PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of scanLink to Publisher's site
 
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2010 Jun-Sep; 5(2-3): 148–158.
Published online Jan 18, 2010. doi:  10.1093/scan/nsp058
PMCID: PMC2894662

Culture and neuroscience: additive or synergistic?

Abstract

The investigation of cultural phenomena using neuroscientific methods—cultural neuroscience (CN)—is receiving increasing attention. Yet it is unclear whether the integration of cultural study and neuroscience is merely additive, providing additional evidence of neural plasticity in the human brain, or truly synergistic, yielding discoveries that neither discipline could have achieved alone. We discuss how the parent fields to CN: cross-cultural psychology, psychological anthropology and cognitive neuroscience inform the investigation of the role of cultural experience in shaping the brain. Drawing on well-established methodologies from cross-cultural psychology and cognitive neuroscience, we outline a set of guidelines for CN, evaluate 17 CN studies in terms of these guidelines, and provide a summary table of our results. We conclude that the combination of culture and neuroscience is both additive and synergistic; while some CN methodologies and findings will represent the direct union of information from parent fields, CN studies employing the methodological rigor required by this logistically challenging new field have the potential to transform existing methodologies and produce unique findings.

Keywords: cross-cultural, cross disciplinary, cultural-neuroscience, culture, neuroscience, neuroimaging

INTRODUCTION

Cultural neuroscience (CN) is an emerging field focused on the bidirectional interaction between cultural experience and the brain (Chiao and Harada, 2008). Cross-cultural psychology has identified cultural variation in many domains of behavior (Jahoda and Krewer, 1997); at the same time, the neurosciences have identified mechanisms of neural plasticity from the molecular to the systems level (Berlucchi and Buchtel, 2009). Thus, investigating the neural underpinnings of cross-cultural behavioral variation seems like a logical next step. Yet, because this cross-disciplinary endeavor represents the integration of a social science, cross-cultural psychology, and a biological science, neuroscience, the concept of CN has been met with skepticism (Chiao and Ambady, 2007). How can something as complex and abstract as ‘culture’ be studied with methodologies that are concrete and quantitative? Furthermore, will CN be greater than the sum of these parent disciplines by yielding truly novel discoveries? Or will neuroscience merely identify brain regions associated with known cross-cultural differences while cross-cultural psychology provides comparable examples of behavioral variation to those already studied in neural plasticity research?

We argue that the combination of cross-cultural psychology and neuroscience is both additive—some CN methodologies and findings will represent the direct union of information from parent fields—and synergistic, having the potential to transform existing methodologies and yield truly novel findings. We use this additive/synergistic framework throughout our review to differentiate potential contributions of CN that represent extensions of work in parent fields from those that are truly novel.

Our review is organized into four sections. In the first section, we discuss the conceptual and methodological contributions of cross-cultural psychology and neuroscience to CN by describing relevant studies of cross-cultural behavioral differences and experience-dependent neural plasticity. We also outline methodologies from cross-cultural psychology and neuroscience that address challenges common to these fields and CN. Based on these translational methodologies, in the second section, we propose a set of methodological guidelines for CN studies. We intend these guidelines to aid CN researchers in fully capitalizing on the well-developed methodologies of these parent fields. In the third section, we evaluate current CN studies in terms of our guidelines in order to assess the current state of the field and suggest avenues for future growth. Finally, in the fourth section we summarize the contributions of parent fields to CN and discuss the additive and synergistic findings CN may yield.

HOW CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY AND NEURAL PLASTICITY RESEARCH CAN INFORM CULTURAL NEUROSCIENCE

Cross-cultural behavioral variation

Cross-cultural variation in human behavior has been described in all areas of human cognition (Jahoda and Krewer, 1997). In this section we describe cross-cultural psychology and psychological anthropology studies from domains of cognition currently being investigated in CN: language, perception and social cognition. The cross-cultural differences in behavior identified by such studies provide useful starting points for CN studies of the neural basis of cultural experience-dependant plasticity. Also, the theoretical and ethnographic background presented in these studies can generate testable hypotheses for CN to explore.

Language is one of the most prominent domains of cross-cultural research. Although there is not always a one-to-one relationship between language and culture, an individual’s language(s) and cultural experience are inextricably linked (Nida, 2003). Language-specific syntax (Sera et al., 2002; Bastiaanse and Edwards, 2004), phonology and orthography (Cheung et al., 2001; McBride-Chang and Kail, 2002; McBride-Chang et al., 2005) influence other aspects of language, such as language learning. For example, Cheung et al. (2001) found that both phonological complexity and orthography impact phonological awareness by comparing pre-reading and literate children who speak alphabetic and non-alphabetic languages of different levels of phonological complexity. CN studies have begun to identify the neural correlates of such cross-linguistic differences (Kochunov et al., 2003; Valaki et al., 2004; Bolger et al., 2005). The extensive cross-linguistic literature will provide detailed background and testable hypotheses for future CN studies.

The relationship of language-specific structure and semantics to cognition is also studied in domains such as color and spatial perception (Byrnes and Gelman, 1991); however, whether language structures thought (cf. Whorf, 1956), or language and thought are largely independent (cf. Jackendoff, 1983) remains under debate. For example, despite wide variation in the number of color categories in different languages, color categorization has a predictable cross-linguistic structure related to the physiology of the human visual system (Abramov and Gordon, 1994; Kay and McDaniel, 1978), suggesting that color perception and categorization is physiologically rather than linguistically determined. In contrast, several studies have suggested that linguistic color categories influence color recognition memory and discrimination (e.g. Lantz and Stefflre, 1964; Roberson et al., 2005), suggesting that language may influence higher-order aspects of color perception. CN research may shed light on the language and thought debate by providing information about the maliability of neural regions underlying different cognitive functions.

Perceptual domains unrelated to language, such as context sensitivity and visual illusion susceptibility, have also been found to vary cross-culturally (Segall et al., 1963; Nisbett and Miyamoto, 2005). For example, Segall et al., (1963) found substantial differences in the geometric optical illusion susceptibility of individuals from 17 different cultural groups. Three European groups were more susceptible to two of four illusions administered, while 14 non-European groups were more susceptible to the other two illusions. The authors deduced that cross-cultural variation in susceptibility to the illusions studied was likely related to perceptual habits acquired in different ecological and cultural environments. For example, rectangularity was widespread in the urban environments common among the European groups and much less widespread in the plains and equatorial forest dwellers in the non-European samples. Thus, the influence of cultural experience on behavior can be seen in cognitive domains as basic as visual perception suggesting that perceptual neural systems may also be fruitful areas of exploration for CN research.

One of the most productive areas of cross-cultural comparison is social cognition. Two theoretical frameworks that have dominated this field are are the classification of cultures as either individualistic or collectivistic (Triandis, 1995; Kagitçibasi, 1996) and the classification of individuals as having either an independent or interdependent self-construal (Markus and Kitayama, 1991; Killen and Wainryb, 2000). Here I will focus on the more prevalent framework, individualism/collectivism (IC), however these two frameworks are closely related and studies utilizing both frameworks have yielded similar results. Individualism is characterized by the assumption that individuals are separate in identity and responsibility from one another, and is typically associated with Western cultures. Collectivism is characterized by the idea that individuals are defined by and obligated to their social groups and is typically associated with East Asian cultures. The individualism/collectivism (IC) framework has been used to explain cross-cultural differences in visual perception, causal attribution, motivation and emotion (Kagitçibasi, 1996). Research on IC has been criticized because these concepts are applied broadly and are often not treated as independent constructs, but rather as two ends the same continuum (Schwartz, 1990; Fiske, 2002). In line with this assessment, Oyserman et al. (2002) performed a meta-analysis of IC studies and found that individualism and collectivism are indeed independent constructs, are not robust to measurement technique variation, and are not closely aligned with the East/West dichotomy, i.e. Westerners are not reliably more individualistic and less collectivistic than Easterners. Despite these criticisms, as of 1994, one-third of cross-cultural studies employed IC in their explanation of cross-cultural differences (Hui and Yee, 1994), and many CN studies described later in this manuscript also rely heavily on IC. Thus it seems the IC framework will continue to be prominent in cross-cultural studies; however, future CN investigations will be able to more effectively investigate the neural correlates of IC by paying careful attention to critical analyses of the IC framework in cultural anthropology and psychology.

Cross-cultural methods

Van de Vijver and Leung (1997) argue that ‘culture is too global a concept to be used as a meaningful independent variable in the interpretation [of observed cultural differences]’ (pp. 260). The challenge of establishing specific causation in cross-cultural comparisons is that cultures do not represent true experimental treatments—individuals cannot be randomly assigned to cultural groups, and culture entails a whole suite of correlated behavioral traits (Van de Vijver and Leung, 1997). Even cross-cultural elements that appear similar may result from different historical, environmental and psychological influences (Boas, 1896). The causes of between-group cultural differences are therefore difficult to identify. In cross-cultural psychology, a general strategy for establishing causation is to deconstruct culture into psychologically relevant components and use these components to design more tractable and controlled experiments (Van de Vijver and Leung, 1997). Cross-cultural psychology provides several methods to facilitate this deconstruction of culture that are directly applicable to CN research.

Selection of cultures for comparison is the most critical step for effectively establishing causation in cross-cultural studies. Although the comparison of a single Western with a single non-Western culture is common in cross-cultural psychology and CN studies, comparing at least three cultures, a pairing of which share the cultural variable of interest, and another pairing of which share an alternative explanatory variable (i.e. ‘triangulation’), is needed to identify the cause of behavioral variation (Jahoda and Krewer, 1997; Medin and Atran, 2004). Without studying replicate cultures that share a feature of interest it is impossible to isolate which of the many differences between two cultures may be the cause of any observed between-group difference. Further cultural replication (beyond triangulation) and theory-driven rather than convenience-driven selection of cultures may also further limit the number of alternative explanations of cultural differences (Van de Vijver and Leung, 1997). For example, Kuhnen et al. (2001) found differences between two collectivistic (Malaysia and Russia) and two individualistic (Germany and US) cultures on their level of visual context sensitivity. By using two examples of each type of culture, they reduced the chance of detecting spurious differences between types, driven by unmeasured confounding variables. For example, an alternative explanation for visual context sensitivity is that it is related to use of a non-Roman alphabet. In this case, because Malaysians use the Roman alphabet while Russians use the Cyrillic alphabet, this alternative explanation was ruled out.

Another methodological strategy used in cross-cultural studies is to reduce inter-group variation by ensuring that subjects from different cultures are closely matched on demographic variables such as socioeconomic status (SES) and education level, as well as any elements of culture not directly under study (Van de Vijver and Leung, 1997; Schaffer and Riordan, 2003). For instance, Ji et al. (2000) reduce unwanted intergroup variation by comparing East Asian undergraduates attending college in the US to European American undergraduates in the same department. When it is impossible to match subjects on a demographic variable, the variable should be measured and used as a covariate in analyses (Van de Vijver and Leung, 1997).

Finally, valid cross-cultural studies are designed so that research stimuli are of equal familiarity and meaning to all subjects (Van de Vijver and Leung, 1997). A study by Yoon et al. (2004) illustrated the danger of not verifying cross-cultural equivalence of stimuli: only 22% of the 260 Snodgrass and Vanderwart (1980) line drawings of everyday objects, typically used for picture naming studies, showed both name and concept agreement between American and Chinese subjects. Language stimuli must also be translated effectively, and the characteristics of the test administrator and response procedures (e.g. computer use) must be of equal familiarity to all subjects (Van de Vijver and Leung, 1997).

Neural plasticity: mechanisms and methods

Cellular and systems level mechanisms of experience-dependent plasticity have been described in the neurosciences through the use of ex vivo molecular studies and in vivo studies in animals and humans. Because it is difficult to investigate cellular mechanisms of neural plasticity in humans, tissue culture and animal studies provide CN insight into the mechanisms and limits of neural plasticity that may underlie cultural differences, such as those described in the preceding section. Human neuroimaging studies of neural plasticity, on the other hand, provide an actual model for CN studies of cultural experience-dependent plasticity.

Two key cellular mechanisms of neural plasticity are long-term potentiation (LTP), which strengthens neural connections, and long-term depression (LTD), which weakens neural connections (Lynch, 2004). The long-lasting effects of LTP/D are thought to involve the morphological alteration of synapses such as the modification and generation of dendritic spines (Malenka and Bear, 2004). LTP and LTD likely occur in some form at every excitatory synapse in the mammalian brain (Malenka and Bear, 2004) and thus likely underlie neural plasticity induced by cultural experience.

The remarkable plasticity of the mammalian brain has been demonstrated by studies of animal sensory development. These studies have revealed that although genetic programs guide the development of neurobiological systems, normative sensory experience is critical in the development of functional neural circuitry (Knudsen, 2004). For instance, depriving young organisms of input from various sensory modalities including vision (Morishita and Hensch, 2008) and audition (Dahmen and King, 2007), results in profound functional disruptions of these systems. Following such major perturbations, large-scale reorganizations of sensory systems can occur, even cross-modally (Bavelier and Neville, 2002). For instance, Collignon et al. (2009) found that congenitally blind individuals recruit areas of visual cortex during auditory processing. Similarly, Ptito et al. (2008) found that blind Braille readers felt sensations in their fingers in response to transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) of occipital cortex. Such studies inform CN as to the range of possible plastic changes in the brain associated with different types of experience.

Learning experiences influence the brain throughout life, as exemplified by neural plasticity studies on human expertise. Although tissue and animal studies illustrate the range of the brain’s plastic potential, studies of human expertise may be the most appropriate models for the study of cultural experience-dependent plasticity. Furthermore, studies of human expertise are typically conducted using neuroimaging techniques. Therefore, the methodological approaches in the following studies are most applicable to CN.

A prototypical example of human expertise is musical training (Münte et al., 2002). Musical training influences auditory perception, motor performance, visuospatial processing, and interhemispheric processing (Stewart, 2008). These behavioral alterations are accompanied by structural alterations such as increased gray matter (GM) and white matter (WM) in primary auditory cortex, altered motor and somatosensory maps, and an enlargement of the anterior corpus callosum (Stewart, 2008). Structural brain changes are quantified using MRI and a number of structural analysis techniques including voxel-based morphometry (Ashburner and Friston, 2000) and cortical thickness measurement (Sowell et al., 2001). Functional and structural changes related to musical training are often instrument- and effector-specific and are associated with the age of initiation of musical training even when years of musical experience are controlled for (for a review see Stewart, 2008).

Occupational specializations have also been associated with functional and structural neural plasticity including driving a taxi (increased GM in posterior hippocampus; Maguire et al., 2000) and being a mathematician (increased GM in parietal cortex; Aydin et al., 2007). Even short-term practice, such as learning to juggle, can result in structural changes (increased in GM volume in visual area MT and left intraperietal sulcus) evident after as little as 7 days of juggling training (Driemeyer et al., 2008). Similar plastic changes were also seen in the brains of elderly individuals learning to juggle; however, changes in additional brain areas were seen as well suggesting that the nature of neural plasticity may change with age (Boyke et al., 2008).

An important consideration in experience-dependant plasticity research is the potential contribution of genes to behavioral and neural variation. The young field of imaging genetics has begun to identify associations between common genetic variants, human cognitive functions and their neural correlates (Goldberg and Weinberger, 2004). Identified genetic polymorphisms associated with cognitive and neural effects are primarily those related to monoamine neurotransmitter receptors and transporters. For example genetic variation in the Catechol-O-methyltransferase gene (COMT) has been linked to variation in working memory and prefrontal cortex function (e.g. Caldú et al., 2007). Similarly, genetic variation in the seratonin transporter gene has been linked to variation in anxiety related behavior and cingulate cortex-amygdala interactions (e.g. Pezawas et al., 2005). Such genetic polymorphisms have been found in higher frequencies in certain regional groups (e.g. East Asians) than others (Benjamin et al., 1996); therefore, it will be important for CN studies to consider the potential effects of such regional genetic variation on cross-cultural behavioral and neural differences.

METHODOLOGICAL GUIDELINES FOR CN

Methodologies from both psychology and neuroscience are directly applicable to CN studies. We have developed a set of eight guidelines for CN studies based on well-established methods from these parent fields. These guidelines concentrate on aspects of study design most different between other cognitive neuroscience imaging studies and CN: group selection, subject selection and stimulus selection. These design elements are especially important to the establishment of causation in CN because of the quasi-experimental nature of cross-cultural comparisons. The purpose of these guidelines is to allow CN researchers to fully capitalize on methodological approaches, both from cross-cultural psychology and cognitive neuroscience, which address challenges common to these fields and CN.

The first three guidelines relate to the selection of cultures for comparison:

  1. Define and measure culture. Culture should be clearly defined in the same way for all groups compared, not conflated with ethnicity or nationality, and measured using some form of acculturation scale (Van de Vijver and Leung, 1997). The measurement of cultural endorsement is especially important when studying individuals whose cultural heritage is different than their culture of residence (i.e. first or second-generation immigrants) as there is wide variation in which culture these individuals may endorse (e.g. Phinney and Devitch-Navarro, 1997).
  2. Unpackage’ culture. In order to establish causation, culture should be deconstructed into psychologically relevant components. These components (e.g. individualism/collectivism) should be used to select cultural groups for comparison, and should be assessed in every study participant.
  3. Replicate cultures containing cultural element of interest. In order to establish causality, at least three cultural groups must be compared, and further replication of cultures (i.e. more than three) is desireable (Jahoda and Krewer, 1997; Medin and Atran, 2004). Cultural groups should be selected that replicate the predicted causal cultural element. When such replication is not possible this should be considered during interpretation of results.

The next three guidelines refer to subject-specific elements of cultural groups selected.

  1. Match or measure onset/amount of cultural experience. Studies of human neural plasticity have demonstrated that both age at onset of experience and duration of experience are related to plasticity (Stewart, 2008). More specifically, Goh et al., 2007 have demonstrated an interaction between aging and the influence of cultural experience on the brain and Park and Gutchess, 2006 have suggested that this type of interaction may be widespread. Therefore both age at onset of cultural experience and amount of experience should be measured and/or matched in CN studies. As in studies of animal and human sensory experience and human expertise, there may be a sensitive period for the impact of cultural experience on the brain.
  2. Consider the effects of regional genetic variation. Regional genetic variation has been shown to have measurable effects on the brain and behavior and genetic heritage and culture are often counfounded (Benjamin et al., 1996; Goldberg and Weinberger, 2004); therefore, in order to make inferences about the relative contributions of genetics and cultural experience to cross-cultural neural differences, comparison groups with shared genetic heritage but differing cultural experience should be included. For instance, a study investigating the neural basis of individualism/collectivism differences in Chinese and European American subjects could also include a third generation or later Chinese American group. If cross-national differences in IC are driven by culture, the European American and Chinese American group should be more similar to each other on the dependant neural measure than to the Chinese group, whereas if cross-national differences are driven by regional genetic variation, the Chinese and Chinese American group should be more similar to each other than to the European American group. Alternatively, participants could be genotyped at loci known to be related to the cognitive function under study.
  3. Match groups. Factors other than the cultural element of interest should be matched between groups or measured and included as covariates in the analysis when matching is not possible (Schaffer and Riordan, 2003; Van de Vijver and Leung, 1997).

The final two guidelines concern characteristics of the experimental stimuli used.

  1. Equate stimuli. The equivalence of the experimental stimuli for all cultural groups under study should be verified (Van de Vijver and Leung, 1997). Equating stimuli may involve group-specific adjustments to stimuli.
  2. Equate performance. In order to disassociate neural and behavioral differences, task performance between groups should be equated as closely as possible (Price and Friston, 1999). Equating performance may also require group-specific adjustments to stimuli.

LITERATURE REVIEW RELATIVE TO GUIDELINES

Literature review: selection of studies

We have selected seventeen studies from the literature for inclusion in our review. These studies are summarized in Table 1. Our goal was to provide a picture of the nascent field of CN and to compare the extant CN literature against our methodological guidelines derived from cross-cultural psychology and cognitive neuroscience. In order to limit the scope of our review and ensure studies are maximally comparable, we have only included neuroimaging studies that compare at least two cultural (or national/linguistic) groups exposed to the same or equivalent stimuli. Based on these criteria, we have excluded some studies important to the field of CN: cultural priming studies in bicultural individuals (Sui and Han, 2007; Lin et al., 2008) and studies that compare two cultural groups exposed to culturally familiar and unfamiliar stimuli, such as studies of cross-cultural music perception (Morrison et al., 2003; Nan et al., 2008). While the contributions of such studies are valuable to CN, methodological differences between these studies and those included in this review, especially the lack of two comparison groups in the case of cultural priming studies, would have made comparison against our guidelines less meaningful. However, it is important to note that cultural priming studies avoid some of the methodological challenges related to group matching that our guidelines address, and in this way make a unique and important contribution to CN research.

Table 1
Cultural neuroscience studies compared to our guidelines derived from cross-cultural psychology and cognitive neuroscience

For our literature search we relied primarily on PubMed and Google Scholar using the search terms ‘cultural, cross-cultural, brain, neuroscience, neuroimaging, cognition and perception’. We also identified studies from the citations of other collected studies and the ‘related articles’ feature on PubMed. Selected studies cluster in three broad domains of cognition: language, perception, social cognition. We have evaluated each of the included studies relative to our guidelines and indicated whether each guideline was met, partially met or not met in Table 1. In the following section we discuss each guideline in terms of the current CN literature and provide an example of a study that met that guideline particularly well. Criteria for partially meeting each guideline are also outlined below.

Guidelines with case studies

  1. Define and measure culture. A study could partially meet this guideline by clearly defining culture the same way for all groups; however, to fully address guideline 1, the definition of culture could not simply be a nation or language, and culture, as defined, must have been measured in all participants. The only study to fully meet this guideline, Han et al. (2008), compared Christian to non-religious Chinese adults. The religious attitudes and religious knowledge of both groups were measured and the Christian group was found to be higher than the non-religious group on both measures. Though Han et al. (2008) employ a narrow definition of culture (religion), a similar strategy for definition and measurement can be used with broader, more typical designations of culture. Many of the studies we considered defined culture as either a nation or a language despite the fact that there often is not a one-to-one relationship between language and/or nationality and culture. Culture was also commonly defined as a multi-national group such as East Asian or Western, implying that these broad groups are culturally homogeneous, which is only true at the most superficial levels of analysis. Finally, a number of studies defined culture in different ways for different groups, e.g. Japanese compared to Caucasian. All of these group selection strategies can result in comparing non-equivalent groups, making interpretation difficult or meaningless. Thus, properly defining culture will be critical for future CN studies.
  2. Unpackage’ culture. Any study that mentioned specific cultural elements rather than ‘culture’ alone, while describing the research question or explaining the findings was considered to partially satisfy Guideline 2. Only studies that measured the cultural variable of interest in both groups and used these measurements in the ensuing analysis fully addressed this guideline. Chiao et al. (2008) fully met this guideline by measuring IC in their subjects and regressing this variable against fMRI data on a self/other judgment task. Although Chiao et al. (2008) also found correlations with IC in their data, culture (whether participants where Japanese or American) was not correlated with IC, suggesting the need for caution when selecting possible explanatory cultural variables. Only one other study, (Lewis et al., 2008), fully met this guideline by measuring the cultural variable of interest (IC) and using it in their analysis. Future CN studies will contribute more to our understanding of the relationship between culture and the brain by not only unpackaging culture but measuring how specific cultural elements relate to their dependant neural measure.
  3. Replicate cultures containing cultural element of interest. Studies could partially satisfy this guideline by including more than one group with a given cultural element of interest. To fully meet Guideline 3 a study must have included at least two cultures representing each cultural element of interest in sufficient numbers that these groups could be analyzed separately. A meta-analysis of word reading studies in Western alphabetic languages compared to word reading studies in Eastern logographic languages was the only study to fully meet this guideline (Bolger et al., 2005). By comparing multiple cultures that shared orthography but differed in other ways, the authors were able to specifically identify neural regions that related to orthography rather than other elements of language or culture. Although replication in CN studies is logistically challenging and costly, replication is crucial for identifying an explanatory variable more specific than ‘culture’. Such replication may be accomplished though meta-analyses such as the one above or multi-site collaborations such as the study by Henrich et al. (2005) exploring economic decision making behavior in 15 small-scale societies.
  4. Match or measure onset/amount of cultural experience. Studies that mentioned and roughly matched the amount of cultural experience typical of each study group partially met this guideline. However, only studies that tested subjects within their native cultures fully met Guideline 4. Chiao et al. (2008) met this guideline in a novel way by conducting fMRI scans of subjects residing in their native cultures (Japanese or American) and then comparing these scans (from two different scanners) directly. Because cross-scanner comparison is difficult due to scanner-specific artifacts in the data, the methods of cross-scanner comparison described in Chiao et al. (2008) will be useful for future CN studies. Unfortunately, none of the studies considered included individuals with different amounts and different ages at onset of cultural experience, (e.g. subjects who came into their culture of residence at different ages) and/or used onset or years of cultural experience in their statistical analyses. However, given that age of onset is a powerful mediator of neural plastic changes resulting from expertise (Stewart, 2008), this strategy may be useful in determining whether a sensitive period exists for the neural influences of cultural experience.
  5. Consider genetics. A study that included at least one group in which culture and genetic heritage were not completely confounded could partially satisfy guideline 5; however, in order to fully meet Guideline 5, and be able to make inferences about cultural versus genetic effects, a study had to include a separate group with cultural experience similar to one comparison group and genetic heritage similar to that of the other. Lewis, Goto and Kong (2008) was the only study to even partially satisfy this guideline. They compared mono-cultural European Americans to bi-cultural Asian Americans and measured each subject’s level of individualism and collectivism (treated independently). By including level of cultural endorsement in regression analyses with event-related potential (ERP) data, they were able to determine that the level of collectivism mediated the relationship between an ERP component and culture, suggesting that the collectivism component of culture rather than genetic factors—which likely did not vary with IC—was driving their ERP group difference. Although disentangling the influence of genetic and cultural factors on brain function requires the inclusion of additional control groups, attention to the potential influence of regional genetic variation in CN studies will greatly enhance the information CN may contribute to the nature/nurture debate.
  6. Match groups. Studies that considered basic elements of group matching such as age and handedness (important for neural lateralization) partially addressed this guideline. Only studies that measured additional neuropsychological variables relevant to the study topic, such as IQ or reading ability, and verified group matching by direct statistical comparison, fully addressed this guideline. Gutchess et al. (2006) provide a particularly good example of group matching; the authors not only matched groups on age, education level, and culturally-appropriate questions of world knowledge (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale information subtest), but also two visual speed of processing tests relevant to the visual scene processing task under investigation. In this way, the authors were able to rule out alternative explanations of group differences based on general demographics and cognitive skills tangentially related to the task. More than half of the studies considered compared groups matched only on basic demographic criteria such as handedness; however, given the nature of cross-cultural comparison, additional group matching measures, beyond those typically employed in cognitive neuroscience studies, are necessary for unambiguous interpretation of between-group differences.
  7. Equate stimuli. Tasks that partially met this guideline made some attempt to equate stimuli for both groups or used tasks simple enough to reasonably assume cultural equivalence. Only studies that verified stimuli equivalence for all groups fully satisfied this guideline. For instance, Hot et al. (2006) used emotional stimuli whose emotional valence ratings had been previously normed in both comparison groups (Japanese and French). Similarly, Paulesu et al. (2000) designed their word reading task using words that were among the 7500 most common in both language groups compared. Unfortunately, about half of the studies considered did not verify stimuli equivalence across comparison groups and/or acknowledged substantial differences in stimuli meaning across comparison groups.
  8. Equate performance. Studies partially met this guideline if they measured performance and it was at least partially equivalent across groups. Fully meeting this guideline required that all measured aspects of performance were equivalent across groups. Kobayashiet al. (2006) measured both accuracy and reaction time on their false belief task and found no group differences on either measure. In general, attendance to performance matching was mixed among the studies. Stimulus and performance matching will be especially important for future CN studies because these strategies reduce the number of possible explanations of between-group differences, which are especially high during cultural comparison.

Overall many studies in CN are already unpackaging culture (ii), matching groups (vi), using culturally equivalent stimuli (vii) and matching performance (viii). Future consideration of these guidelines will further aid in the interpretation of the inherently complex results of CN studies. Fewer studies are currently defining and measuring culture consistently (i), replicating cultures sharing a cultural element of interest (iii), matching the amount of cultural experience across groups (iv), and disentangling culture and genetics (v). It should involve relatively little effort for future CN studies to improve the consistency of their cultural definitions and reference to the anthropological and psychological literature should aid in cultural definition improvement. However, studying individuals immersed in their own cultures by performing multi-site imaging studies (iv) and increasing the number of study groups in order to rule out alternative genetic and cultural explanations of between-group differences (v) will require future studies to employ larger and more complicated designs. Additionally, only one study considered here performed brain structural analyses as opposed to functional analysis only (Kochunov et al., 2003). Given the structural brain changes seen in studies of human expertise, future CN research will benefit from including structural as well as functional analyses.

CULTURE AND NEUROSCIENCE: ADDITIVE AND SYNERGISTIC

Considering the contributions of parent fields to CN, current CN studies, and potential future contributions of CN research we conclude that the combination of culture and neuroscience is both additive and synergistic. Cross-cultural psychology and cognitive neuroscience contribute concepts to CN in an additive fashion. (i) The cultural variations in behavior identified by cross-cultural studies in psychology provide avenues for neuroscientific inquiry. (ii) Cross-cultural psychology and psychological anthropology theory provide useful background and testable hypotheses for CN to explore. (iii) The molecular and systems level mechanisms of neural plasticity identified in animal studies inform CN as these mechanisms also likely underlie neural plasticity that results from cultural experience. (iv) Finally, neuroimaging studies of the effects of expertise on the human brain provide a conceptual and methodological model for CN studies. Cross-cultural psychology and neural plasticity studies in cognitive neuroscience also additively contribute methodologies to CN, as demonstrated in our guidelines. Translational methodologies form parent fields will enhance the explanatory power of CN research. Some of the findings of CN research will also be additive, reflecting a direct union of information from parent fields. For example, neuroimaging will associate brain areas with known cross-cultural differences, and CN research will provide additional evidence of neural plasticity in the human brain.

Culture and neuroscience also have the potential to be truly synergistic; some of the findings of CN may be fundamentally different form those in cross-cultural psychology and neural plasticity research to date. Unique information about the mechanics of human neural plasticity may arise from studying the effects of differential cultural experience on the brain, as cultural experience has properties that differ from expertise and other types of experience previously studies in humans: it is widely shared, not self-selected, socially transmitted, constantly evolving and it influences many cognitive and sensory domains. Additionally, because culture is not self-selected, as expertise in a musical instrument or sport is, it may be easier to tease apart the effects of cultural experience from internal sources of neural variation.

The synergy of cross-cultural studies and neuroscience also holds the potential to reveal some truly novel information about cross-cultural differences in behavior. As in the example of cross-cultural color terminology (Abramov and Gordon, 1994), the physiology of the nervous system constrains variation in human behavior to some extent. Studying the neurobiological basis of cultural variation may help explain the patterns of cross-cultural behavioral variation described in psychological and anthropological studies. Alternatively, just as Boas (1896) noted that cross-culturally similar aspects of behavior can have different historical and environmental causes, they may also have different neural underpinnings. For instance, Grön et al. (2003) found that despite equivalent performance on a nonverbal episodic memory task, Caucasian subjects relied more on the ventral visual stream and Chinese subjects relied more on the dorsal visual stream for neural processing.

Despite the potential novel contributions of CN research, the combination of culture and neuroscience results in some unique logistical challenges. These challenges include cultural differences in the rate of age-related changes in the blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal, differences in brain size and shape, and, most notably, limited availability of non-invasive measures of brain function in different cultures (Park and Gutchess, 2002). Many of the neuroimaging modalities most appropriate for studying CN questions, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), positron emission tomography (PET), and magneto encephalography (MEG), are not portable and not widely available for research purposes in close proximity to all cultural groups of interest (Han and Northoff, 2008). Even when neuroimaging methodologies are available locally, the individual characteristics of each imaging device make cross-device comparisons difficult, though feasible with careful matching of hardware and software, (Sutton et al., 2008). Therefore, although exciting discoveries in CN have already been made (Han and Northoff, 2008), the full potential of CN research will be reached through a combination of international collaboration, translational methodologies from parent fields, and the further development of new methodologies that address the unique challenges of CN studies such as portable imaging technologies and cross-scanner data comparison.

FUNDING

National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship and the FPR-UCLA Center for Culture, Brain, and Development.

Footnotes

The authors thank Neil Losin for his helpful comments on the manuscript.

REFERENCES

  • Abramov I, Gordon J. Color appearance: on seeing red-or yellow, or green, or blue. Annual Review of Psychology. 1994;45:451–85. [PubMed]
  • Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Voxel-Based morphometry-the methods. Neuroimage. 2000;11:805–21. [PubMed]
  • Aydin K, Ucar A, Oguz KK, et al. Increased gray matter density in the parietal cortex of mathematicians: a voxel-based morphometry study. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 2007;28:1859–64. [PubMed]
  • Bastiaanse R, Edwards S. Word order and finiteness in Dutch and English Broca's; and Wernicke's; aphasia. Brain and Language. 2004;89:91–107. [PubMed]
  • Bavelier D, Neville HJ. Cross-modal plasticity: where and how? Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2002;3:443–52. [PubMed]
  • Benjamin J, Li L, Patterson C, et al. Population and familial association between the D4 dopamine receptor gene and measures of novelty seeking. Nature Genetics. 1996;12:81–4. [PubMed]
  • Berlucchi G, Buchtel HA. Neuronal plasticity: historical roots and evolution of meaning. Experimental Brain Research. 2009;192:307–19. [PubMed]
  • Boas F. The limitations of the comparative method in anthropology. Science. 1896;4:901–8. [PubMed]
  • Bolger DJ, Perfetti CA, Schneider W. Cross-cultural effect on the brain revisited: universal structures plus writing system variation. Human Brain Mapping. 2005;25:92–104. [PubMed]
  • Boyke J, Driemeyer J, Gaser C, Buchel C, May A. Training-induced brain structure changes in the elderly. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:7031. [PubMed]
  • Byrnes JP, Gelman SA. Perspectives on thought and language: traditional and contemporary views. In: Brynes JP, Gelman SA, editors. Perspectives on Language and Thought: Interrelations in Development. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press; 1991. pp. 3–30.
  • Caldú X, Vendrell P, Bartrés-Faz D, et al. Impact of the COMT val108/158 met and DAT genotypes on prefrontal function in healthy subjects. Neuroimage. 2007;37:1437–44. [PubMed]
  • Cheung H, Chen HC, Lai CY, Wong OC, Hills M. The development of phonological awareness: effects of spoken language experience and orthography. Cognition. 2001;81:227–41. [PubMed]
  • Chiao JY, Ambady N. Cultural neuroscience: parsing universality and diversity across levels of analysis. In: Kitayama S, Cohen D, editors. Handbook of cultural psychology. New York: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 237–254.
  • Chiao JY, Harada T, Komeda H. Neural basis of individualistic and collectivistic views of self. Human Brain Mapping. 2008;9:2813–20. [PubMed]
  • Chiao J, Harada T. Cultural neuroscience of consciousness: from visual perception to self-awareness. Journal of Consciousness Studies. 2008;15:58–69.
  • Collignon O, Voss P, Lassonde M, Lepore F. Cross-modal plasticity for the spatial processing of sounds in visually deprived subjects. Experimental Brain Research. 2009;192:343–58. [PubMed]
  • Dahmen JC, King AJ. Learning to hear: plasticity of auditory cortical processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2007;17:456–64. [PubMed]
  • Driemeyer J, Boyke J, Gaser C, Büchel C, May A. Changes in gray matter induced by learning revisited. Plos ONE. 2008;3:e2669. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002669. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Fiske AP. Using individualism and collectivism to compare cultures–a critique of the validity and measurement of the constructs: comment on Oyserman et al. (2002) Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128:78–88. [PubMed]
  • Goh JO, Chee MW, Tan JC, et al. Age and culture modulate object processing and object-scene binding in the ventral visual area. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience. 2007;7:44–52. [PubMed]
  • Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR. Genes and the parsing of cognitive processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004;8:325–35. [PubMed]
  • Grön G, Schul D, Bretschneider V, Wunderlich AP, Riepe MW. Alike performance during nonverbal episodic learning from diversely imprinted neural networks. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;18:3112–20. [PubMed]
  • Gutchess AH, Welsh RC, Boduroglu A, Park DC. Cultural differences in neural function associated with object processing. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience. 2006;6:102–9. [PubMed]
  • Han S, Northoff G. Culture-sensitive neural substrates of human cognition: a transcultural neuroimaging approach. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9:646–54. [PubMed]
  • Han S, Mao L, Gu X, Zhu Y, Ge J, Ma Y. Neural consequences of religious belief on self-referential processing. Social Neuroscience. 2008;3:1–15. [PubMed]
  • Hedden T, Ketay S, Aron A, Markus HR, Gabrieli JD. Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychological Science. 2008;19:12–7. [PubMed]
  • Henrich J, Boyd R, Bowles S. “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2005;28:795–815. [PubMed]
  • Hot P, Saito Y, Mandai O, Kobayashi T, Sequeira H. An ERP investigation of emotional processing in european and japanese individuals. Brain Research. 2006;1122:171–8. [PubMed]
  • Hui CH, Yee C. The shortened individualism-collectivism scale: its relationship to demographic and work-related variables. Journal of Research in Personality. 1994;28:409–409.
  • Jackendoff R. Semantics and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT press; 1983.
  • Jahoda G, Krewer B. History of cross-cultural and cultural psychology. In: Berry JW, Poortinga YP, Pandey J, editors. Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology, Volume 1: Theory and Method. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon; 1997. pp. 1–42.
  • Ji LJ, Peng K, Nisbett RE. Culture, control, and perception of relationships in the environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;78:943–55. [PubMed]
  • Kagitçibasi C. Individualism and collectivism. In: Berry JW, Segall MH, Kagitcibasi C, editors. Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology, Volume 3: Social Behavior and Applications. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon; 1996. pp. 1–50.
  • Kay P, McDaniel CK. The linguistic significance of the meanings of basic color terms. Language. 1978;54:610–46.
  • Killen M, Wainryb C. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2000. 2000. Independence and interdependence in diverse cultural contexts. pp. 5–21. [PubMed]
  • Knudsen EI. Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004;16:1412–25. [PubMed]
  • Kobayashi C, Glover GH, Temple E. Cultural and linguistic influence on neural bases of ‘theory of mind’: an fmri study with Japanese bilinguals. Brain and Language. 2006;98:210–20. [PubMed]
  • Kobayashi C, Glover GH, Temple E. Cultural and linguistic effects on neural bases of ‘theory of mind’ in American and Japanese children. Brain Research. 2007;1164:95–107. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Kochunov P, Fox P, Lancaster J, et al. Localized morphological brain differences between English-speaking Caucasians and Chinese-speaking Asians: new evidence of anatomical plasticity. Neuroreport. 2003;14:961–4. [PubMed]
  • Kuhnen U, Hannover B, Roeder U, et al. Cross-Cultural variations in identifying embedded figures: Comparisons from the United States, Germany, Russia, and Malaysia. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2001;32:366..
  • Lantz DL, Stefflre V. Language and cognition revisited. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 1964;69:472–81. [PubMed]
  • Lewis RS, Goto SG, Kong LL. Culture and context: East Asian American and European American differences in P3 event-related potentials and self-construal. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2008;34:623–34. [PubMed]
  • Lin Z, Lin Y, Han S. Self-construal priming modulates visual activity underlying global/local perception. Biological Psychology. 2008;77:93–7. [PubMed]
  • Lynch MA. Long-term potentiation and memory. Physiological Reviews. 2004;84:87–136. [PubMed]
  • Maguire EA, Gadian DG, Johnsrude IS, et al. Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2000;97:4398–403. [PubMed]
  • Malenka RC, Bear MF. LTP and LTD: an embarrassment of riches. Neuron. 2004;44:5–21. [PubMed]
  • Markus HR, Kitayama S. Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review. 1991;98:224–53.
  • McBride-Chang C, Kail RV. Cross-cultural similarities in the predictors of reading acquisition. Child Development. 2002;73:1392–407. [PubMed]
  • McBride-Chang C, Cho JR, Liu H, et al. Changing models across cultures: associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure awareness with vocabulary and word recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2005;92:140–60. [PubMed]
  • Medin DL, Atran S. The native mind: biological categorization and reasoning in development and across cultures. Psychological Review. 2004;111:960–83. [PubMed]
  • Moriguchi Y, Ohnishi T, Kawachi T, et al. Specific brain activation in Japanese and Caucasian people to fearful faces. Neuroreport. 2005;16:133–6. [PubMed]
  • Morishita H, Hensch TK. Critical period revisited: impact on vision. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2008;18:101–7. [PubMed]
  • Morrison SJ, Demorest SM, Aylward EH, Cramer SC, Maravilla KR. fMRI investigation of cross-cultural music comprehension. Neuroimage. 2003;20:378–84. [PubMed]
  • Münte TF, Altenmüller E, Jäncke L. The musician's; brain as a model of neuroplasticity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2002;3:473–8. [PubMed]
  • Nan Y, Knösche TR, Zysset S, Friederici AD. Cross-cultural music phrase processing: an fmri study. Human Brain Mapping. 2008;29:312–28. [PubMed]
  • Nida EA. Language and culture. In: Mejri S, editor. Traduire la langue, traduire la culture. Paris, France: Maisonneuve and Larose; 2003. p. 193.
  • Nisbett RE, Miyamoto Y. The influence of culture: holistic versus analytic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9:467–73. [PubMed]
  • Oyserman D, Coon HM, Kemmelmeier M. Rethinking individualism and collectivism: evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128:3–72. [PubMed]
  • Park DC, Gutchess AH. Aging, cognition, and culture: a neuroscientific perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2002;26:859–67. [PubMed]
  • Park D, Gutchess A. The cognitive neuroscience of aging and culture. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2006;15:105–108.
  • Paulesu E, McCrory E, Fazio F, et al. A cultural effect on brain function. Nature Neuroscience. 2000;3:91–6. [PubMed]
  • Pezawas L, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Drabant EM, et al. 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:828–34. [PubMed]
  • Phinney JS, Devitch-Navarro M. Variations in bicultural identification among African American and Mexican American adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 1997;7:3–32.
  • Price CJ, Friston KJ. Scanning patients with tasks they can perform. Human Brain Mapping. 1999;8:102–8. [PubMed]
  • Ptito M, Fumal A, de Noordhout AM, Schoenen J, Gjedde A, Kupers R. TMS of the occipital cortex induces tactile sensations in the fingers of blind braille readers. Experimental Brain Research. 2008;184:193–200. [PubMed]
  • Roberson D, Davidoff J, Davies IR, Shapiro LR. Color categories: evidence for the cultural relativity hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology. 2005;50:378–411. [PubMed]
  • Schaffer BS, Riordan CM. A review of cross-cultural methodologies for organizational research: a best-practices approach. Organizational Research Methods. 2003;6:169.
  • Schwartz SH. Individualism-collectivism: critique and proposed refinements. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 1990;21:139.
  • Segall MH, Campbell DT, Herskovitz MJ. Cultural differences in the perception of geometric illusions. Science. 1963;139:769–71. [PubMed]
  • Sera MD, Elieff C, Forbes J, Burch MC, Rodríguez W, Dubois DP. When language affects cognition and when it does not: an analysis of grammatical gender and classification. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 2002;131:377–97. [PubMed]
  • Snodgrass JG, Vanderwart M. A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1980;6:174–215. [PubMed]
  • Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Tessner KD, Toga AW. Mapping continued brain growth and gray matter density reduction in dorsal frontal cortex: inverse relationships during postadolescent brain maturation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:8819. [PubMed]
  • Stewart L. Do musicians have different brains? Clinical Medicine. 2008;8:304–8. [PubMed]
  • Sui J, Han S, Ji Y, Shen J, et al. Arithmetic processing in the brain shaped by cultures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2006;103:10775–80. [PubMed]
  • Sui J, Han S. Self-Construal priming modulates neural substrates of self-awareness. Psychological Science. 2007;18:861–66. [PubMed]
  • Sutton BP, Goh J, Hebrank A, Welsh RC, Chee MWL, Park DC. Investigation and validation of intersite fMRI studies using the same imaging hardware. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging. 2008;28 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Triandis HC. Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, Colorado: Westview Press; 1995.
  • Valaki CE, Maestu F, Simos PG, et al. Cortical organization for receptive language functions in Chinese, English, and Spanish: a cross-linguistic MEG study. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:967–79. [PubMed]
  • Van de Vijver FJR, Leung K. Methods and data analysis of comparative research. In: Berry JW, Poortinga YP, Pandey J, editors. Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology, Volume 1: Theory and Method. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon; 1997. pp. 257–300.
  • Whorf B. Langauge, Thought, and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1956.
  • Yoon C, Feinberg F, Luo T, et al. A cross-culturally standardized set of pictures for younger and older adults: American and Chinese norms for name agreement, concept agreement, and familiarity. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers. 2004;36:639–49. [PubMed]
  • Zhu Y, Zhang L, Fan J, Han S. Neural basis of cultural influence on self-representation. Neuroimage. 2007;34:1310–6. [PubMed]

Articles from Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press