PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of ijerphMDPI Open Access JournalsMDPI Open Access JournalsThis articleThis JournalInstructions for authorsAdd your e-mail address to receive forthcoming issues of this journal
 
Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2010 April; 7(4): 1508–1519.
Published online 2010 April 1. doi:  10.3390/ijerph7041508
PMCID: PMC2872339

Blood Lead Levels Among Pregnant Women: Historical Versus Contemporaneous Exposures

Abstract

Blood lead among pregnant women, even at modest levels, may impair offspring cognitive development. We examine whether blood lead levels (BLLs) result from current versus historic exposures, among a cohort of pregnant women. Cumulative logit models were used to characterize the relationship between maternal risk factors and higher BLLs. Maternal blood lead levels more likely result from lead remobilization from historic versus contemporaneous exposures. Even if all lead sources were abated immediately, women and their fetuses would experience lead exposure for decades. This work emphasizes the importance of addressing sources of environmental lead exposure in the United States and internationally.

Keywords: blood lead, pregnancy, birth outcomes, lead exposure

1. Introduction

Medical professionals have long recognized severe lead poisoning as a debilitating disease. Since the late 1970s, however, mounting research demonstrates that lead causes irreversible, asymptomatic effects at levels far below thresholds previously considered safe. Research suggests that significant adverse health effects occur at blood lead levels (BLLs) below the current CDC blood lead action level of 10 μg/dL, such as learning and behavioral deficits [15] and decreased performance on standardized IQ and educational achievement tests [210]. Meta-analysis and reviews suggest that there is no threshold effect level, and thus any level of exposure is potentially detrimental [1114]. A number of studies demonstrate that significant damage occurs even at blood lead levels below 5 μg/dL [5,9,15].

Children exposed to lead in utero and in the postnatal period tend to be smaller, weaker, less coordinated, and less intelligent than children who have not had significant exposure [2,1619]. Lead exposure during the critical initial neurological development of a fetus may be particularly harmful [20]. Prenatal lead exposure has also been associated with criminality [21]. Prenatal exposure can occur if the mother is exposed to lead through infrastructure (i.e., lead-based paint), diet, cosmetics, or occupational hazards, or if she has lead stores in her body from previous exposure. Pregnant women and nursing mothers with high blood lead levels may experience elevations in both systolic and diastolic blood pressure [22,23]. In turn, elevated maternal blood pressure during pregnancy has been linked to adverse maternal health, as well as slowed and perturbed fetal development [24,25].

Blood lead levels in women of child-bearing age have been decreasing over the previous decades, but remain a concern. NHANES data from 1999 to 2002 suggest that women aged 20–59 nationally have a mean level of 1.2 μg/dL and 0.3% of women have blood lead levels above 10 μg/dL [26]. The rate is slightly lower for all females between age 6 and 19, at 0.2% with BLLs above 10 μg/dL and a mean BLL of 1.0 μg/dL. Non-Hispanic black women aged 20–59 have a significantly higher mean level of 1.4 μg/dL [26]. Other demographic predictors are consistent with those for children’s BLLs, including education, income, age of housing, as well as smoking, alcohol consumption, and living in an urban area [27].

Relatively little is known about blood lead levels in pregnant women. Rothenberg et al. examined the pattern of blood lead levels over the course of a pregnancy among 105 women residing in Mexico [28]. The authors documented a significant decrease in blood lead levels from week 12 to week 20, which they attribute to hemodilution and organ growth [28]. After 20 weeks, however, blood lead levels rose again, peaking at parturition. Similarly, Hertz-Piccioto et al. performed a nested cohort study of pregnant women in Pittsburgh, PA to examine the pattern of maternal BLLs over the course of gestation [29]. They identified the same distinct U-shaped pattern in BLLs as was found in the Rothenberg et al. study. Furthermore, they determined that older mothers had a steeper increase in BLLs during the latter half of pregnancy than did younger mothers, which was modified by calcium intake. Indeed, calcium supplementation may be a cost effective way to reduce maternal blood lead levels and thus fetal exposure [30]. The U-shaped pattern of blood lead remains despite supplementation, and Lamadrid-Figueroa et al. suggest that exposure is underestimated when plasma lead levels are not considered [31].

Positive findings of blood lead among pregnant women may be indicative of contemporaneous exposure to lead or may result from remobilization of lead from bone stores due to either the aging process or the physiological stress of pregnancy. This paper has two purposes: first, to document the blood lead burdens among a cohort of pregnant women in Durham County, NC; and second, to model predictors of blood lead level, with a particular emphasis on disentangling current versus historic exposures. The results of the analysis are directly relevant to state, national, and international lead policies, as well as the aggressiveness with which the policies should be pursued.

2. Data and Methods

The Healthy Pregnancy, Healthy Baby study is an ongoing prospective cohort study of the effects of environmental, social, and host factors on racial disparities in pregnancy outcomes. Duke University Medical Center (DUMC) Institutional Review Board approval was obtained to enroll pregnant women from the Duke Obstetrics Clinic and the Durham County Health Department Prenatal Clinic. Women were excluded from participation if they were less than 18 years of age, were not English-literate, were greater than 28 weeks’ gestation at study enrollment, lived outside of Durham County, had a multi-fetal gestation, had a known fetal genetic or congenital abnormality, or were planning not to deliver at DUMC. Demographic, health behavior, and medical history data were obtained by direct patient interview at the time of enrollment and through electronic medical record review. Participants recruited from June 2005 to December 2008 are included in this analysis. Of the 1505 women approached for participation in the study, 1294 consented (86%). At the time of this analysis, we had lead levels available for 927 of the enrolled and consented women.

All participants were geocoded to the individual tax parcel unit based on the residential address reported at time of enrollment (96.7% georeferenced). Such highly resolved spatial referencing of the data allowed each participant to be linked with parcel level data on age of housing and lead exposure risk level. The age of housing (year built) for each parcel was provided by the Durham County Tax Assessor. Each tax parcel’s lead exposure risk level was calculated using a model of lead risk that has been validated by collection of environmental samples in homes in Durham County. Using tax assessor, lead screening, and U.S. Census data, a modeled lead exposure risk estimate for each residential tax parcel was calculated by weighting risk factors for lead exposure, including age of housing, Census blockgroup median income, and Census blockgroup percent African American (detailed methods described previously in [32,33]).

Maternal blood lead levels were measured in blood samples collected at the time of admission to the DUMC Birthing Center for delivery. Whole blood, collected in a trace-metal free vacutainer tube, was sent to the Mayo Medical Laboratories for graphic furnace atomic absorption spectrometry for determination of BLLs, with a lower limit of detection of 1.0 μg/dL [34].

Descriptive statistics were calculated to provide a sense of the cohort under study in terms of both demographic data and lead exposure. The population for this analysis was restricted to non-Hispanic white, non-Hispanic black, and Hispanic participants who completed the study and had a valid lead result available. As only 10.4% of women in our study had blood lead levels greater than 1 μg/dL, we categorized blood lead into three ordered groups: below the detection limit, 1 μg/dL, and ≥2 μg/dL. We then used cumulative logit models with a proportional odds assumption to assess the relationship between maternal risk factors and higher lead levels. The proportional odds assumption implies that the covariate effects are the same across logits and was tested via a score test. The score tests on all three models that we estimate support the validity of the proportional odds assumption. We report the results in terms of adjusted odds ratios, confidence intervals, and p-values. Because these analyses were exploratory in nature, tests of statistical significance did not include an alpha adjustment. All analyses were undertaken using SAS 9.2 (Cary, NC).

3. Results

Of the 1294 participants screened and enrolled between June 2005 and December 2008, 6.5% were lost to follow-up and 2.2% withdrew. Lead results were only available for participants completing the study because blood is collected for lead analysis at delivery rather than enrollment. Lead results were available for 927 participants at the time of this study. In addition, only non-Hispanic white, non-Hispanic black, and Hispanic participants were included in the analyses presented here. Under these restrictions, data on 864 participants were used in the overall analysis. Although almost 97% of participants were geocoded, age of housing data was available for 770 of these participants and modeled lead exposure risk was available for 701 of these participants.

Table 1 provides demographic characteristics of the study population for each subset. The demographic composition of the each subsets used for the models with a measure of current lead exposure (last two columns of Table 1) were not statistically different from the demographic composition of the participants in the basic model (p > 0.05 for χ2 tests of each categorical demographic variable; p > 0.05 for t-tests for differences in mean parity). Non-Hispanic black women account for almost three-fourths (72.6% in basic model) of the study population (oversampling intentional), and roughly two-thirds (65% in the basic model) of participants were under 30 years old. Parity measures the total number of deliveries to the mother, including previous term births, previous preterm births, and the current delivery. From both national and North Carolina birth data, we know that rates of tobacco use during pregnancy are typically highest among non-Hispanic white women and lowest among Hispanic women [35,36]. Our study population does not follow this typical pattern, with tobacco use rates highest among non-Hispanic black participants (data not shown here).

Table 1.
Demographic distribution of observations in each model.

Figure 1 displays the race-specific distributions of blood lead levels among the 864 women included in the basic model. As expected, the non-Hispanic white women in our sample generally had lower blood lead levels than non-Hispanic black and Hispanic women. In our study, Hispanic women had the highest blood lead levels, with 16% having ≥ 2 μg/dL compared to 6% among non-Hispanic white and 11% among non-Hispanic black women. Almost half of the Hispanic participants in our sample were not born in the United States, so maternal lead levels for these women may be related to non-US exposures to lead.

Figure 1.
Blood lead levels by race.

As shown in Table 1 and Figure 1, both overall and within each race, the vast majority of participants had blood lead levels below the detection limit of 1 μg/dL. Only 2 participants had a blood lead level of 5 μg/dL or higher. Due to this sparseness, lead levels over 1 μg/dL were collapsed into a single category, and blood lead levels were modeled as three ordered categories of <1 μg/dL, 1 μg/dL, and ≥2 μg/dL.

Three cumulative logit models for the ordered lead level categories were fit: (1) the basic model including race, age, education, parity, and tobacco use during pregnancy; (2) the basic model covariates plus age of housing; and (3) the basic model covariates plus modeled lead exposure risk. The categories of non-Hispanic white and 25–29 years of age served as reference groups. The score tests in all three models supported the assumption of proportional odds (score test χ2 = 5.39, p = 0.86 for basic model; χ2 = 4.97, p = 0.93 for age of housing model; and χ2 = 6.14, p = 0.86 for modeled exposure risk model), indicating that the cumulative logit models could be appropriately applied in this case.

Table 2 presents the results for each model. The adjusted odds ratios (aORs) presented in this table indicate the probability of having a higher blood lead level compared to the associated reference group. In all three models, race, age, and educational attainment were significantly associated with blood lead level (p < 0.05). Tobacco use during pregnancy was also significant in the basic and age of housing models (p < 0.05; p = 0.07 in the modeled exposure risk model). Non-Hispanic black and Hispanic race were significantly associated with increased blood lead level compared to non-Hispanic white race (aOR, basic model = 2.90 and 4.92; aOR, age of housing model = 3.49 and 4.54; and aOR, modeled exposure risk model = 3.11 and 3.84, respectively; all p < 0.05). Having less than a high school education and using tobacco during pregnancy were also associated with higher blood lead levels in each model (p < 0.05 in basic and age of housing models). Parity was not significantly associated with blood lead level in any of the models, but the adjusted odds ratios on parity were in the direction we would expect; i.e., women with previous pregnancies have lower blood lead levels, potentially indicating that maternal lead body burdens may have been at least partially off-loaded during the earlier pregnancies.

Table 2.
Adjusted odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for blood lead levels in cumulative logit models.

The probability of having a higher blood lead level significantly increased with age. In the basic model, compared to the referent of 25–29 years, participants aged 18–19 had an aOR of 0.60 (95% CI = 0.28, 1.27), those aged 20–24 years an aOR of 0.54 (95% CI = 0.33, 0.89), those aged 30–34 years an aOR of 2.39 (95% CI = 1.47, 3.91), those aged 35–39 years an aOR of 2.98 (95% CI = 1.71, 5.18), and those aged 40–44 years an aOR of 7.69 (95% CI = 3.49, 16.93). A similar pattern by age was found in both models that included a measure of current lead exposure; although in the third model which added modeled lead exposure risk, the 20–24 years age category was not significantly different (p = 0.058) from the 25–29 years referent group. Note, however, that age of housing was not significant in the second model and modeled lead exposure risk was not significant in the third model. Note also that the confidence intervals are wide in some cases due to low sample sizes.

In addition, we checked the sensitivity of the results to both outliers and the classification of blood lead level. The two participants with blood lead levels >5 μg/dL, who were originally included in all three models, were removed from the dataset and the set of cumulative logit models rerun. Removing these outliers did not influence the results or their interpretation. Fitting the three models as simple logistic models with blood lead levels dichotomized as detectable/non-detectable (<1 μg/dL and ≥1 μg/dL) also did not affect the results. All standard covariates followed similar patterns, both in terms of direction and statistical significance, as in the corresponding cumulative logit models. Again, the two measures of current lead exposure, age of housing and modeled lead exposure risk, were not significantly associated with blood lead level.

4. Discussion

Maternal blood lead levels can result from either contemporaneous exposure to lead or through the remobilization of lead that has been sequestered in mineralized tissue in response to a historic exposure. When distributed to mineralized tissue, especially bone, lead is mistaken for calcium and used as faulty building blocks [37]. An estimated 95% of adult’s lead burden is in their bones, compared to over 70% of child lead stores [38]. Women are considered be at greater risk than men for the remobilization, and much literature documents the significant disparity in the large lead burdens rates of blacks [26,39].

Research indicates that bone is a living organ that accumulates lead in three compartments with three different half-lives and continually remobilizes lead to the bloodstream and other organs. The first compartment, periosteum, resembles soft tissue and is very common in growing infants. Periosteum readily releases lead stores into the bloodstream [38]. A second compartment is the spongy or trabecular bone (found in the pelvis, ribs, and skull) with a half-life of 3–5 years. The third compartment is cortical bone (midtibia and midfemur) and has the longest resident time with a half-life of about 30 years [40].

Maternal blood lead levels are inextricably tied to biological processes associated with calcium needs, absorption, and desorption. Pregnancy clearly induces a demand for calcium within the fetal compartment. Maternal responses to meet this demand can occur through increased absorption of calcium in the intestinal tract, changes in calcium conservation via kidney function, or calcium desorption from bone [41]. If lead has been absorbed into bone, then a remobilization process may be induced by pregnancy itself, due to the demand for calcium within the fetal compartment.

Maternal blood lead levels may be related to the aging process as well. Peak bone mass is generally considered to be achieved in young adulthood [42]. After bone mass peaks, bone loss begins, with resorption greater than formation [42]. Again, if lead has been used as a faulty building block in place of calcium, then we would expect blood lead levels to increase as women age. In our analysis, maternal age was strongly associated with blood lead levels, with younger women having a reduced risk and older women having an elevated risk. This result may be indicative of the resorption of bone lead in pregnancy. It may also be related to the fact that, due to the temporal patterns of lead exposure, older mothers (compared to younger mothers) may have been exposed to more lead on average during their childhoods. Other research has suggested a strong connection between bone lead and health outcomes potentially arising from exposure. In one study, bone lead predictions had more significant associations with hypertension than blood lead [43].

This study is limited to women from one county in North Carolina in a study population that intentionally oversamples non-Hispanic black women. As such, it should be replicated in other study populations to confirm the results found here. In addition, we are unable to incorporate direct measures of bone turnover, nutritional intake, or genetic vulnerabilities, all of which may contribute to blood lead levels among pregnant women. Nevertheless, we feel that the results presented here constitute an important contribution to the literature on lead levels during pregnancy.

Maternal blood lead levels may also be related to current exposures. In our analyses, however, neither measure of current lead exposure (age of housing and modeled lead exposure risk) was significantly associated with blood lead levels. The aOR for age of housing was 1.00 (95% CI = 0.99, 1.01) and for modeled lead exposure risk was 0.88 (95% CI = 0.18, 4.29). Taken in combination with the results on maternal age, this finding indicates that maternal blood lead levels are much more likely the result of lead remobilization from historic exposures as opposed to contemporaneous exposures.

5. Conclusions

The 2005–2006 National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) data reveal blood lead levels elevated above the CDC action level of 10 μg/dL in 1.3 percent of one to five year olds in the United States, with children tested having an overall geometric mean blood lead level of 1.7 μg/dL [44]. These data indicate that over 500,000 children under age six currently experience blood lead levels above the CDC blood lead action level [45]. Given the recent research on health effects of low level lead exposure, roughly 37% of children aged one to five in the United States are estimated to have blood lead levels greater than or equal to 2 μg/dL [44].

The results from this analysis indicate that pregnant women themselves (and adults more generally) are likely serving as a reservoir for lead stores. These stores are remobilized to the blood stream through natural aging processes, and rates of remobilization are likely accelerated by the physiological stress and calcium homeostasis of pregnancy. Lead diffuses from the mother’s bloodstream to the fetus across the placenta and accumulates in fetal organs. From a public health perspective, this means that even if all lead sources were abated immediately and completely, women and their growing fetuses would remain at risk for lead exposure for decades. Yet a significant number of US children today still carry unacceptable blood lead levels. These numbers are even higher globally when considering the several countries that have limited or no restrictions on lead in gasoline and industry. Thus, this work again emphasizes the critical importance of aggressively addressing sources of environmental lead exposure both in the United States and internationally.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by funding from the Environmental Protection Agency (RD-83329301-0).

References

1. Lanphear BP, Dietrich K, Auinger P, Cox C. Cognitive deficits associated with blood lead concentrations <10 μg/dL in US children and adolescents. Public Health Rep. 2000;115:521–529. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
2. Schnaas L, Rothenberg S, Flores MF, Martinez S, Hernandez C, Osorio E, Velasco SR, Perroni E. Reduced intellectual development in children with prenatal lead exposure. Environ. Health Perspect. 2006;114:791–797. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
3. Chiodo LM, Jacobson SW, Jacobson JL. Neurodevelopmental effects of postnatal lead exposure at very low levels. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2004;26:359–371. [PubMed]
4. Canfield RL, Henderson CR, Cory-Slechta DA, Cox C, Jusko TA, Lanphear BP. Intellectual impairment in children with blood lead concentrations below 10 μg per deciliter. N. Engl. J. Med. 2003;348:1517–1526. [PubMed]
5. Miranda ML, Kim D, Overstreet Galeano MA, Paul C, Hull A, Morgan SP. The relationship between early childhood blood lead levels and performance on End of Grade Tests. Environ. Health Perspect. 2007;115:1242–1247. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
6. Tong S, Baghurst P, McMichael A, Sawyer M, Mudge J. Lifetime exposure to environmental lead and children's intelligence at 11–13 years, the Port Pirie cohort study. Br. Med. J. 1996;312:1569–1575. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
7. Bellinger DC, Stiles KM, Needleman HL. Low-level lead exposure, intelligence and academic achievement, a long-term follow-up study. Pediatrics. 1992;90:855–861. [PubMed]
8. Dietrich KN, Berger OG, Succop PA, Hammond PB, Bornschein RL. The developmental consequences of low to moderate prenatal and postnatal lead exposure, intellectual attainment in the Cincinnati Lead Study Cohort following school entry. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 1993;15:37–44. [PubMed]
9. Miranda ML, Kim D, Reiter J, Overstreet Galeano MA, Maxson P. Environmental contributors to the achievement gap. Neurotoxicology. 2009;30:1019–1024. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
10. Miranda ML, Maxson P, Kim D. Early childhood lead exposure and exceptionality designations for students Int J Child Health Hum Dev 2010. 3in press [PMC free article] [PubMed]
11. Lanphear BP, Hornung R, Khoury J, Yolton K, Baghurst P, Bellinger DC, Canfield RL, Dietrich KN, Bornschein R, Greene T, Rothenberg SJ, Needleman HL, Schnaas L, Wasserman G, Graziano J, Roberts R. Low-level environmental lead exposure and children's intellectual function, an international pooled analysis. Environ. Health Perspect. 2005;113:894–899. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
12. Schwartz J. Low-level lead exposure and children's IQ, a meta-analysis and search for a threshold. Environ. Res. 1994;65:42–55. [PubMed]
13. Gatsonis CA, Needleman HL. Recent epidemiologic studies of low-level lead exposure and the IQ of children, A meta-analytic review. In: Needleman HL, editor. Human Lead Exposure. CRC Press; Boca Raton, FL, USA: 1992. pp. 244–255.
14. Schwartz J. Beyond LOEL's, p values, and vote counting, methods for looking at the shapes and strengths of associations. Neurotoxicology. 1993;14:237–246. [PubMed]
15. Needleman HL, Landrigan PJ. Letter, What level of lead in blood is toxic for a child? Am. J. Public Health. 2004;94:8. [PubMed]
16. Schwartz J, Angle C, Pitcher H. Relationship between childhood blood lead levels and stature. Pediatrics. 1986;77:281–288. [PubMed]
17. Huseman CA, Varma MM, Angle CR. Neuroendocrine effects of toxic and low blood lead levels in children. Pediatrics. 1992;90:186–189. [PubMed]
18. Gomaa A, Hu H, Bellinger D, Schwartz J, Tsaih SW, Gonzalez-Cossio T, Schnaas L, Peterson K, Aro A, Hernandez-Avila M. Maternal bone lead as an independent risk factor for fetal neurotoxicity, a prospective study. Pediatrics. 2002;110:110–118. [PubMed]
19. Jelliffe-Pawlowski LL, Miles SQ, Courtney JG, Materna B, Charlton V. Effect of magnitude and timing of maternal pregnancy blood lead (Pb) levels on birth outcomes. J. Perinatol. 2006;26:154–162. [PubMed]
20. Ronchetti R, van den Hazel P, Schoeters G, Hanke W, Rennezova Z, Barreto M, Villa MP. Lead neurotoxicity in children: Is prenatal exposure more important than postnatal exposure? Acta Paediatr. 2006;95:45–49. [PubMed]
21. Wright JP, Dietrich KN, Ris MD, Hornung RW, Wessel SD, Lanphear BP, Ho M, Rae MN. Association of prenatal and childhood blood lead concentrations with criminal arrests in early adulthood. PLoS Med. 2008;5:e101. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
22. Mushak P, Davis JM, Crocetti AF, Grant LD. Prenatal and postnatal effects of low-level lead exposure, integrated summary of a report to the U.S. Congress on childhood lead poisoning. Environ. Res. 1989;50:11–36. [PubMed]
23. Sowers M, Jannausch M, Scholl T, Li W, Kemp FW, Bogden JD. Blood lead concentrations and pregnancy outcomes. Arch. Environ. Health. 2002;57:489–495. [PubMed]
24. Hauth JC, Ewell MG, Levine RJ, Esterlitz JR, Sibai B, Curet LB, Catalano PM, Morris CD. Pregnancy outcomes in healthy nulliparas who developed hypertension. Calcium for Preeclampsia Prevention Study Group. Obstet. Gynecol. 2000;95:24–28. [PubMed]
25. Allen V, Joseph KS, Murphy K, Magee L, Ohlsson A. The effect of hypertensive disorders in pregnancy on small for gestational age and stillbirth, a population based study. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2004;4:17. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
26. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Blood Lead Levels—United States, 1999–2002. MMWR. 2005;54:513–516. [PubMed]
27. Lee MG, Chun OK, Song WO. Determinants of the blood lead level of US women of reproductive age. J. Am. Coll. Nutr. 2005;24:1–9. [PubMed]
28. Rothenberg SJ, Karchmer S, Schnaas L, Perroni E, Zea F, Fernandez AJ. Changes in serial blood lead levels during pregnancy. Environ. Health Perspect. 1994;102:876–880. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
29. Hertz-Picciotto I, Schramm M, Watt-Morse M, Chantala K, Anderson J, Osterloh J. Patterns and determinants of blood lead during pregnancy. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2000;152:829–837. [PubMed]
30. Ettinger AS, Lamadrid-Figueroa H, Tellez-Rojo MM, Mercado-Garcia A, Peterson KE, Schwartz J, Hu H, Hernandez-Avila M. Effect of calcium supplementation on blood lead levels in pregnancy, a randomized placebo-controlled trial. Environ. Health Perspect. 2009;117:26–31. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
31. Lamadrid-Figueroa H, Tellez-Rojo MM, Hernandez-Cadena L, Mercado-Garcia A, Smith D, Solano-Gonzalez M, Hernandez-Avila M, Hu H. Biological markers of fetal lead exposure at each stage of pregnancy. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health A. 2006;69:1781–1796. [PubMed]
32. Miranda ML, Dolinoy DC, Overstreet MA. Mapping for prevention, GIS models for directing childhood lead poisoning prevention programs. Environ. Health Perspect. 2002;110:947–953. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
33. Kim D, Overstreet Galeano MA, Hull A, Miranda ML. A Framework for widespread replication of a highly spatially resolved childhood lead exposure risk model. Environ. Health Perspect. 2008;116:1735–1739. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
34. Mayo Medical Laboratories. Test Catalog. Mayo Clinic; Rochester, MN, USA: 2008.
35. National Center for Health Statistics. VitalStats. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; Hyattsville, MD, USA: Sep 1, 2009.
36. Detailed Birth Record. North Carolina State Center for Health Statistics; Raleigh, NC, USA: 2007. (Confidential Patient Database)
37. Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry. The Nature and Extent of Childhood Lead Poisoning in Children in the United States. Department of Health and Human Services/Public Health Service; Atlanta, GA, USA: 1988.
38. Anderson AC, Pueschel SM, Linakis JG. Pathophysiology of lead poisoning. In: Pueschel SM, Linakis JG, Anderson AC, editors. Lead Poisoning in Childhood. Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co; Baltimore, MD, USA: 1996.
39. Theppeang K, Glass TA, Bandeen-Roche K, Todd AC, Rohde CA, Schwartz BS. Gender and race/ethnicity differences in lead dose biomarkers. Am. J. Public Health. 2008;98:1248–1255. [PubMed]
40. Pueschel SM, Linakis JG, Anderson A. A historical perspective. In: Pueschel SM, Linakis JG, Anderson AC, editors. Lead Poisoning in Childhood. Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co; Baltimore, MD, USA: 1996.
41. Sowers MF, Scholl T, Harris L, Jannausch M. Bone loss in adolescent and adult pregnant women. Obstet. Gynecol. 2000;96:189–193. [PubMed]
42. Ilich JZ, Kerstetter JE. Nutrition in bone health revisited, A story beyond calcium. J. Am. Coll. Nutr. 2000;19:715–737. [PubMed]
43. Park SK, Mukherjee B, Xia X, Sparrow D, Weisskopf MG, Nie HL, Hu H. Bone lead level prediction models and their application to examine the relationship of lead exposure and hypertension in the third national health and nutrition examination Survey. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 2009;51:1422–1436. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
44. National Center for Health Statistics. National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey Data 2005–2006. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; Hyattsville, MD, USA: 2008.
45. Census 2000, Summary File 3. USA Census Bureau; Washington, DC, USA: 2006. Available online: http://factfinder.census.gov (accessed on October 5, 2009).

Articles from International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)