PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of advcogpsychAbout ACPSubscribeSumit a manuscriptACP Journal
 
Adv Cogn Psychol. 2007; 3(1-2): 107–109.
Published online 2008 July 15. doi:  10.2478/v10053-008-0018-z
PMCID: PMC2864972

Grouping based feature attribution in metacontrast masking

Abstract

The visibility of a target can be strongly suppressed by metacontrast masking. Still, some features of the target can be perceived within the mask. Usually, these rare cases of feature mis-localizations are assumed to reflect errors of the visual system. To the contrary, I will show that feature "mis-localizations" in metacontrast masking follow rules of motion grouping and, hence, should be viewed as part of a systematic feature attribution process.

Keywords: metacontrast masking, feature attribution, feature integration, motion grouping, attention

Feature mis-localization in metacontrast masking

In metacontrast masking, the visibility of a target is reduced by a temporally succeeding and spatially non-overlapping mask (Alpern, 1953; Stigler, 1910; for a recent monograph see Breitmeyer & Öğmen, 2006). Metacontrast masking yields non-monotonic U-shaped masking functions, that is, performance on the target is most deteriorated for intermediate SOAs. For example, if a single line is followed by a pair of flanking lines after about 50 ms, only the flanking lines are perceived (Figure 1A). However, the target is clearly visible if the SOA is either very short (e.g. 0 ms) or very long (>150 ms).

Figure 1.
Classical metacontrast masking. (A) A central line is followed by two, non-overlapping flanking lines. The central line is rendered largely invisible if the flanks appear about 50 ms later. (B) Feature mis-localization in metacontrast ...

Surprisingly, when the target line itself is invisible, some features of the suppressed target can be perceived as mis-localized within the flanking lines (Figure 2B). Werner (1935) was the first to observe feature mis-localizations in metacontrast masking. When he presented a polygon followed by a surrounding ring, the ring appeared as a “ring with teeth” (Werner, 1935, p. 58). Similarly, there are other anecdotal reports of feature mis-localization (e.g., Hogben & Di Lollo, 1984; Stewart & Purcell, 1970; Stoper & Banffy, 1977), but only a few systematic studies (Hofer, Walder, & Groner, 1989; Wilson & Johnson, 1985). It has been shown that not only contour features of a target can be inherited but also brightness (Burr, 1984; Toch, 1956), and that the duration of an invisible target can contribute to the perceived duration of the following mask (Scharlau, this volume). Moreover, feature mis-localizations can occur in pattern masking (Herzog & Koch, 2001). In summary, although the visibility of a target can be strongly reduced in metacontrast masking, several features of the target can be perceived within the mask. Here, the question arises, if the target itself is suppressed, how are these features processed?

Figure 2.
Sequential metacontrast. The central target line is followed by a sequence of flanking lines, here by two streams of lines shifting to the left. (A) Observers were asked to report the offset of the attended left stream of lines. If only ...

Feature attribution is determined by motion grouping

Recently, Otto, Öğmen, and Herzog (2006) introduced a paradigm, coined sequential metacontrast, to study feature attribution in metacontrast masking. In sequential metacontrast, a target line is not only followed by one pair of flanking lines (as in Figure 1A), but by sequences of lines. These sequences elicit the percept of lines in apparent motion, whereas the target line is not visible itself. If the first line is offset (as in Figure 1B), this offset can be perceived to be mis-localized in the motion streams in a rather broad spatial window up to

0.5 deg (Otto et al., 2006). Interestingly, if multiple offsets are presented within one stream of lines, these offsets can be integrated with the target line offset. Importantly, this feature integration occurs only within a grouped motion stream. For example in Figure 2, two streams of lines shifting in the same direction are presented after the display of the target line. If the target line is offset, observers report a corresponding offset in both the left and the right motion stream (Figure 2A, D). However, if a second offset is added non-ambiguously either to the right (Figure 2B, E) or left motion stream (Figure 2C, F), performance strongly differs depending on which stream was attended, although the physical stimulus is exactly the same. Hence, two offsets, even if they are presented at the same spatial location as in Figure 2B, E, are only integrated if they belong to the same attended motion stream.

To summarize, the visibility of a target can be strongly suppressed by sequential metacontrast masking. However, although the target line itself is invisible, its offset can be perceived as mis-localized within the masking lines. Usually, the rare cases of feature mis-localizations are interpreted to reflect limitations or errors of the visual system. For example, illusory conjunctions- the incorrect perceptual combinations of correctly perceived features like color and shape- usually occur when the observer’s attention is diverted (Treisman & Schmidt, 1982). Consequently, this illusory feature mis-localization has been interpreted to result from limited attentional resources. Similarly, feature mis-localizations in metacontrast masking might be explained in terms of a limited processing capacity of the visual system unable to cope with the fast rate of stimulus presentations. However, the selectivity of feature integration in sequential metacontrast indicates that grouping operations can access and process individual features prior to an integration stage (Figure 2). Hence, the feature “mis-localizations” in sequential metacontrast masking should not be viewed as errors of the visual system, but rather as part of a systematic process of feature attribution determined by attention and motion grouping (Öğmen, Otto, & Herzog, 2006; Otto et al., 2006). The exact underlying mechanisms- while possibly involving recurrent processing as proposed by Hamker (this volume) – have to be unearthed in future research.

Acknowledgements

The experiment presented in Figure 2 was partly published in the Journal of Vision. I thank the Journal of Vision for the permission to use the original material.

References

  • Alpern M. Metacontrast. Journal of the Optical Society of America. 1953;43:648–657. [PubMed]
  • Breitmeyer B. G., Öğmen H. Visual masking: Time slices through conscious and unconscious vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2006.
  • Burr D. C. Summation of target and mask metacontrast stimuli. Perception. 1984;13:183–192. [PubMed]
  • Hamker F. E. The mechanisms of feature inheritance as predicted by a systems-level model of visual attention and decision making. Advances in Cognitive Psychology. 2007;3:111–123. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Herzog M. H., Koch C. Seeing properties of an invisible object: feature inheritance and shine-through. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2001;98:4271–4275. [PubMed]
  • Hogben J. H., Di Lollo V. Practice reduces suppression in metacontrast and in apparent motion. Perception & Psychophysics. 1984;35:441–445. [PubMed]
  • Öğmen H., Otto T. U., Herzog M. H. Perceptual grouping induces non-retinotopic feature attribution in human vision. Vision Research. 2006;46:3234–3242. [PubMed]
  • Otto T. U., Öğmen H., Herzog M. H. The flight path of the phoenix-the visible trace of invisible elements in human vision. Journal of Vision. 2006;6:1079–1086. [PubMed]
  • Scharlau I. Temporal processes in prime–mask interaction: Assessing perceptual consequences of masked information. Advances in Cognitive Psychology. 2007;3:241–255. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Stewart A. L., Purcell D. G. U-shaped masking functions in visual backward masking - effects of target configuration and retinal position. Perception & Psychophysics. 1970;7:253–256.
  • Stigler R. Chronophotische Studien über den Umgebungskontrast. Pflügers Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie. 1910;134:365–435.
  • Stoper A. E., Banffy S. Relation of split apparent motion to metacontrast. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1977;3:258–277. [PubMed]
  • Toch H. H. The perceptual elaboration of stroboscopic presentations. American Journal of Psychology. 1956;69:345–358. [PubMed]
  • Treisman A., Schmidt H. Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects. Cognitive Psychology. 1982;14:107–141. [PubMed]
  • Werner H. Studies on contour: I. Qualitative analyses. American Journal of Psychology. 1935;47:40–64.

Articles from Advances in Cognitive Psychology are provided here courtesy of University of Finance and Management in Warsaw