; this is particularly important at the effectiveness or dissemination phases of research. Relatively more of this type of work has been conducted with school-based interventions than with other types (eg, family focused); research focused on moderation of alcohol-related outcomes is especially limited.16 Moreover, there are limited findings supporting the “universality” of intervention effects on alcohol outcomes, with the possible exception of family-focused interventions.16

Small Samples for Community, Policy, and Environmental Interventions

Of the few community-based studies we reviewed, most were conducted in a single community or a small number of communities.70,103 Although the findings of these studies showed some evidence of efficacy, the small sample sizes in these studies limit the validity and generalizability of the findings. Future efforts should build on this work and examine larger numbers of communities and community heterogeneity, in an attempt to identify what factors might foster or inhibit success for adaptations of community interventions beyond the communities involved in the initial study samples.

Strong Consistent Standards for Evidence and Research Reporting

Need for Consistency in and Broader Application of Evaluation Criteria

During the past few decades, there has been a proliferation of published criteria with which to evaluate the effects of evidence-based interventions, including the summary reports cited above. For example, in the area of evidence-based medicine, a review indicated 20 different scales and 11 different checklists with which reviews assessed the nature of evidence from randomized trials.104 In every case, literature reviews and program lists used somewhat different criteria for the inclusion of effective programs.23 The use of different criteria has resulted in, at most, a moderate degree of overlap across rating groups.105

One way to address inconsistency in the application of standards of evidence is to apply standardized scoring of the quality of evidence. There are several potentially major issues with standardized scoring, however. When many evidence-quality criteria are weighted equally in the scoring, study factors that may pose direct and substantial threats to the validity of outcome conclusions (eg, the quality of design–with designs considered ranging from simple, single-group–before/after designs to randomized, controlled trials, or appropriate treatment of differential attrition) could be weighted the same as factors that may have much more limited relevance to validity (eg, participant expectations). Also, quality criteria that are intended to be applied across all types of interventions (individual, small-group, and policy interventions), across all phases of intervention research (from pilot study to effectiveness trial), and across outcomes at all levels (from individual to system levels) do not allow adequate differentiation of the applicability of the criteria to study-specific characteristics and objectives. For example, information on rating for effect size to indicate the practical significance of outcomes may be less exclusively important in universal intervention studies, where impact is a product of both population reach and effect size.106 Finally, it may be difficult to score specific evaluation criteria readily and reliably in complex studies, such as scoring involving single ratings of reliability and validity for multimethod, multiinformant studies with measures of varying psychometric quality. Therefore, the application of standardized scoring for consistency in the application of standards of evidence warrants additional careful consideration.

In addition, although the field of prevention science has shown great improvement in evaluating programs in the area of substance abuse and mental health, many documents that we reviewed were substandard in a number of ways. We think that it would be helpful to have researchers fully use widely accepted, rigorous standards of evidence. One example is the standards of evidence developed by the Society for Prevention Research regarding the criteria for efficacy, effectiveness, and dissemination.92,107

We refer readers to published documents92 for an in-depth consideration of standards of evidence. Although no single method can be used to assess all interventions, the standards place a high priority on the use of randomized trials, when feasible. The standards also note the importance of using multiple unbiased reporters, examining follow-up effects with a minimal follow-up period of 6 months, fully reporting all outcome data, and taking into account the level of assignment in the method of analysis. Furthermore, to meet the criteria for efficacy, there should be consistent findings in 2 different, high-quality studies that each have adequate statistical power and that demonstrate a consistent pattern of statistically significant findings, in which no serious negative or iatrogenic effects occur and there is some demonstration of the practical public health significance of the findings.

In addition, there are additional standards to be met for an intervention to be considered an effective program or one that is ready for full dissemination or “going to scale.” An effective intervention not only would meet all standards for efficacious interventions but also would (1) offer manuals, appropriate training, and technical support to allow third parties to adopt and to implement the interventions, (2) be evaluated under real-world conditions in studies that include sound measurements of the levels of implementation and engagement of the target audience in both the intervention and control conditions, (3) demonstrate the practical importance of intervention outcome effects, and (4) specify the populations to which intervention findings can be generalized.92

Although meeting the complete set of standards is a goal toward which researchers should aspire, it is recognized that few intervention research programs meet all of the current standards (for example, multiple replications). This is 1 reason why we had designations for interventions as most promising or showing mixed or emerging evidence but not as meeting the complete criteria for effectiveness. As noted earlier, one of the greatest needs is to conduct multiple, independent replications of currently existing programs, to test their efficacy and effectiveness fully.23

Need for Improved Standards Concerning Intervention Replications

Earlier in this report, 2 issues concerning replication research were discussed, namely, the limited amount of replication study overall and limited standards for judging when a replication study is truly a replication study (eg, when a program has been changed substantially and those changes are not being evaluated systematically, a study may not qualify as a replication study). There is a related issue that would benefit from clearer standards, that is, standards for judging when the data for an originally developed intervention are applicable to an intervention that has been revised in significant or substantial ways but has not yet undergone replication study. This issue concerns the applicability of the evidence on efficacy or effectiveness of tests of original versions of interventions in published reports to subsequently revised versions of interventions that have not been studied themselves. It is common for intervention developers and researchers to use process evaluation data and evaluations of intervention-mediating mechanisms to refine interventions after outcome studies. The refined version of the program, not the originally tested version, may be the only one that is available to prospective consumers. Under these circumstances, the question of whether the findings in the published studies are applicable to the currently available version of an intervention arises. Standards to guide an answer to this question would be helpful (eg, guidelines to evaluate the degree to which “active ingredients” of an intervention are affected by revision).

Need for Improved Reporting Standards

The reporting of many of the studies reviewed failed to include many types of information important for the evaluation of evidence in the studies (eg, randomization model or differential attrition). Because of the great concern regarding variation in the quality of reporting in medical and public health trials, there has been a recognized need for a stronger focus on the development of clear criteria for both designs and reporting. Among the most important innovations has been the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) Statement (available at: www.consort-statement.org/index.html). The CONSORT Statement was developed in the health care area and has become the standard for the reporting of randomized, controlled trials in the field of health care and medicine. The CONSORT Statement provides a 22-item checklist for the transparent reporting of randomized, clinical trials. It covers specific aspects of the background, methods, results, and discussion sections. It also provides a model flow diagram to show the progress of all participants in the trial from the time they are randomly assigned until the end of their involvement. This allows readers to see clearly how many subjects are involved at any point in the trial. Since 1996, the CONSORT Statement has been adopted by >150 journals (mostly medical or psychological); although it is subject to additional improvement, it is quite useful. Transparent Reporting of Evaluations with Nonrandomized Designs (www.trend-statement.org/asp/statement.asp) provides a similar kind of model for reporting evaluations with non-randomized trials. The model is consistent with the CONSORT model but is more focused on behavioral interventions.

Although some of the studies we reviewed herein were reported before the establishment of the CONSORT criteria, quite a number were more recent. In numerous cases, there was inconsistent reporting of information on subjects, design, measurement, and analysis. We think that consistent use of the CONSORT model and the Society for Prevention Research standards of evidence would lead to substantial improvement in the validity and interpretability of results.

Adopting Public Health Impact-Oriented Models

Although an essential step in the process of developing effective interventions to prevent underage drinking on a larger scale is the clear demonstration of positive effects for individual interventions, there are additional steps that need to be taken to ensure greater public health impact. Most currently implemented programs and practices do not meet standards of evidence such as those of the Society for Prevention Research.92,107 For example, a number of reviews of the actual implementation of interventions disseminated in school and community settings have shown that only a limited proportion are interventions considered to be evidence-based.108110 Among those that are evidence-based, many are not implemented with sufficient quality to be expected to produce desired, long-term, alcohol reduction outcomes.36,111,112 Furthermore, among the evidence-based interventions implemented initially with quality in a community setting, few are implemented with quality on a sustained basis. When the total group of interventions designed to address underage drinking, across all developmental stages, is considered, only a very small proportion of interventions are evidence-based and implemented with quality in sustained ways.43,113

Additional steps entail translating effective interventions into widespread practice, effective interventions that ultimately have the kind of broad coverage suggested above (across developmental phases, domains, and populations). Key among these additional steps is substantial expansion of the knowledge base regarding factors influencing dissemination of evidence-based interventions and sustained quality implementation of them, guided by current intervention research models tailored to specific phases of research.64

To achieve large-scale public impact, existing models of preventive intervention research44 could be adapted to enhance the likelihood of such impact. The Institute of Medicine model specifies that developmental and etiologic theories guide the design and pilot testing of interventions. After refinement on the basis of pilot test results, interventions are subjected to rigorous testing intended to evaluate their efficacy. This is followed by replication and effectiveness studies that evaluate the extent to which the intervention is efficacious for different populations in different settings, after which the intervention is ready for the final step of dissemination.

There is extensive literature on factors that promote effective dissemination of evidence-based interventions, to guide the achievement of broad, population-based, public health impact.40,114 Such factors include the readiness and capacity of organizations for implementation, the quality of training and technical assistance, the level of opinion leadership, and support from administrators in the implementation system. The relevant literature also incorporates guidance on factors influencing the quality of implementation of evidence-based interventions,36,111,115,116 as well as the sustainability of quality implementation.117 In addition, there is guidance from this body of literature on addressing barriers to dissemination of evidence-based public health interventions and carefully considering how to adapt dissemination strategies that were designed originally for the health care field.118

Of great relevance to the achievement of public health impact through dissemination of evidence-based interventions are emerging models that build on the traditional Institute of Medicine model45 of the phases of intervention development and evaluation summarized above. These emerging models entail greater emphasis on community participatory- and consumer-oriented research, from the earliest formative phases of research forward.24,119121 Emerging models focus on better integration of private enterprise procedures for product development and marketing120 or service development models121 that, much like health care,122 incorporate careful consideration of consumer, provider, and funder issues of relevance. These considerations may be useful for optimizing effective, broad-based dissemination.

In addition to broader application of emerging models of intervention development, testing, and dissemination, progress toward public health impact would be facilitated by more-extensive and -consistent evaluation of “dissemination values” at each phase of research; an applicable set of procedures and methods has been developed123 for this purpose. Also of relevance is a community-university partnership model implemented through the outreach and dissemination arm of the national land-grant university system linked with public school systems.124 Data indicate that community teams supported by the community-university partnerships can effectively engage prospective intervention participants in evidence-based interventions that can be implemented with quality on a sustained basis, with a range of positive community-level outcomes.112,116 Most importantly, much more emphasis is needed on the translational function of intervention-related research, defined as the translation of research from basic causes (eg, peer and family influences on young adolescent decisions to drink alcohol) to real-world applications.125 The translational function centers on translating science into widespread prevention practice. This requires transdisciplinary research105 that could serve to shift the field of prevention of underage drinking toward a paradigm emphasizing the social value of translating science into practice with public health impact, or following a translational impact paradigm designed to accelerate the rate of population-level effects.113

To summarize the range of current models for dissemination of evidence-based preventive intervention, oriented toward translation of science into practice, offer great promise for taking critically important steps to achieving public health impact through reduced underage drinking. Implementing these models warrants increased federal resources and the use of innovative funding mechanisms, such as those that “braid” funding for evidence-based services with that for preventive intervention research (eg, see www.preventionresearch.org).

CONCLUSIONS

This review indicated that a number of preventive interventions, particularly universal and selective ones, significantly reduced the rate of alcohol use in studied underage populations, as well as bolstered protective factors among children that reduce risks for alcohol use. The review also underscores a number of advances in preventive interventions to address underage drinking over the past 15 years, advances that reflect progression of the field of prevention more generally. For example, there have been substantial methodologic improvements in study design and analyses, along with the use of randomized, clinical trials that have been crucial to legitimizing prevention efforts by enhancing the credibility of reported outcomes. In addition, there is an expanding armamentarium of interventions that are ready for dissemination, as illustrated by the current number of carefully manualized, replicable models of intervention presented in Tables 1 to to6.6. The growing number of evidence-based interventions reflects progress in the field of prevention science and its application to public health issues.43,44,126,127

Reaching the potential suggested by recent advances will require careful attention to needed work indicated by this review, such as filling the gaps in the intervention evidence base, particularly for early tweens, late teens, and young adults who are not in college and for nonmajority populations, addressing critical research issues, and promulgating stronger, more consistently applied standards of evidence and reporting. In particular, it will require the application of emerging models for engaging consumers, providers, funders, and scientists in an enterprise oriented toward real-world impact. A public health approach of this kind has several salient features, that is, ecologically valid, evidence-based, preventive interventions on a large scale, well integrated across individual, family, school, work-place, and community domains. Most importantly, it has the necessary infrastructure and capacity-building to support ongoing research and sustained, quality implementation of interventions, at the community, state, and national levels. A strategy for mobilizing community, state, and federal resources to accomplish such an impact clearly is indicated.

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge the very helpful feedback from Vivian Faden, Ralph Hingston, Cheryl Perry, and Mark Eddy on an earlier version of this manuscript and the valuable assistance of Tony Jung, Leslie Winjum, Pandora Lamar, Nadine Mastroleo, Anne Ray, Jerod Stapleton, Caitlin Abar, Rachel Bachrach, and Kathryn Peters in conducting the extensive literature searches, compiling resultant information, and typing this report.

Abbreviation

CONSORT
Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials

Footnotes

The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

References

1. Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future: National Results on Adolescent Drug Use: Overview of Key Findings. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse; 2006. NIH publication 06–5882.
2. Hawks D, Scott K, McBride N, Jones P, Stockwell T. Prevention of Psychoactive Substance Use: A Selected Review of What Works in the Area of Prevention. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2002.
3. Hanson GR, Li T. Public health implications of excessive alcohol consumption. JAMA. 2003;289(8):1031–1032. [PubMed]
4. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Youth Drinking: Risk Factors and Consequences: Alcohol Alert 37. Washington, DC: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; 1997.
5. National Institute of Mental Health. Mental Health: A Report of the Surgeon General. Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health; 1999.
6. Office of the Surgeon General. Surgeon General’s Call to Action on Preventing Underage Drinking. Washington, DC: Office of the Surgeon General; 2006.
7. Hingson R, Kenkel D. National Research Council, Institute of Medicine. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility: Background Papers [CD-ROM] Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2004. Social, health, and economic consequences of underage drinking.
8. Grunbaum JA, Kann L, Kindun SA, et al. Youth risk behavior surveillance: United States, 2001. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2002;51(4):1–62. [PubMed]
9. Hingson RW, Zakocs RC, Heeren T, Winter MR, Rosenbloom D, DeJong W. Effects on alcohol related fatal crashes of a community based initiative to increase substance abuse treatment and reduce alcohol availability. Inj Prev. 2005;11(2):84–90. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
10. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Alcohol, aggression, and injury. Alcohol Health Res World. 1993;17(2)
11. Dee TS. Alcohol abuse and economic conditions: evidence from repeated cross-sections of individual-level data. Health Econ. 2001;10(3):257–270. [PubMed]
12. Windle M. Temperament and social support in adolescence: interrelations with depressive symptoms and delinquent behaviors. J Youth Adolesc. 1992;21(1):1–21. [PubMed]
13. Windle M, Windle RC. Depressive symptoms and cigarette smoking among middle adolescents: prospective associations and intrapersonal and interpersonal influences. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2001;69(2):215–226. [PubMed]
14. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. SAMHSA model programs. [Accessed February 15, 2008]. Available at: www.nrepp.samhsa.gov.
15. Hingson R, Heeren T, Zakocs R, Kopstein A, Wechsler H. Magnitude of alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among US college students ages 18–24. J Stud Alcohol. 2002;63(2):136–144. [PubMed]
16. Spoth R, Shin C, Guyll M, Redmond C, Azevedo K. Universality of effects: an examination of the comparability of long-term family intervention effects on substance use across risk-related subgroups. Prev Sci. 2006;7(2):209–224. [PubMed]
17. Swartzwelder HS, Wilson WA, Tayyeb MI. Age-dependent inhibition of long-term potentiation by ethanol in immature versus mature hippocampus. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1995;19(6):1480–1485. [PubMed]
18. Foster SE, Vaughan RD, Foster WH, Califano JA. Alcohol consumption and expenditure for underage drinking and adult excessive drinking. JAMA. 2003;289(8):989–995. [PubMed]
19. Levy DT, Miller TR, Cox KC. Costs of Underage Drinking. Rockville, MD: US Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention; 1999.
20. Tapert SF, Caldwell L, Burke C. Alcohol and the adolescent brain: human studies. Alcohol Res Health. 2004/2005. [Accessed February 12, 2008]. Available at: http://pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/publications/arh284/toc28-4.htm.
21. Tapert SF, Schweinsburg AD. The human adolescent brain and alcohol use disorders. In: Galanter M, editor. Alcohol Problems in Adolescents and Young Adults: Epidemiology, Neurobiology, Prevention, and Treatment. New York, NY: Springer Science; 2006. pp. 177–197.
22. Grant BF, Dawson DA. Age at onset of alcohol use and its association with DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence: results from the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. J Subst Abuse. 1997;9:103–110. [PubMed]
23. Greenberg MT. Current and future challenges in school-based prevention: the researcher perspective. Prev Sci. 2004;5(1):5–13. [PubMed]
24. Spoth RL, Greenberg MT. Toward a comprehensive strategy for effective practitioner-scientist partnerships and larger-scale community benefits. Am J Community Psychol. 2005;35(3/4):107–126. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
25. Blitz CC, Arthur MW, Hawkins JD. Preventing alcohol, tobacco, and other substance abuse. In: Jason LA, Glenwick DS, editors. Innovative Strategies for Promoting Health and Mental Health Across the Life Span. New York, NY: Springer; 2002. pp. 176–201.
26. Gottfredson DC, Wilson DB. Characteristics of effective school-based substance abuse prevention. Prev Sci. 2003;4(1):27–38. [PubMed]
27. Larimer ME, Cronce JM. Identification, prevention, and treatment revisited: individual-focused college drinking prevention. Addict Behav. 2002;32(11):2439–2468. [PubMed]
28. Lochman JE, van den Steenhoven A. Family-based approaches to substance abuse prevention. J Prim Prev. 2002;23(1):49–114.
29. Paglia A, Room R. Preventing substance use problems among youth: a literature review and recommendations. J Prim Prev. 1999;20(1):3–50.
30. Skara S, Sussman S. A review of 25 long-term adolescent tobacco and other drug use prevention program evaluations. Prev Med. 2003;37(5):451–474. [PubMed]
31. Tobler NS, Roona MR, Ochshorn P, Marshall DG, Streke AV, Stackpole KM. School-based adolescent drug prevention programs: 1998 meta-analysis. J Prim Prev. 2000;20(4):275–336.
32. Velleman RD, Templeton LJ, Copello AG. The role of the family in preventing and intervening with substance use and misuse: a comprehensive review of family interventions, with a focus on young people. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2005;24(2):93–109. [PubMed]
33. Elliott DS, editor. Blueprints for Violence Prevention, Vol 5: Life Skills Training. Boulder, CO: Center for the Study of Prevention and Violence, University of Colorado; 1998. [Accessed February 28, 2008]. Available at: www.colorado.edu/cspv/blueprints/model/programs/LST.html.
34. Wilson DB, Gottfredson DC, Najaka SS. School-based prevention of problem behaviors: a meta-analysis. J Quant Criminol. 2001;17(3):247–272.
35. Durlak JA, Wells AM. Primary prevention mental health programs: the future is exciting. Am J Community Psychol. 1997;25(2):233–243. [PubMed]
36. Greenberg MT, Domitrovich C, Bumbarger B. The prevention of mental disorders in school-aged children: current state of the field. [Accessed February 16, 2008]. Available at: http://prevention.psu.edu/pubs/doc/CMHS.pdf.
37. Catalano RF, Berglund ML, Ryan JAM, Lonczak HC, Hawkins JD. Positive Youth Development in the United States: Research Findings on Evaluations of Positive Youth Development Programs. Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Services; 1998.
38. Foxcroft DR, Ireland D, Lister-Sharp DJ, Lowe G, Breen R. Longer-term primary prevention for alcohol misuse in young people: a systematic review. Addiction. 2003;94(4):397–411. [PubMed]
39. Komro KA, Toomey TL. Strategies to prevent underage drinking. Alcohol Res Health. 2002;26(1):5–14. [PubMed]
40. Elliott DS, Mihalic S. Issues in disseminating and replicating effective prevention programs. Prev Sci. 2004;5(1):47–53. [PubMed]
41. Barkham M, Margison F, Leach C, et al. Service profiling and outcomes benchmarking using the CORE-OM: toward practice-based evidence in the psychological therapies. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2001;68(2):184–196. [PubMed]
42. National Center for Education Statistics. Violence and Discipline Problems in US Public Schools: 1996–97. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office; 1998. NCES publication 98-030.
43. Federal Collaboration on What Works. Draft Report of the Working Group: The What Works Repository. Washington, DC: Office of Justice Programs; 2005.
44. Mrazek PJ, Haggerty RJ, editors. Reducing Risks for Mental Disorders: Frontiers for Preventive Intervention Research. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1994.
45. Clark DB, Cornelius JR, Kirisci L, Tarter RE. Childhood risk categories for adolescent substance involvement: a general liability typology. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005;77(1):13–21. [PubMed]
46. Clark DB, Winters KC. Measuring risks and outcomes in substance use disorders prevention research. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2002;70(6):1207–1223. [PubMed]
47. Dobkin PL, Tremblay RE, Masse LC, Vitaro F. Individual and peer characteristics in predicting boys’ early onset of substance abuse: a seven-year longitudinal study. Child Dev. 1995;66(4):1198–1214. [PubMed]
48. Kaplow JB, Curran PJ, Dodge KA. Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. Child, parent, and peer predictors of early-onset substance use: a multi-site longitudinal study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2002;30(3):199–216. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
49. Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. Evaluation of the first three years of the Fast Track Prevention Trial with children at high risk for adolescent conduct problems. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2002;30(1):19–35. [PubMed]
50. Zucker RA, Ellis DA, Bingham CR, Fitzgerald HE. The development of alcoholic subtypes: risk variation among alcoholic families during early childhood. Alcohol Health Res World. 1996;20(1):46–54.
51. Catalano RF, Berglund ML, Ryan JAM, Lonczak HS, Hawkins JD. Positive youth development in the United States: research findings on evaluations of positive youth development programs. Prev Treat. 2002. [Accessed February 28, 2008]. Available at: http://content.apa.org/journals/pre/5/1.
52. Weissberg RP, Greenberg MT. Community and school prevention. In: Siegel I, Renninger A, editors. Handbook of Child Psychology, Vol. 4: Child Psychology in Practice. 5. New York, NY: Wiley; 1998. pp. 877–954.
53. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. NREPP Overview. Rockville, MD: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration; 2006. [Accessed February 12, 2008]. Available at: www.nrepp.samhsa.gov/review-criteria.htm.
54. Yu J, Shacket RW. Long-term change in underage drinking and impaired driving after the establishment of drinking age laws in New York State. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1998;22(7):1443–1449. [PubMed]
55. Hingson RW, Scotch N, Mangoine T, et al. Impact of legislation raising the legal drinking age in Massachusetts from 18 to 20. Am J Public Health. 1983;73(2):163–170. [PubMed]
56. Dee TS. State alcohol policies, teen drinking and traffic fatalities. J Public Econ. 1999;72(2):289–315.
57. Engs RC, Hanson DJ. University students’ drinking patterns and problems: examining the effects of raising the purchase age. Public Health Rep. 1988;103(6):667–673. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
58. O’Malley PM, Wagenaar AC. Effects of minimum drinking age laws on alcohol use, related behaviors and traffic crash involvement among American youth: 1976–1987. J Stud Alcohol. 1991;52(5):478–491. [PubMed]
59. Holder HD, Wagenaar A. Mandated server training and reduced alcohol-involved traffic crashes: a time series analysis of the Oregon experience. Accid Anal Prev. 1994;26(1):89–97. [PubMed]
60. Chaloupka FJ, Wechsler H. Binge drinking in college: the impact of price, availability, and alcohol control policies. Contemp Econ Policy. 1996;14(4):112–124.
61. Hingson RW, Heeren T, Winter MR. Lower blood alcohol limits for young drivers. Public Health Rep. 1994;109(6):738–744. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
62. Hingson RW, Heeren T, Winter MR. Lowering state legal blood alcohol limits to 0.08%: the effect on fatal motor vehicle crashes. Am J Public Health. 1996;86(9):1297–1299. [PubMed]
63. Offord DR, Kraemer HC, Kazdin AE, Jensen PS, Harrington R. Lowering the burden of suffering from child psychiatric disorder: trade-offs among clinical, targeted, and universal interventions. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;37(7):686–694. [PubMed]
64. Kellam SG, Langevin DJ. A framework for understanding “evidence” in prevention research and programs. Prev Sci. 2003;4(3):137–153. [PubMed]
65. Turrisi R, Jaccard J, Taki R, Dunnam H, Grimes J. Examination of the short-term efficacy of a parent-based intervention to reduce college student drinking tendencies. Psychol Addict Behav. 2001;15(4):366–372. [PubMed]
66. Sussman S, Dent CW, Stacy AW. Project Towards No Drug Abuse: a review of the findings and future directions. Am J Health Behav. 2002;26(5):354–364. [PubMed]
67. Goldberg L, MacKinnon DP, Elliot DL, Esther LM, Clark G, Cheong J. The Adolescent Training and Learning to Avoid Steroids program. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2000;154(4):332–338. [PubMed]
68. Spoth RL, Redmond C, Trudeau L, Shin C. Longitudinal substance initiation outcomes for a universal preventive intervention combining family and school programs. Psychol Addict Behav. 2002;16(2):129–134. [PubMed]
69. Stigler MH, Perry CL, Komro KA, Cudeck R, Williams CL. Teasing apart a multiple component approach to adolescent alcohol prevention: what worked in Project Northland? Prev Sci. 2006;7(3):269–280. [PubMed]
70. Holder HD, Gruenewald PJ, Ponicki WR, et al. Effect of community-based interventions on high-risk drinking and alcohol-related injuries. JAMA. 2000;284(18):2341–2347. [PubMed]
71. Wagenaar AC, Gehan JP, Jones-Webb R, et al. Communities Mobilizing for Change on Alcohol: lessons and results from a 15-community randomized trial. J Community Psychol. 1999;27(3):315–326.
72. Evans DL, Foa EB, Guy RE, et al. Treating and Preventing Adolescent Mental Health Disorders: What We Know and What We Don’t Know. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2005.
73. Pentz MA, Dwyer JH, MacKinnon DP, et al. A multi-community trial for primary prevention of adolescent drug abuse: effects on drug use prevalence. JAMA. 1989;261(22):3259–3266. [PubMed]
74. Center on Alcohol Marketing and Youth. Underage Drinking in the United States: A Status Report, 2005. Washington, DC: Center on Alcohol Marketing and Youth; 2006.
75. Christenson PG, Henriksen L, Roberts DF. Substance Use in Popular Prime Time Television. Washington, DC: Office of National Drug Control Policy; 2000.
76. Grube JW, Wallack L. Television beer advertising and drinking knowledge, beliefs, and intentions among schoolchildren. Am J Public Health. 1994;84(2):254–259. [PubMed]
77. Decker MD, Graitcer PL, Schaffner W. Reduction in motor vehicle fatalities associated with an increase in the minimum drinking age. JAMA. 1988;260(24):3604–3610. [PubMed]
78. Perkins HW, Berkowitz A. Stability and contradiction in college students’ drinking following a drinking age law change. J Alcohol Drug Educ. 1989;35(1):60–77.
79. Gonzalez GM. Effects of raising the drinking age among college students in Florida. Coll Stud J. 1989;23(1):67–75.
80. Vingilis E, Smart RG. Effects of raising the legal drinking age in Ontario. Br J Addict. 1981;76(4):415–424. [PubMed]
81. Ruhm CJ. Alcohol policies and highway vehicle fatalities. J Health Econ. 1996;15(4):435–454. [PubMed]
82. Smith RA, Hingson RW, Morelock S, et al. Legislation raising the legal drinking age in Massachusetts from 18 to 20: effect on 16 and 17 year-olds. J Stud Alcohol. 1984;45(6):534–539. [PubMed]
83. Wagenaar AC, Toomey TL. Effects of minimum drinking age laws: review and analyses of the literature from 1960 to 2000. J Stud Alcohol Suppl. 2002;(14):206–225. [PubMed]
84. Shultz RA, Elder RW, Sleet DA, et al. Reviews of evidence regarding interventions to reduce alcohol-impaired driving. Am J Prev Med. 2001;21(4 suppl):66–88. [PubMed]
85. Hawkins JD, Graham JW, Maguin E, Abbott R, Hill KG, Catalano RF. Exploring the effects of age of alcohol use initiation and psychosocial risk factors on subsequent alcohol misuse. J Stud Alcohol. 1997;58(3):280–290. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
86. Oxford ML, Harachi TW, Catalano RF, Abbott RD. Preadolescent predictors of substance initiation: a test of both the direct and mediated effect of family social control factors on deviant peer associations and substance initiation. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2001;27(4):599–616. [PubMed]
87. O’Malley PM, Johnston LD. Epidemiology of alcohol and other drug use among college students. J Stud Alcohol Suppl. 2002;(14):23–39. [PubMed]
88. Dishion TJ, Kavanagh K. Intervening in Adolescent Problem Behavior: A Family-Centered Approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2003.
89. Sanders MR, Markie-Dadds C, Tully L, Bor W. The Triple P Positive Parenting Program: a comparison of enhanced, standard, and self-directed behavioral family intervention for parents of children with early onset conduct problems. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2000;68(4):624–640. [PubMed]
90. Kumpfer KL, Alvarado R, Smith P, Bellamy N. Cultural sensitivity and adaptation in family-based prevention interventions. Prev Sci. 2002;3(3):241–246. [PubMed]
91. Azevedo K, Redmond C, Lillehoj C, Spoth RL. Contradictions across in-school and in-home reports of adolescent substance use initiation. Presented at the Society for Prevention Research 11th Annual Meeting; June 11–13, 2003; Washington, DC.
92. Flay BR, Biglan A, Boruch RF, et al. Standards of evidence: criteria for efficacy, effectiveness and dissemination. Prev Sci. 2005;6(3):151–175. [PubMed]
93. St Pierre TL, Osgood DW, Mincemoyer CC, Kaltreider DL, Kauh TJ. Results of an independent evaluation of Project ALERT delivered in schools by cooperative extension. Prev Sci. 2005;6(4):305–317. [PubMed]
94. Hawkins JD. When is replication, replication? The Seattle Social Development Project and the Raising Healthy Children Project. Presented at the Society for Prevention Research-Cornell University Medical Center Institute for Prevention Research conference; April 19–21, 2004; Snowbird, UT.
95. Pentz MA, Trebow EA, Hansen WB, et al. Effects of program implementation on adolescent drug use behavior: the Mid-western Prevention Project (MPP) Eval Rev. 1990;14(3):264–289.
96. Collins LM, Murphy SA, Nair V, Strecher V. A strategy for optimizing and evaluating behavioral interventions. Ann Behav Med. 2005;30(1):65–73. [PubMed]
97. Komro KA, Perry CL, Williams MH, Stigler K, Farbakhsh K, Veblen-Mortenson S. How did Project Northland reduce alcohol use among young adolescents? Analysis of mediating variables. Health Educ Res. 2001;16(1):59–70. [PubMed]
98. Spoth R, Guyll M, Day SX. Universal family-focused interventions in alcohol-use disorder prevention: cost-effectiveness and cost-benefit analyses of two interventions. J Stud Alcohol. 2002;63(2):219–228. [PubMed]
99. Aos S, Lieb R, Mayfield M, Miller M, Pennucci A. Benefits and costs of prevention and early intervention programs for youth, technical appendix. Olympia, WA: Washington State Institute for Public Policy; 2004.
100. Brown CH, Liao J. Principles for designing randomized preventive trials in mental health: an emerging developmental epidemiology paradigm. Am J Community Psychol. 1999;27(5):673–710. [PubMed]
101. Dawson-McClure SR, Sandler IN, Wolchik SA, Millsap RE. Risk as a moderator of the effects of prevention programs for children from divorced families: a six-year longitudinal study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2004;32(2):175–190. [PubMed]
102. Kellam SG, Rebok GW. Building developmental and etiological theory through epidemiologically based preventive intervention trials. In: McCord J, Tremblay RE, editors. Preventing Antisocial Behavior: Interventions From Birth Through Adolescence. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1992. pp. 162–195.
103. Grube JW. Preventing sales of alcohol to minors: results from a community trial. Addiction. 1997;92(suppl 2):S251–S260. [PubMed]
104. Lohr KN. Rating the strength of scientific evidence: relevance for quality improvement programs. Int J Qual Health Care. 2004;16(1):9–18. [PubMed]
105. Mihalic S. Matrix of Programs as Identified by Various Federal and Private Agencies. Boulder, CO: Center for the Study and Prevention of Violence, University of Colorado; 2002–2004. [Accessed February 13, 2008]. Available at: www.colorado.edu/cspv/blueprints/matrix/matrix.pdf.
106. Abrams DB. Transdisciplinary paradigms for tobacco prevention research. Nicotine Tob Res. 1999;1(suppl 1):S15–S23. [PubMed]
107. Society for Prevention Research. Standards of Evidence: Criteria for Efficacy, Effectiveness and Dissemination. Falls Church, VA: Society for Prevention Research; 2004.
108. Ennett ST, Ringwalt CL, Thorne J, et al. A comparison of current practice in school-based substance use prevention programs with meta-analysis findings. Prev Sci. 2003;4(1):1–14. [PubMed]
109. Gottfredson DC, Najaka SS. Youth Strategies Consolidated Grant: Evaluation Report for First Funding Cycle. College Park, MD: Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice, University of Maryland; 2003.
110. Hallfors D, Cho H, Livert D, Kadushin C. Fighting back against substance abuse: are community coalitions winning? Am J Prev Med. 2002;23(4):237–245. [PubMed]
111. Fixsen DL, Naoom SF, Blase KA, Friedman RM, Wallace F. Implementation Research: A Synthesis of the Literature. Tampa, FL: Louis de la Parte Florida Mental Health Institute; 2005. FMHI publication 231.
112. Spoth R, Randall GK, Shin C. Experimental support for a model of partnership-based family intervention effects on long-term academic success. School Psychol Q. 2007;22(3):1–30.
113. Spoth R. Translation of family-focused prevention science into practice: plotting a course for a public health impact paradigm. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. In press.
114. Dearing JW. Improving the state of health programming by using diffusion theory. J Health Commun. 2004;9(1):21–36. [PubMed]
115. Adelman HS, Taylor L. On sustainability of project innovations as systemic change. J Educ Psychol Consult. 2003;14(1):1–25.
116. Spoth R, Guyll M, Lillehoj CJ, Redmond C, Greenberg M. PROSPER study of evidence-based intervention implementation quality by community-university partnerships. J Community Psychol. 2007;35(8):981–999. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
117. Scheirer MA. Is sustainability possible? A review and commentary on empirical studies of program sustainability. Am J Eval. 2005;26(3):320–347.
118. Green LW. From research to “best practices” in other settings and populations. Am J Health Behav. 2001;25(3):165–178. [PubMed]
119. Greenberg MT, Weissberg RP, O’Brien MU, et al. Enhancing school-based prevention and youth development through coordinated social, emotional, and academic learning. Am Psychol. 2003;58(6–7):466–474. [PubMed]
120. Rotheram-Borus MJ, Duan N. Next generation of preventive interventions. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003;42(5):518–530. [PubMed]
121. Sandler I, Ostrom A, Bitner MJ, Ayers TS, Wolchik S, Daniels VS. Developing effective prevention services for the real world: a prevention service development model. Am J Community Psychol. 2005;35(3–4):127–142. [PubMed]
122. Herzlinger RE. Consumer-Driven Health Care: Implications for Providers, Payers, and Policy-Makers. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 2004.
123. Glasgow RE, Klesges LM, Dzewaltowski DA, Bull SS, Estabrooks P. The future of health behavior change research: what is needed to improve translation of research into health promotion practice. Ann Behav Med. 2004;27(1):3–12. [PubMed]
124. Spoth R, Greenberg M, Bierman K, Redmond C. PROSPER community-university partnership model for public education systems: capacity-building for evidence-based, competence-building prevention. Prev Sci. 2004;5(1):31–39. [PubMed]
125. Sussman S, Valente TW, Rohrbach LA, Skara S, Pentz MA. Translation in the health professions: converting science into action. Eval Health Prof. 2006;29(1):7–32. [PubMed]
126. Biglan A, Mrazek PJ, Carnine D, Flay BR. The integration of research and practice in the prevention of youth problem behaviors. Am Psychol. 2003;58(6–7):433–440. [PubMed]
127. Coie JD, Watt NF, West SG, et al. The science of prevention: a conceptual framework and some directions for a national research program. Am Psychol. 1993;48(10):1013–1022. [PubMed]
128. Brown EC, Catalano RF, Fleming CB, Haggerty KH, Abbott RD. Adolescent substance use outcomes in the Raising Healthy Children Project: a two-part latent growth curve analysis. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2005;73(4):699–710. [PubMed]
129. Hawkins JD, Von Cleve E, Catalano RF. Reducing early childhood aggression: results of a primary prevention project. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1991;30(2):208–217. [PubMed]
130. Eddy MJ, Reid JR, Fetrow RA. An elementary school-based prevention program targeting modifiable antecedents of youth delinquency and violence: Linking the Interests of Families and Teachers (LIFT) J Emot Behav Disord. 2000;8(3):165–176.
131. Eddy MJ, Reid JR, Stoolmiller M, Fetrow RA. Outcomes during middle school for an elementary school-based preventive intervention for conduct problems: follow-up results from a randomized trial. Behav Ther. 2003;34(4):535–552.
132. Catalano RF, Mazza JJ, Harachi TW, Abbott RD, Haggerty KH, Fleming CB. Raising healthy children through enhancing social development in elementary school: results after 1.5 years. J Sch Psychol. 2003;41(2):143–164.
133. Hawkins JD, Catalano RF, Morrison DM, O’Donnell J, Abbott RD, Day LE. The Seattle Social Development Project: effects of the first four years on protective factors and problem behaviors. In: McCord J, Tremblay RE, editors. Preventing Antisocial Behavior: Interventions From Birth Through Adolescence. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1992. pp. 139–161.
134. Olds D, Henderson CR, Cole R, et al. Long-term effects of nurse home visitation on children’s criminal and antisocial behavior: 15-year follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 1998;280(14):1238–1244. [PubMed]
135. Tremblay RE, Masse L, Pagani L, Vitaro F. From childhood physical aggression to adolescent maladjustment: the Montreal Prevention Experiment. In: Peters RD, McMahon RJ, editors. Preventing Childhood Disorders, Substance Abuse, and Delinquency. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1996. pp. 268–298.
136. Ialongo NS, Podska J, Werthamer L, Kellam S. The distal impact of two first-grade preventive interventions on conduct problems and disorder in early adolescence. J Emot Behav Disord. 2001;9(3):146–160.
137. Furr-Holden CD, Ialongo NS, Anthony JC, Petras H, Kellam SG. Developmentally inspired drug prevention: middle school outcomes in a school-based randomized trial. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2004;73(2):149–158. [PubMed]
138. Kratchowill TR, McDonald L, Levin JR, Young Bear-Tibbetts H, Demaray MK. Families and Schools Together: an experimental analysis of a parent-mediated multi-family group program for American Indian children. J Sch Psychol. 2004;42(5):359–383.
139. Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. Initial impact of the Fast Track prevention trial for conduct problems, part I: the high-risk sample. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1999;67(5):631–647. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
140. Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. The effects of the Fast Track Program on serious problem outcomes at the end of elementary school. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2004;33(4):650–661. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
141. Walker HM, Forness SR, Kauffman JM, et al. Macro-social validation: referencing outcomes in behavioral disorders to societal issues and problems. Behav Disord. 1998;24(1):7–18.
142. Kellam SG, Rebok GW, Ialongo N, Mayer LS. The course and malleability of aggressive behavior from early first grade into middle school: results of a developmental epidemiologically-based prevention trial. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1994;35(2):259–281. [PubMed]
143. Kellam SG, Ling X, Mersica R, Brown HC, Ialongo N. The effect of level of aggression in the first grade classroom on the course and malleability of aggressive behavior in middle school. Dev Psychopathol. 1998;10(2):165–185. [PubMed]
144. Shure MB. Interpersonal problem solving in young children: a cognitive approach to prevention. Am J Community Psychol. 1982;10(3):341–356. [PubMed]
145. Olweus D. Bully/victim problems among schoolchildren: basic facts and effects of a school-based intervention program. In: Pepler DJ, Rubin KH, editors. The Development and Treatment of Childhood Aggression. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1991. pp. 411–448.
146. Olweus D, Limber SP, Mihalic S. Blueprints for Violence Prevention, Vol. 10: The Bullying Prevention Program. Boulder, CO: Center for the Study and Prevention of Violence; 1999.
147. Smith EA, Swisher JD, Vicary JR, et al. Evaluation of Life Skills Training and Infused-Life Skills Training in a rural setting: outcomes at two years. J Alcohol Drug Educ. 2004;48(1):51–70.
148. Schweinhart LJ, Weikart DP. Young Children Grow Up: The Effects of the Perry Preschool Program on Youths Through Age 15. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press; 1980.
149. Schweinhart LJ, Barnes HV, Weikart DP. Significant Benefits: The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study Through Age 27. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press; 1993.
150. Kam C, Greenberg MT, Kusché CA. Sustained effects of the PATHS curriculum on the social and psychological adjustment of children in special education. J Emot Behav Disord. 2004;12(2):66–78.
151. Riggs NR, Greenberg MT, Kusche CA, Pentz MA. The mediational role of neurocognition in the behavioral outcomes of a social-emotional prevention program in elementary school students: effects of the PATHS curriculum. Prev Sci. 2006;7(1):91–102. [PubMed]
152. Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. Initial impact of the Fast Track prevention trial for conduct problems, part II: classroom effects. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1999;67(5):648–657. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
153. Tolan PH, Dodge KA. Children’s mental health as a primary care and concern: a system for comprehensive support and service. Am Psychol. 2005;60(6):601–614. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
154. Grossman DC, Neckerman HJ, Koepsell TD, et al. Effectiveness of a violence prevention curriculum among children in elementary school. JAMA. 1997;277(20):1605–1611. [PubMed]
155. Reid MJ, Webster-Stratton C, Beauchaine TP. Parent training in Head Start: a comparison of program response among African American, Asian American, Caucasian, and Hispanic mothers. Prev Sci. 2002;2(4):209–227. [PubMed]
156. Webster-Stratton C, Taylor T. Nipping early risk factors in the bud: preventing substance abuse, delinquency, and violence in adolescence: interventions targeted at young children (ages 0–8 years) Prev Sci. 2001;2(3):165–192. [PubMed]
157. Gross D, Fogg L, Webster-Stratton C, Garvey C, Julion W, Grady J. Parent training with multiethnic families of toddlers in day care in low-income urban communities. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2003;71(2):261–278. [PubMed]
158. Heinrichs N, Hahlweg K, Bertram H, Kuschel A, Naumann S, Harstick S. The long-term efficacy of a parent training for the universal prevention of children’s behavioral disorders: results from the mothers and fathers [in German] Z Klin Psychol Psychother. 2006;35:82–96.
159. Hecht ML, Marsiglia FF, Elek E, et al. Culturally grounded substance use prevention: an evaluation of the Keepin’ it R.E.A.L. curriculum. Prev Sci. 2003;4(4):233–248. [PubMed]
160. Pentz MA, Valente T. Project STAR: a substance abuse prevention campaign in Kansas City. In: Backer TE, Rogers EM, editors. Successful Health Communications Campaigns: Organizational Dimensions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1995. pp. 37–60.
161. Chou C, Montgomery S, Pentz MA, et al. Effects of a community-based prevention program in decreasing drug use in high-risk adolescents. Am J Public Health. 1998;88(6):944–948. [PubMed]
162. Perry CL, Williams CL, Veblen-Mortenson S, et al. Project Northland: outcomes of a communitywide alcohol use prevention program during early adolescence. Am J Public Health. 1996;86(7):956–965. [PubMed]
163. Klepp K, Kelder SH, Perry CL. Alcohol and marijuana use among adolescents: long-term outcomes of the Class of 1989 Study. Soc Behav Med. 1995;17(1):19–24. [PubMed]
164. Perry CL, Williams CL, Komro KA, et al. Project Northland: long-term outcomes of community action to reduce adolescent alcohol use. Health Educ Res. 2002;17(1):117–132. [PubMed]
165. Spoth R, Redmond C, Shin C. Randomized trial of brief family interventions for general populations: adolescent substance use outcomes four years following baseline. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2001;69(4):627–642. [PubMed]
166. Spoth R, Redmond C, Shin C, Azevedo K. Brief family intervention effects on adolescent substance initiation: school-level curvilinear growth curve analyses six years following baseline. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2004;72(3):535–542. [PubMed]
167. Spoth R, Randall GK, Shin C, Redmond C. Randomized study of combined universal family and school preventive interventions: patterns of long-term effects on initiation, regular use, and weekly drunkenness. Psychol Addict Behav. 2005;19(4):372–381. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
168. Schinke SP, Tepavac L, Cole KC. Preventing substance use among Native American youth: three-year results. Addict Behav. 2000;25(3):387–397. [PubMed]
169. Bauman KE, Ennett ST, Foshee VA, Pemberton M, King TS, Koch GG. Influence of a family-directed program on adolescent cigarette and alcohol cessation. Prev Sci. 2000;1(4):227–237. [PubMed]
170. Bauman KE, Ennett ST, Foshee VA, Pemberton M, King TS, Koch GG. Influence of a family program on adolescent smoking and drinking prevalence. Prev Sci. 2002;3(1):35–42. [PubMed]
171. Bauman KE, Foshee VA, Ennett ST, et al. The influence of a family program on adolescent tobacco and alcohol use. Am J Public Health. 2001;91(4):604–610. [PubMed]
172. Ennett ST, Bauman KE, Pemberton M, et al. Mediation in a family-directed program for prevention of adolescent tobacco and alcohol use. Prev Med. 2001;33(4):333–346. [PubMed]
173. Park J, Kosterman R, Hawkins JD, et al. Effects of the “Preparing for the Drug Free Years” curriculum on growth in alcohol use and risk for alcohol use in early adolescence. Prev Sci. 2000;1(3):125–138. [PubMed]
174. Cuijpers P, Jonkers R, de Weerdt I, de Jong A. The effects of drug abuse prevention at school: the “Healthy School and Drugs” project. Addiction. 2002;97(1):67–73. [PubMed]
175. Botvin GJ, Baker E, Dusenbury L, Botvin EM, Diaz T. Long-term follow-up results of a randomized drug abuse prevention trial in a white middle-class population. JAMA. 1995;273(14):1106–1112. [PubMed]
176. Griffin KW, Botvin GJ, Nichols TR, Doyle MM. Effectiveness of a universal drug abuse prevention approach for youth at high risk for substance use initiation. Prev Med. 2003;36(1):1–7. [PubMed]
177. Wolchik SA, Sandler IN, Millsap RE, et al. Six-year follow-up of preventive interventions for children of divorce. JAMA. 2002;288(15):1874–1881. [PubMed]
178. Ellickson PL, McCaffrey DF, Ghosh-Dastidar B, Longshore DL. New inroads in preventing adolescent drug use: results from a large-scale trial of Project ALERT in middle schools. Am J Public Health. 2003;93(11):1830–1836. [PubMed]
179. Ellickson PL, Bell PL. Drug prevention in junior high: a multi-site longitudinal test. Science. 1990;247(4948):1299–1305. [PubMed]
180. McBride N, Farringdon F, Midford R, Meuleners L, Phillips M. Harm minimization in school drug education: final results of the School Health and Alcohol Harm Reduction Project (SHAHRP) Addiction. 2004;99(3):278–291. [PubMed]
181. McBride N, Midford R, Farringdon F, Phillips M. Early results from a school alcohol harm minimization study: the School Health and Alcohol Harm Reduction Project. Addiction. 2000;95(7):1021–1042. [PubMed]
182. Schinke SP, Schwinn TM, Di Noia J, Cole KC. Reducing the risks of alcohol abuse among urban youth: 3-year effects of computer-based and parent involvement interventions. J Stud Alcohol. 2004;65(4):443–449. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
183. Snow DL, Swan SC, Wilton L. A workplace coping-skills intervention to prevent alcohol abuse. In: Bennett JB, Lehman WEK, editors. Workplace Substance Abuse Prevention: Beyond Drug Testing to Wellness. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2002. pp. 57–96.
184. Wells-Parker E, Williams M. Enhancing the effectiveness of traditional interventions with drinking drivers by adding brief individual intervention components. J Stud Alcohol. 2002;63(6):655–664. [PubMed]
185. Longabaugh R, Woodlard RF, Nirenberg TD, et al. Evaluating the effects of a brief motivational intervention for injured drinkers in the emergency department. J Stud Alcohol. 2001;62(6):806–816. [PubMed]
186. Anderson BK, Larimer ME. Problem drinking and the workplace: an individualized approach to prevention. Psychol Addict Behav. 2002;16(3):243–251. [PubMed]
187. Figlio DN. The effect of drinking age laws and alcohol-related crashes: time-series evidence from Wisconsin. J Policy Anal Manage. 1995;14(4):555–566.
188. Asch P, Levy DT. Young driver fatalities: the roles of drinking age and drinking experience. South Econ J. 1990;57(2):512–520.
189. Lillis RP, Williams TP, Williford WR. The impact of the 19-year-old drinking age in New York. In: Holder HD, editor. Advances in Substance Abuse: Behavioral and Biological Research, Supplement 1: Control Issues in Alcohol Abuse Prevention: Strategies for States and Communities. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press; 1987. pp. 133–146.
190. Wilkinson JT. Reducing drunken driving: which policies are most effective? South Econ J. 1987;54(2):322–334.
191. Wagenaar AC. Preventing highway crashes by raising the legal minimum age for drinking: the Michigan experience 6 years later. J Safety Res. 1986;17(3):101–109.
192. Williams TP, Lillis RP. Changes in alcohol consumption by 18-year-olds following an increase in New York State’s purchase age to 19. J Stud Alcohol. 1986;47(4):290–296. [PubMed]
193. MacKinnon DP, Woodward JA. The impact of raising the minimum drinking age on driver fatalities. Int J Addict. 1986;21(12):1331–1338. [PubMed]