PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptNIH Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. Author manuscript; available in PMC May 1, 2011.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC2862788
NIHMSID: NIHMS153570
Cholinergic Mechanisms, Early Brain Development, and Risk for Schizophrenia
Randal G Ross,a Karen E Stevens,ba William R Proctor,ba Sherry Leonard,ba Michael A Kisley,c Sharon K Hunter,a Robert Freedman,ba and Catherine E Adamsba
aDepartment of Psychiatry, University of Colorado Denver, Aurora, CO, USA
bDepartment of Research, Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Denver, Colorado, USA
cDepartment of Psychology, University of Colorado Colorado Springs, Colorado Springs, CO, USA
Correspondence to Randy Ross, MD, University of Colorado Denver, Box F546, 13001 E 17th Pl, Denver, CO, USA; Phn: 1-303-724-6203; randy.ross/at/ucdenver.edu
Neuropsychiatric diseases are complex illnesses where the onset of diagnostic symptomology is often the end result of a decades-long process of aberrant brain development. The identification of novel treatment strategies aimed at normalizing early brain development and preventing mental illness should be a major therapeutic goal; however, there are few models for how this goal might be achieved. This report uses the attentional deficits of schizophrenia as an example and reviews data from genetic, anatomical, physiological, and pharmacologic studies to hypothesize a developmental model with translational primary prevention implications. Specifically, the model suggests that an early interaction between α7 nicotinic receptor density and choline availability may contribute to the development of schizophrenia-associated attentional deficits. Translational implications, including perinatal dietary choline supplementation, are discussed. It is hoped that presentation of this model will stimulate other efforts to develop empirically-driven primary prevention strategies.
Most psychiatric illnesses are complex diseases. Like most complex diseases (e.g. heart disease, diabetes, asthma, etc.), the complexity is found in multiple domains: 1) the diseases are heterogeneous in that the symptoms vary from individual to individual; 2) the diseases are multi-factorial with etiologic factors are both genetic and environmental and these etiologic factors are likely interactive rather than additive; and 3) the diseases are developmental suggesting that many of the brain changes that lead to disease are present years to decades prior to onset of the diagnostic symptomology. The development of new and better treatments for psychiatric illnesses is a critical goal for the field; however, for complex illnesses, successful strategies to decrease morbidity and mortality also often include efforts in primary prevention. However, because primary prevention, by definition, includes intervention before onset of disease, efforts need to focus not on the disease itself, but on risk factors for the disease; and often, when the disease is symptomologically complex, on risk factors for components of the disease. Primary prevention has not been a major focus of psychiatric research; and thus there are few models for psychiatric-illness primary prevention research. This manuscript attempts to decrease that gap by focusing on a single component (attentional dysfunction) in a single illness (schizophrenia). The approach uses multiple levels of analyses, from neural circuits to symptomatic behavior (see Figure 1), to describe a neurodevelopmental model which includes an interaction between genetic and environmental factors and a biological marker of risk. Preclinical studies which support the potential for a primary prevention strategy are also reviewed.
Figure 1
Figure 1
Analysis of a developmental illness can occur at several levels and at several ages. deficits at each level correlate with each other. Schizophrenia (represented in black) occurs in adults, adolescents, and rarely in children and is associated with attentional, (more ...)
Schizophrenia has often been conceptualized as a disorder of at least 3 symptom domains: positive symptoms (such as hallucinations and delusions), negative symptoms (such as lack of strong affect and motivation), and cognitive dysfunction (such as problems in attention and working memory). Chronic presentation of the positive symptoms, and to a large extent the negative symptoms, are relatively specific to schizophrenia. The diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia reflect this specificity being heavily weighted towards these two symptom domains. The onset of chronic positive symptoms suggestive of schizophrenia has been documented in a child as young as 3 years of age (Beresford, Hepburn, & Ross, 2005) and in individuals over 75 years of age (Barak, Aizenberg, Mirecki, Mazeh, & Achiron, 2002; Howard, Castle, Wessely, & Murray, 1993). However, the majority of cases have onset of hallucinations and delusions between 15 and 35 years of age (Morgan, Castle, & Jablensky, 2008), leading to multiple research efforts focused on early identification (McKenna, Gordon, & Rapoport, 1994; Miller et al., 1999; Preda et al., 2002; Ross et al., 2003; Schaeffer & Ross, 2002; Yung et al., 2003), neurocognitive presentation (Davalos, Compagnon, Heinlein, & Ross, 2004; Cornblatt, Obuchowski, Roberts, Pollack, & Erlenmeyer-Kimling, 1999; Seidman et al., 2006), brain changes (Giedd et al., 1999; Thompson et al., 2001; Vidal et al., 2006) and treatment efforts (Cornblatt, McGorry, McGlashan, & Ross, 2000) in the timeframe preceding the period of highest risk, generally between about 7 and 19 years of age.
While positive symptoms generally develop in late adolescence or early adulthood, the cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia, including problems in attention, develop much earlier. Schizophrenia-associated cognitive impairments are stable across the course of the disease (Rund, 1998; Heaton et al., 2001; Censits, Ragland, Gur, & Gur, 1997; Hill, Schuepbach, Herbener, Keshevan, & Sweeney, 2004) and present at the same level of impairment at first episode (Bilder et al., 2000; Hill et al., 2004; Hoff et al., 1999; Mohamed, Paulsen, O'Leary, Arndt, & Andreason, 1999; Saykin et al., 1994). Because the diagnostic (positive) symptoms do not generally present until adolescence or later, inferences about onset of cognitive deficits are generally drawn from groups with higher risk for later developing schizophrenia. For example, having a parent with schizophrenia increases the risk a child will later develop schizophrenia by approximately 10 times, so a parental history of schizophrenia is often used as a marker of risk. Neuropsychological, behavioral, and symptomatic assessments of children with a parent with schizophrenia demonstrate that schizophrenia-associated attentional difficulties are fully present as young as 6 years of age (Rieder & Nichols, 1979; Cornblatt et al., 1999; Rosenberg et al., 1997; Jones, Rodgers, Murray, & Marmon, 1994; Niendam et al., 2003; Erlenmeyer-Kimling et al., 2000) and those who develop the psychotic symptomology come from this group of children who have attentional deficits (Erlenmeyer-Kimling et al., 2000; Marcus, Hans, Auerbach, & Auerbach, 1993; Gheorge, Baloescu, & Grigorescu, 2004; Davidson et al., 1999). Attentional deficits can be thought of as an early expression of risk for the disorder.
Standardized, well-accepted assessments of attention are generally only available for children 6 years of age and older and the majority of work on age-of-onset of attentional dysfunction has focused on children age 6 years and above. As a result, until recently, there has been little attempt to determine if the attentional problems are present at even younger ages such as in preschoolers or even infantrs. However, delays in infant motor development correlate with later cognitive performance and infants who later develop schizophrenia have delays in early motor development (Ridler et al., 2006) suggesting that schizophrenia-associated cognitive dysfunction may be present by early infancy.
While attentional tests for adults and older children can be verbally based, neither verbally-based instructions nor verbally-based outcomes are possible with young infants. In young infants, attentional measures are limited to those that can be inferred from physiological measurements such as auditory evoked potentials (e.g. P50 auditory evoked potential sensory gating, mismatch negativity), heart rate (e.g. heart rate changes in response to visual stimuli and heart rate variability), autonomic responsivity (e.g. prepulse inhibition of startle), and visual-based attention tasks (such as the visual expectation task). We use P50 sensory gating here as a illustrative example because of its association with the genetics (Leonard et al., 2002; Flomen et al., 2006) and cognitive deficits of schizophrenia (described below) and its applicability to infant populations (Kisley, Polk, Ross, Levisohn, & Freedman, 2003b).
Physiological tests of attention are based on the concepts that (a) selective attention toward one stimulus requires the inhibition of attention towards other stimuli and (b) that much of this inhibitory process is automatic. For example, when a hippocampal pyramidal cell is tuned to specific stimulus characteristics, this also leads to increased activity in surrounding inhibitory interneurons. The inhibitory interneurons both inhibit activity in neighboring pyramidal cells (maximizing spectral discrimination) and loop back to inhibit the initial pyramidal cell (maximizing temporal discrimination; see Figure 2B).
Figure 2
Figure 2
The α7 nicotinic receptor and local inhibitory circuits at different stages of development. (A) Early in brain development, GABA is an excitatory neurotransmitter and the α7 nicotinic receptor is found in multiple locations (generalized) (more ...)
The P50 sensory gating paradigm tests the local inhibitory circuit by presenting two identical auditory stimuli 500 milliseconds apart. For most individuals, there is a strong evoked potential approximately 50 milliseconds after the first stimulus, and a much reduced or “gated” response to the second stimulus. The ratio of the amplitude of the second P50 wave to the first P50 wave is the dependent measure. (A ratio of 0 represents complete suppression of the second response and is considered optimal inhibitory circuit performance; most adults have ratios below 0.4; see Figure 3A).
Figure 3
Figure 3
(reprinted with permission from Kisley et al, Figure 1 (Kisley et al., 2003b)): P50 sensory gating in (A) a normal adult and (B) an adult with schizophrenia. Evoked potentials are recorded after 2 identical stimuli presented 500 milliseconds apart. The (more ...)
There is adrenergic input into the local inhibitory circuit and increased adrenergic tone (as might be caused by anxiety or stress) temporarily blocks sensory gating, increasing the P50 ratio (Adler et al., 1990). Infants are affectively volatile, with frequent unpredictable agitation, including high effort motor activity and crying. Older infants, with their improved motor control, will also often interfere with physiological recording by unintentionally grabbing recording wires and removing them from the body surface. The combination of agitation, motor activity, and unintentional interference makes reliable electrophysiological recording difficult. In addition, high adrenergic tone can create false positives for abnormal P50 sensory gating (Adler et al., 1990); thus, agitation creates a confounding variable in interpreting evoked potential results. Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep (termed active sleep in an infant) is a period where motor activity, agitation, and adrenergic tone are decreased or absent. In adults, we have demonstrated that both normal and schizophrenia-associated abnormalities in P50 sensory gating are unchanged between waking and REM sleep (Kisley, Olincy, & Freedman, 2001; Kisley et al., 2003a), so REM sleep provides an optimal environment to assess infants. Figure 4 presents examples of both intact and impaired infant sensory gating recorded during REM sleep (Kisley et al., 2003b). Recording infants during REM sleep is also reliable with a test-retest reliability of 0.87 (Hunter, Corral, Ponicsan, & Ross, 2008).
Figure 4
Figure 4
(reprinted with permission from Stevens et al, Figure 1 (Stevens et al., 1996)): Grand-average (a) and individual examples (b,c) of responses by infants during REM sleep by conditioning (left) and test (right) clicks. Clicks occur at 0 ms. Component P1 (more ...)
P50 sensory gating is correlated with other measures of attention, for example with performance on the digit vigilance task (Cullum et al., 1993) and with a self-report of the ability to selectively attend (Kisley, Noecker, & Guinther, 2004). As a physiological correlate of aspects of attentional function, P50 sensory gating shows promise as a surrogate for infant development of attention.
Consistent with their known attentional deficits, individuals with schizophrenia have elevated (impaired) P50 sensory gating ratios (Figure 3B). Schizophrenia-associated P50 sensory gating deficits have been reliably demonstrated across laboratories (Table 1). Meta-analyses suggest an overall effect size of 1.28-1.56 (de Wilde, Bour, Dingemans, Koelman, & Linszen, 2007b; Bramon, Rabe-Hesketh, Sham, Murray, & Frangou, 2004), which can be increased to 1.8 with careful attention to sound levels, subject position, and filter settings (de Wilde et al., 2007b). Like their affected relatives, around 50% of unaffected first-degree relatives have impairments in attention (Laurent et al., 2000; Ma et al., 2007; Birkett et al., 2007) and abnormalities in P50 sensory gating (Freedman et al., 2000); the attentional and P50 sensory gating deficits co-segregate in families with schizophrenia (Harris et al., 1996; Ross et al., 1999). For these unaffected relatives, a meta-analysis identified an effect size for the P50, relative to controls, of 0.85 (de Wilde et al., 2007b). The impact of having a first-degree relative with chronic psychosis is already present by a few weeks after birth (Hunter, Kisley, Freedman, & Ross, 2008), suggesting that, for vulnerable individuals, the perinatal period may be a time of particular malleability.
Table 1
Table 1
Replication studies of the P50 sensory gating in schizophrenia, 2000- March 2008. All papers compare individuals with schizophrenia to a normal comparison group. Only the latest report per group from 2000 or later is shown.
The development of physiological (non-verbal) correlates of attention, such as P50 sensory gating, are not only necessary for the study of preverbal children, but are also of utility in developing convergent animal models.
Mice demonstrate a similar auditory sensory gating phenomenon to humans: when presented with two identical auditory stimuli, the early evoked response (the N40) to the second (test) auditory stimulus is dramatically reduced compared to response to the first (conditioning) stimulus the response is gated). Notably, when compared across several inbred mouse strains, the degree of gating is correlated with the amount of hippocampal α-bungarotoxin binding, a measure of genetically-mediated α7 nicotinic receptor expression (95). For example, the C3H mouse expresses a high level of hippocampal α7 nicotinic receptors and has a high degree of sensory gating (the N40 evoked response is lower to the second sound than the first). The DBA/2 mouse has a strain-specific restriction fragment length polymorphism in the α7 receptor gene locus (79) associated with decreased density of α7 nicotinic receptor binding (97) and impaired sensory gating (95). Figure 5 illustrates the differences in sensory gating seen between C3H and DBA/2 mice.
Figure 5
Figure 5
(copied with permission from Adams et al, Figure 1 (Adams, 2003)): Auditory evoked potentials in response to two identical clicks presented 500 milliseconds apart. Arrows represent the presentation of the stimuli; amplitudes of the N40 wave are measured (more ...)
Two other mouse models, congenic and heterozygotic knockouts, support the role of the α7 nicotinic receptor in sensory gating. Congenic mice are mice in which, through selective breeding, a small region of one chromosome is moved onto the genetic background of another strain. When the chromosomal region containing the α7 nicotinic receptor gene from a DBA/2 mouse is bred onto a C3H background, the animal gates like a DBA/2 mouse, not a C3H mouse (Adams, Yoncheck, & Stitzel, 2006). This supports the role of this region of chromosome on gating ability. Heterozygotic knockout mice were developed on a C3H background; one of the α7 nicotinic receptor genes is a “normal gene;” the other has been removed. The heterozygotic knockout has α 7 nicotinic receptors less than 50% of the levels of the wildtype animal (Adams, Yoncheck, Zheng, Collins, & Stevens, 2008). While C3H mice generally show good sensory gating (that is a low test/conditioning ratio), the heterozygotic knockouts have poor sensory gating, supporting the specific role of the α7 nicotinic receptor gene.
Given that schizophrenia-associated attentional deficits are seen years to decades prior to onset of the diagnostic symptomology, it is important to assess whether the α7 nicotinic receptor plays a role not only in adult circuit functioning, but also in the developmental processes which create that circuit. To explore the effects of this receptor on early development, DBA/2 and C3H mouse brains were analyzed prenatally (Adams, 2003). A differential level and distribution of the α7 receptor was identifiable as early as the first day receptors could be seen and continued throughout prenatal and early postnatal development. Figure 6 summarizes the prenatal results: α -bungarotoxin binding, reflecting α7 nicotinic receptor densities, is first identifiable on embryonic day 13 (E13) in C3H mice, but not until E16 in the DBA/2 mouse. For C3H mice, binding begins in the dorsal portion of the hippocampal anlage, while for the DBA/2 mouse binding is first identified in area CA3. C3H mice show an increase in binding in hippocampal areas CA1and CA3 between E18 and postnatal day 5, while DBA/2 mice show no such increase.
Figure 6
Figure 6
(A) Binding for α-bungarotoxin (α-BTX) is initially observed in the dorsal portion of the hippocampal anlage of C3H mice on embryonic day 13 (E13). Binding is both diffuse (arrowheads) and clumped (arrows) in appearance. No α-BTX (more ...)
In summary, across prenatal development, C3H mice express α7 nicotinic receptors sooner, at higher levels, and in different locations than DBA/2 mice. In particular, C3H mice exhibit a rapid, large, and transient increase in receptors late in gestation that is not reproduced in the DBA mouse. The increased availability of perinatal receptors available in the C3H mouse does not occur in congenic mice (DBA α7 nicotinic receptor on a C3H background); the lack of the α7 receptor increase is associated later in development with impaired adult sensory gating. The close relationship between perinatal α7 nicotinic receptor density and physiological outcome suggests that adequate stimulation of the α7 nicotinic receptor is necessary for optimal development of these local inhibitory circuits.
The strong relationship between early α7 nicotinic receptor density and later sensory gating suggests normal development of inhibitory neurocircuits requires adequate stimulation of the α7 nicotinic receptor. This suggests that both receptor density and ligand availability may play critical roles.
For rodent models, hippocampal choline acetyltransferase is absent until late prenatal or early postnatal development (Makuch et al., 2001; Descarries, Aznavour, & Hamel, 2008; Perry, Piggott, Court, Johnson, & Perry, 1993). Thus, while sufficient stimulation of the receptor appears to be necessary for normal local inhibitory circuit development, the early developmental ligand for this receptor is not acetylcholine. At physiological concentrations, choline is a selective α7 agonist (Uteshev, Meyer, & Papke, 2003), and it appears that choline acts as the primary ligand during the prenatal period.
Choline is actively transported through the placenta and then into the brain, leading to fetal brain choline concentrations several times maternal serum levels (Zeisel, 2006). Females synthesize choline de novo; however de novo synthesis is insufficient to meet fetal demands (Garner, Mar, & Zeisel, 1995; Zeisel, 2000; Zeisel, Mar, Zhou, & da Costa, 1995); maternal dietary intake is also required. Thus, by manipulating maternal dietary choline, it is possible to manipulate choline availability and to assess the role of choline in development of P50 sensory gating. Two such manipulations have been completed.
In the first, an animal model with good P50 sensory gating, Sprague-Dawley rats were fed choline-deficient diets during embryonic days 12-18, a critical developmental period (Meck, Williams, Cermak, & Blusztajn, 2008). The offspring of that pregnancy were then fed a normal-choline diet until adulthood. When tested as young adults, a history of deficient perinatal dietary choline is associated with impaired P50 sensory gating (Figure 7)(Stevens, Adams, Mellot, Robbins, & Kisley, 2008b).
Figure 7
Figure 7
(copied with permission from Stevens et al, Figure 2 (Stevens et al., 2008b)): The test to conditioning N40 ratio for young adult rats gestated on normal (con), deficient (def), or supplemented (sup) diets during gestation. N= 14 (con) 13 (def) and 7 (more ...)
While the choline deprivation model is helpful in exploring the concept of choline involvement in hippocampal development, the ubiquitous presence of choline makes it highly unlikely that humans will be gestated on a diet with a deficit limited to choline. In contrast, the DBA/2 mouse model, as a model of a genetically vulnerable organism, may be more translatable to the human condition. The DBA/2 mouse has a strain-specific restriction fragment length polymorphism in the α7 receptor gene locus (Stitzel, Farnham, & Collins, 1996) associated with decreased density of α7 nicotinic receptor binding (Adams, Stitzel, Collins, & Freedman, 2001; Stitzel et al., 1996) and impaired sensory gating (Stevens et al., 1996). Pregnant DBA/2 mice were given daily dietary supplements of choline from onset of pregnancy through weaning; then sensory gating was measured in the pups after they reached early adulthood. Choline supplementation during the perinatal period increased α-bungarotoxin binding in the adult brain (Figure 8) and normalized adult sensory gating (Figure 9), consistent with the hypothesis that choline supplementation normalizes hippocampal developmental by stimulation of the α7 nicotinic receptor (Stevens et al., 2008a).
Figure 8
Figure 8
(copied with permission from Stevens et al, Figure 3 (Stevens et al., 2008a)). Gestational supplementation with choline produced an overall increase in α-bungarotoxin (α7) binding in the hippocampus of DBA/2 mice. Regionally, both the (more ...)
Figure 9
Figure 9
The DBA/2 mouse has been utilized extensively as a model for one aspect of schizophrenia-associated brain functioning. Both individuals with schizophrenia and DBA/2 mice have a polymorphism in CHRNA7, a gene associated with decreased α7 nicotinic (more ...)
Based on the human adult and animal models, we propose a model suggesting that the development of schizophrenia-associated attentional dysfunction occurs as a result of an interaction, during very early brain development, between α7 nicotinic receptor density and choline availability. Receptor density and choline availability are both likely multi-determined, but both have genetic contributions. Genetic variation in the α7 nicotinic receptor gene (CHRNA7), neuregulin (which is influential in α7 receptor activity), and phosphatidylethanolamine-N-methyltransferase (PEMT; a synthetic enzyme for de novo synthesis of choline) would all be predicted to impact the developmental process. Notably, while the genetics of schizophrenia remain incompletely determined, all three of these genes have been associated with risk for schizophrenia (CHRNA7: (Freedman et al., 2001; Leonard et al., 1998; Liu et al., 2001; Riley et al., 2000; Xu et al., 2001; Tsuang et al., 2001; Gejman, Sanders, Badner, Cao, & Zhang, 2001); neuregulin: (Mathew et al., 2007; Addington et al., 2007; Walss-Bass et al., 2006; Munafo, Thiselton, Clark, & Flint, 2006; Norton et al., 2006; Hall, Gogos, & Karayiorgou, 2004; Li et al., 2004; Yang et al., 2003; Williams et al., 2003); PEMT (Liu et al., 2007)). CHRNA7 polymorphisms have also been associated with P50 sensory gating performance in a non-psychotic population (Leonard et al., 2002).
The enzyme phosphatidylethanolamine-N-methyltransferase (PEMT) allows for human de novo synthesis of choline. In the healthy premenopausal adult female, this de novo synthesis is normally sufficient to meet daily needs (Fischer et al., 2007). However, pregnancy is a time of heavy choline demand, primarily because of the strong fetal need for choline-containing-phospholipids as a necessary component of new cell development. Pregnant women are unable to synthesize all of the required choline and therefore dietary sources are needed; variation in dietary intake can lead to relative choline deficiency in some pregnant women (Gossell-Williams, Fletcher, McFarlane-Anderson, Jacob, & Zeisel, 2005). The combination of lowered alpha7 nicotinic receptor density and lower choline availability may alter inhibitory neurocircuit development and increase risk for later schizophrenia-associated attentional deficits (Figure 10).
Figure 10
Figure 10
A developmental hypothesis for development of schizophrenia-associated deficits.
There has been a hesitation to translate genetic findings into primary prevention strategies with concerns predominantly focused on the risks of pharmacological intervention in vulnerable but not yet affected brains. The ultimate risk of primary prevention is the introduction of negative consequences in individuals who may never develop the illness of interest. One of the fortuitous components of the current developmental hypothesis is the role of choline, a commonly occurring nutritional component included in the class of substances the U.S. Federal Drug Administration defines as “generally regarded as safe.” It may be possible to supplement pregnant women with choline-containing compounds during pregnancy at doses sufficient to prevent acute deficiency with minimal risk of negative consequences.
The development of any primary prevention strategy is difficult; low frequency of illness and long duration between intervention and outcome are particularly problematic methodological issues. For example, given the low frequency of schizophrenia in the general population, even optimistic projections of effect size and attrition rates suggests a controlled perinatal choline supplementation trial might require enrollment in the tens of thousands of pregnant women with 30+ year follow-up of their offspring. In other illnesses and with other prevention strategies, primary prevention trials have enrolled tens of thousands (e.g. see (Bartolucci & Howard, 2006)), and decades-long follow-up of perinatal intervention is possible (e.g. (Olds et al., 1998; Olds, 2006). However, prior to devoting the resources necessary such a trial, several intermediate steps need to be completed.
First and foremost is safety. Choline is included in products categorized by the U.S. Food and Drug Administration as “generally regarded as safe;” and supplementation doses that fall within the normal range of daily intake likely have low risk. However, particularly given the vulnerability of the population to be exposed (including fetuses), careful attention to safety is warranted.
Second, the biological effect of choline supplementation should be established. P50 sensory gating appears strong influenced by α7 nicotinic receptor stimulation. If perinatal choline has a positive impact on infant P50 sensory gating, this would support the biological impact of the intervention and may clarify which doses during which critical windows are most likely to be effective.
Third, assess more proximal intermediate outcomes. The prevalence of schizophrenia is around 1% in the general population and onset generally is not until late adolescence or early adulthood. However attentional dysfunction not only is present by early school-age years, it is more penetrant than the disease itself. The type of attentional dysfunction associated with schizophrenia is relatively common in the general population. While the etiology of attentional function is likely as complex as the full schizophrenia phenotype with differences in perinatal α7 nicotinic receptor stimulation explaining only a small part of the variance, the fact that dysfunction is more prevalent and is measureable at much young ages raises the possibility that the impact of perinatal intervention may be measurable with smaller sample sizes and with much shorter follow-up than possible when waiting for disease onset. Relating infant P50 sensory gating to childhood attentional function, both in general and in the presence of peri-natal choline supplementation, are important next steps in model development.
One of the fundamental promises of molecular studies has been the identification of drug targets amenable to intervention. For example, in adults with schizophrenia, the hypothesized relationship between genetic polymorphisms, α7 nicotinic receptor density, electrophysiologically measured sensory gating, and attentional dysfunction has led to clinical trials, with early success, of nicotinic agonists (Olincy et al., 2006; Lee, Lee, Lee, & Kim, 2007; Schubert, Young, & Hicks, 2006). However, for the development of primary prevention strategies, the value to identifying etiologic agents may not be in their direct application to primary prevention strategy. Instead, a less direct process may be more appropriate, first using genetic findings to advance developmental models only then followed by primary prevention development. For example, the association between CHRNA7 and schizophrenia has been useful in creating animal exemplars, which in turn have allowed for advancement of neurocircuitry paradigms, and testing of potential primary interventions. In this case, the association between CHRNA7 and schizophrenia led to a developmental model where local inhibitory neurocircuits maturation requires adequate stimulation of the α7 nicotinic receptor at appropriate times during early development. Once the model was developed, it was possible to recognize that CHRNA polymorphisms are only one of many factors which could effects this ligand-receptor interaction. Other genetic influenced possibilities include neuregulin levels (neuregulin influences α7 receptor activity), and phosphatidylethanolamine-N-methyltransferase activity (which would influence in vivo synthesis of choline). Environmental factors that may influence this process include dietary variation in choline intake and maternal stress (which may lead to sequestration of free choline away from fetal access). Independent of the cause of the disruption, the model hypothesizes that a maternal increase in dietary choline would compensate. Thus, while the relation between any etiologic factor and developmental effects may be low, choline supplementation during early development may have a measurable impact.
Similar to decreasing the risk of spina bifida by encouraging all pregnant women to take folate or decreasing the risk for dental caries by population-wide fluoride supplementation, schizophrenia may best be treated by preventive strategies years or even decades prior to the onset of diagnostic symptomology. As is true across multiple medical diseases, a focus on primary prevention aimed at countering risk factors has the potential to dramatically reduce morbidity and mortality for severe psychiatric illness.
The model does not rule out that alternative interactions elsewhere in the circuit might lead to similar neurocognitive difficulties, and it is likely that brain maldevelopment in other locations or other at other times during development also contributes to schizophrenic psychopathology. It is also probable that additional work on the model will enhance the value of some components while reducing or invalidating the value of others. The model is presented here as an example of the process by which interdisciplinary efforts to coordinate genetic, animal, and human studies can lead to the identification of testable hypotheses related to novel primary prevention strategies.
Key points
  • Neurodevelopmental illnesses are complex diseases where illness is the end result of an often decades-long interaction between genetic and environmental factors. The perinatal period may be a stage where even small interventions may alter the developmental course.
  • No single etiologic factor, be it genetic or environmental, will account for much of the risk in developing the disorder. However, identification of even small contributors to illness may be applicable to animal modeling, lead to neurodevelopmental models, which then may suggest primary prevention strategies.
  • The study of cholinergic mechanisms, their role in early brain development, and the relationship of this early brain development to risk for schizophrenia-associated cognitive deficits may be a model for developing mental illness primary prevention strategies.
  • Prenatal activation of α7 nicotinic receptors by maternal dietary choline supplementation deserves further exploration as a primary prevention strategy for the cognitive deficits of schizophrenia.
Acknowledgements
This material is based upon work supported in part by the National Institutes of Health (grants AA016548, DA009457, MH056539, MH068582, MH073725, and MH081177), the Department of Veterans Affairs, Veterans Health Administration, Office of Research and Development (MERIT Awards to KES, WRP, SL, RF, and CEA, and a Career Award to SL), and NARSAD (Toulmin Independent Investigator Award to KES and Young Investigator Award to SKH).
  • Adams CE. Comparison of alpha7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor development in the hippocampal formation of C3H and DBA/2 mice. Brain Research.Developmental Brain Research. 2003;143:137–149. [PubMed]
  • Adams CE, Stitzel JA, Collins AC, Freedman R. Alpha 7-nicotinic receptor expression and the anatomical organization of hippocampal interneurons. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts. 2001;22:377.
  • Adams CE, Yoncheck JC, Stitzel JA. Development of hippocampal alpha7 nicotinic receptors in C3H and DBA/2 congenic mice. Brain Research. 2006;1122:27–35. [PubMed]
  • Adams CE, Yoncheck JC, Zheng L, Collins AC, Stevens KE. Altered hippocampal circuit function in C3H alpha7 null mutant heterozygous mice. Brain Research. 2008;1194:138–145. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Addington AM, Gornick MC, Shaw P, Seal J, Gogtay N, Greenstein D, et al. Neuregulin 1 (8p12) and childhood-onset schizophrenia: susceptibility haplotypes for diagnosis and brain developmental trajectories. Molecular Psychiatry. 2007;12:195–205. [PubMed]
  • Adler LE, Gerhardt GA, Franks R, Baker N, Nagamoto H, Drebing C, et al. Sensory physiology and catecholamines in schizophrenia and mania. Psychiatry Research. 1990;31:297–309. [PubMed]
  • Arnfred SM, Chen AC, Glenthoj BY, Hemmingsen RP. Normal P50 gating in unmedicated schizophrenia outpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:2236–2238. [PubMed]
  • Barak Y, Aizenberg D, Mirecki I, Mazeh D, Achiron A. Very late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis: clinical and imaging characteristics in comparison with elderly patients with schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2002;190:733–736. [PubMed]
  • Bartolucci AA, Howard G. Meta-analysis of data from the six primary prevention trials of cardiovascular events using aspirin. American Journal of Cardiology. 2006;98:746–750. [PubMed]
  • Becker J, Gomes J, Ghisolfi ES, Schuch A, Ramos RL, Ehlers JA, et al. Clozapine, but not typical antipsychotics, corrects corrects P50 suppression deficit in patients with schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol. 2004;115:396–401. [PubMed]
  • Beresford C, Hepburn S, Ross RG. Schizophrenia in preschool children: 2 case reports with longitudinal follow-up for 5.92 and 8.6 Years. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005;10:429–439.
  • Bilder RM, Goldman RS, Robinson D, Reiter G, Bell L, Bates JA, et al. Neuropsychology of first-episode schizophrenia: initial characterization and clinical correlates. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;157:549–559. [PubMed]
  • Birkett P, Sigmundsson T, Sharma T, Toulopoulou T, Griffiths TD, Reveley A, et al. Reaction time and sustained attention in schizophrenia and its genetic predisposition. Schizophrenia Research. 2007;95:76–85. [PubMed]
  • Boutros NN, Korzyukov O, Jansen B, Feingold A, Bell M. Sensory gating deficits during the mid-latency phase of information processing in medicated schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry Research. 2004;106:203–215. [PubMed]
  • Bramon E, Rabe-Hesketh S, Sham P, Murray RM, Frangou S. Meta-analysis of the P300 and P50 waveforms in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2004;70:315–329. [PubMed]
  • Brockhaus-Dumke A, Schultze-Lutter F, Mueller R, Tendolkar I, Bechdolf A, Pucrop R, et al. Sensory gating in schizophrenia: P50 and N100 gating in antipsychotic-free subjects at risk, first-episode, and chronic patients. Biological Psychiatry. (in press) [PubMed]
  • Cadenhead KS, Light GA, Geyer MA, Braff DL. Sensory gating deficits assessed by the P50 event-related potential in subjects with schizotypal personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;157:55. [PubMed]
  • Censits DM, Ragland JD, Gur RC, Gur RE. Neuropsychological evidence supporting a neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia: a longitudinal study. Schizophrenia Research. 1997;24:289–298. [PubMed]
  • Clementz BA, Blumental LD. Multichannel electroencephalographic assessment of auditory evoked response suppression in schizophrenia. Experimental Brain Research. 2001;139:377–390. [PubMed]
  • Cornblatt B, McGorry PD, McGlashan TH, Ross RG. Atypical antipsychotics in prodromal schizophrenia.. International Conference for Early Onset Psychosis..2000.
  • Cornblatt B, Obuchowski M, Roberts S, Pollack S, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L. Cognitive and behavioral precursors of schizophrenia. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11:487–508. [PubMed]
  • Cullum CM, Harris JG, Waldo MC, Smernoff E, Madison A, Nagamoto HT, et al. Neurophysiological and neuropsychological evidence for attentional dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 1993;10:131–141. [PubMed]
  • Davalos DB, Compagnon N, Heinlein S, Ross RG. Neuropsychological deficits associated with genetic predisposition to schizophrenia in school-age children. Schizophrenia Research. 2004;67:123–130. [PubMed]
  • Davidson M, Reichenberg A, Rabinowitz J, Weiser M, Kaplan Z, Mark M. Behavioral and intellectual markers for schizophrenia in apparently healthy male adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156:1328–1335. [PubMed]
  • de Wilde OM, Bour LJ, Dingemans PM, Koelman JHTM, Linszen DH. Failure to find P50 suppression deficits in young first-episode patients with schizophrenia and clinically unaffected siblings. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2007a;33:1319–1323. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • de Wilde OM, Bour LJ, Dingemans PM, Koelman JJ, Linszen DH. A meta-analysis of P50 studies in patients with schizophrenia and relatives: differences in methodology between research groups. Schizophrenia Research. 2007b;97:137–151. [PubMed]
  • Descarries L, Aznavour N, Hamel E. The acetylcholine innervation of cerebral cortex: new data on its normal development and its fate in the hAPP(SW,IND) mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neural Transmission. 2008;112:149–162. [PubMed]
  • Erlenmeyer-Kimling L, Rock D, Roberts SA, Janal M, Kestenbaum C, Cornblatt B, et al. Attention, memory, and motor skills as childhood predictors of schizophrenia-related psychoses: the New York High-Risk Project. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;157:1416–1422. [PubMed]
  • Fischer LM, daCosta KA, Kwock L, Stewart PW, Lu TX, Stabler SP, et al. Sex and menopausal status influence human dietary requirements for the nutrient choline. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2007;85:1275–1285. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Flomen RH, Collier DA, Osborne S, Munro J, Breen G, St Clair D, et al. Association study of CHRFAM7A copy number and 2 bp deletion polymorphisms with schizophrenia and bipolar affective disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics: Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2006;141B:575. [PubMed]
  • Freedman R, Adams C, Adler L, Bickford-Wimer PC, Gault J, Harris JG, et al. An inhibitory neurophysiological deficit as a phenotype for genetic investigation of schizophrenia. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2000;97:58–64. [PubMed]
  • Freedman R, Leonard S, Gault JM, Hopkins J, Cloninger CR, Kaufmann CA, et al. Linkage disequilibrium for schizophrenia at the chromosome 15q13-14 locus of the alpha 7-nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subunit gene (CHRNA7). American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2001;105:20–22. [PubMed]
  • Garner SC, Mar M, Zeisel SH. Choline distribution and metabolism in pregnant rats and fetuses are influenced by the choline content of the maternal diet. Journal of Nutrition. 1995;125:2851–2858. [PubMed]
  • Gejman PV, Sanders AR, Badner JA, Cao O, Zhang J. Linkage analysis of schizophrenia to chromosome 15. American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics) 2001;8:789–793. [PubMed]
  • Gheorge MD, Baloescu A, Grigorescu G. Premorbid cognitive and behavioral functioning in military recruits experiencing the first episode of psychosis. CNS Spectrums. 2004;9:604–606. [PubMed]
  • Giedd JN, Jeffries NO, Blumenthal J, Castellanos FX, Vaituzis AC, Fernandez T, et al. Childhood-onset schizophrenia: Progressive brain changes during adolescence. Biological Psychiatry. 1999;46:892–898. [PubMed]
  • Gossell-Williams M, Fletcher H, McFarlane-Anderson N, Jacob A, Zeisel S. Dietary intake of choline and plasma choline concentrations in pregnant women in Jamaica. West Indian Medical Journal. 2005;54:355–359. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Hall D, Gogos JA, Karayiorgou M. The contribution of three strong candidate schizophrenia susceptibility genes in demographically distinct populations. Genes, Brain, & Behavior. 2004;3:240–248. [PubMed]
  • Harris JG, Adler LE, Young DA, Cullum CM, Rilling LM, Cicerello A, et al. Neuropsychological dysfunction in parents of schizophrenics. Schizophrenia Research. 1996:252–260. [PubMed]
  • Heaton RK, Gladsjo JA, Palmer BW, Kuck J, Marcotte TD, Jeste DV. Stability and course of neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia . Archives of General Psychiatry. 2001;58:24–32. [PubMed]
  • Hill SK, Schuepbach D, Herbener ES, Keshevan MS, Sweeney JA. Pretreatment and longitudinal studies of neuropsychological deficits in antipsychotic-naive patients with schizophrenia . Schizophrenia Research. 2004;68:49–63. [PubMed]
  • Hoff AL, Sakuma M, Wieneke M, Horon R, Kushner M, DeLisi LE. Longitudinal neuropsychological follow-up study of patients with first-episode schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156:1336–1341. [PubMed]
  • Hong LE, Summerfelt A, Wonodi I, Adami H, Buchanana RW, Thaker GK. Independent domains of inhibitory gating in schizophrenia and the effect of stimulus interval. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164:61–65. [PubMed]
  • Houy E, Raux G, Thibaut F, Belmont A, Demily C, Allio G, et al. The promoter -194C polymorphism of the nicotinic alpha 7 receptor gene has a protective effect against the P50 sensory gating deficit. Molecular Psychiatry. 2004;9:320–322. [PubMed]
  • Howard R, Castle D, Wessely S, Murray R. A comparative study of 470 cases of early-onset and late-onset schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1993;163:352–357. [PubMed]
  • Hsieh MH, Liu K, Liu SK, Chiu MJ, Hwu HG, Chen AC. Memory impairment and auditory evoked potential gating deficit in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research. 2004;130:161–169. [PubMed]
  • Hunter SK, Corral N, Ponicsan H, Ross RG. Reliability of p50 auditory sensory gating measures in infants. Neuroreport. 2008;19:79–82. [PubMed]
  • Hunter SK, Kisley MA, Freedman R, Ross RG. Association of parental psychosis and nicotine use with impaired auditory sensory gating in newborns. American Journal of Psychiatry. (in press)
  • Jones P, Rodgers B, Murray R, Marmon M. Child developmental risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet. 1994;344:1398–1402. [PubMed]
  • Kisley MA, Noecker TL, Guinther PM. Comparison of sensory gating to mismatch negativity and self-reported perceptual phenomena in healthy adults. Psychophysiology. 2004;41:604–612. [PubMed]
  • Kisley MA, Olincy A, Freedman R. The effect of state on sensory gating: comparison of waking, REM and non-REM sleep. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2001;112:1154–1165. [PubMed]
  • Kisley MA, Olincy A, Robbins E, Polk SD, Adler LE, Waldo MC, et al. Sensory gating impairment associated with schizophrenia persists into REM sleep. Psychophysiology. 2003a;40:29–38. [PubMed]
  • Kisley MA, Polk SD, Ross RG, Levisohn PM, Freedman R. Early postnatal development of sensory gating. Neuroreport. 2003b;14:693–697. [PubMed]
  • Laurent A, Biloa-Tang M, Bougerol T, Duly D, Anchisi AM, Bosson JL, et al. Executive/attentional performance and measures of schizotypy in patients with schizophrenia and in their nonpsychotic first-degree relatives. Schizophrenia Research. 2000;46:269–283. [PubMed]
  • Lee SW, Lee JG, Lee BJ, Kim YH. A 12-week, double.blind, placebo-controlled trial of galantamine adjunctive treatment to conventional antipsychotics for the cognitive impairments in chronic schizophrenia. International Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2007;22:63–68. [PubMed]
  • Leonard S, Gault J, Hopkins J, Logel J, Vianzon R, Short M, et al. Association of promoter variants in the alpha7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subunit gene with an inhibitory deficit found in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2002;59:1085–1096. [PubMed]
  • Leonard S, Gault J, Moore T, Hopkins J, Robinson M, Olincy A, et al. Further investigation of a chromosome 15 locus in schizophrenia: analysis of affected sibpairs from the NIMH Genetics Initiative. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 1998;81:308–312. [PubMed]
  • Li T, Stefansson H, Gudfinnsson E, Cai G, Liu X, Murray RM, et al. Identification of a novel neuregulin 1 at-risk haplotype in Han schizophrenia Chinese patients, but no association with the Icelandic/Scottish risk haplotype. Molecular Psychiatry. 2004;9:698–704. [PubMed]
  • Liu CM, Hwu HG, Lin MW, Ou-Yang WC, Lee SFC, Fann CSJ, et al. Suggestive evidence for linkage of schizophrenia to markers at chromosome 15q13-14 in Taiwanese families. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2001;105:658–661. [PubMed]
  • Liu Y, Zhang H, Ju G, Zhang X, Xu Q, Liu S, et al. A study of the PEMT gene in schizophrenia. Neuroscience Letters. 2007;424:203–206. [PubMed]
  • Louchart-de la Chapelle S, Nkam I, Houy E, Belmont A, Menard JF, Roussignol AC, et al. A concordance study of three electrophysiological measures in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162:466–474. [PubMed]
  • Ma X, Wang Z, Sham PC, Liu X, Rabe-Hesketh S, Sun X, et al. Neurocognitive deficits in first-episode schizophrenic patients and their first-degree relatives. American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics) 2007;144:407–416. [PubMed]
  • Makuch R, Baratta J, Karaelias LD, Lauterborn JC, Gall CM, Yu J, et al. Arrival of afferents and the differentiation of target neurons: Studies of developing cholinergic projections to the dentate gyrus. Neuroscience. 2001;104:81–91. [PubMed]
  • Marcus J, Hans SL, Auerbach JG, Auerbach AG. Children at risk for schizophrenia: The Jerusalem Infant Development Study. II. Neurobehavioral deficits at school age. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1993;50:797–809. [PubMed]
  • Martin LF, Mei-Hua H, Ross RG, Zerbe G, Freedman R, Olincy A. Physiology of schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and schizoaffective disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164:1900–1906. [PubMed]
  • Mathew SV, Law AJ, Lipska BK, Davila-Garcia MI, Zamora ED, MItkus SM, et al. Alpha7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor mRNA expression and binding in postmortem human brain are associated with genetic variation i neuregulin 1. Hum Mol Genet. 2007;16:2921–2932. [PubMed]
  • McKenna K, Gordon CT, Rapoport JL. Childhood-onset schizophrenia: timely neurobiological research. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1994;33:771–781. [PubMed]
  • Meck WH, Williams CL, Cermak JM, Blusztajn JK. Developmental periods of choline sensitivity provide an ontogenetic mechanisms for regulating memory capacity and age-related dementia. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. 2008;1 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Miller TJ, McGlashan TH, Woods SW, Stein K, Driesen N, Corcoran CM, et al. Symptom assessment in schizophrenic prodromal states. Psychiatric Quarterly. 1999;70:273–287. [PubMed]
  • Mohamed S, Paulsen JS, O'Leary D, Arndt S, Andreason N. Generalized cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: a study of first-episode patients . Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56:749–754. [PubMed]
  • Morgan VA, Castle DJ, Jablensky AV. Do women express and experience psychosis differently from men? Epidemiological evidence from the Australian National Study of Low Prevalence (Psychotic) Disorders. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;42:74–82. [PubMed]
  • Munafo MR, Thiselton DL, Clark TG, Flint J. Association of the NRG1 gene and schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry. 2006;11:539–546. [PubMed]
  • Niendam TA, Bearden CE, Rosso IM, Sanders A, Hadley T, Nuechterlein KH, et al. A prospective study of childhood neurocognitive functioning in schizophrenic patients and their siblings. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:2060–2062. [PubMed]
  • Norton N, Moskvina V, Morris DW, Bray NJ, Zammit S, Williams NM, et al. Evidence that interaction between neuregulin 1 and its receptor erbB4 increases susceptibility to schizophrenia. American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics) 2006;141:96–101. [PubMed]
  • Olds D, Henderson CR, Jr., Cole R, Eckenrode J, Kitzman H, Luckey D, et al. Long-term effects of nurse home visitation on children's crminal and antisocial behavior. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1998;280:1238–1244. [PubMed]
  • Olds DL. The nurse-family partnership: An evidence-based preventive intervention. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2006;27:5–25.
  • Olincy A, Harris JG, Johnson LL, Pender V, Kongs S, Allensworth D, et al. Proof-of-concept trial of an á7 nicotinic agonist in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:630–638. [PubMed]
  • Patterson JV, Jin Y, Gierczak M, Hetrick WP, Potkin S, Bunney WE, Jr., et al. Effects of temporal variability on p50 and the gating ratio in schizophrenia: a frequency domain adaptive filter single-trial analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2000;57:57–64. [PubMed]
  • Perry EK, Piggott MA, Court JA, Johnson M, Perry RH. Transmitters in the developing and senescent human brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1993;695:69–72. [PubMed]
  • Preda A, Miller TJ, Rosen JL, Somjee L, McGlashan TH, Woods SW. Treatment histories of patients with a syndrome putatively prodromal to schizophrenia. Psychiatric Services. 2002;53:342–344. [PubMed]
  • Price GW, Michie PT, Johnston J, Innes-Brown H, Kent A, Clissa P, et al. A multivariate electrophysiological endophenotype, from a unitary cohort, shows greater research utility than any single feature in the Western Australian Family Study of Schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;60:1–10. [PubMed]
  • Ridler K, Veijola JM, Tanskanen P, Miettunen J, Suckling J, Murray GK, et al. Fronto-cerebellar systems are associated with infant motor and adult executive functions in healthy adults but not in schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2006;103:15651–15656. [PubMed]
  • Rieder RO, Nichols PL. Offspring of schizophrenics III: Hyperactivity and neurological soft signs. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1979;36:665–674. [PubMed]
  • Riley BP, Makoff AM, Magudi-Carter M, Jenkins TJ, Williamson R, Collier DA, et al. Haplotype transmission disequilibrium and evidence for linkage of the CHRNA7 gene region to schizophrenia in Southern African Bantu families. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2000;96:196–201. [PubMed]
  • Ringel TM, Heidrich A, Jacob CP, Fallgater AJ. Sensory gatin gdeficit in a subtype of chronic schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry Res. 2004;125:237–245. [PubMed]
  • Rosenberg DR, Sweeney JA, Squires-Wheeler E, Keshavan MS, Cornblatt BA, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L. Eye tracking dysfunction in offspring from the New York High Risk Project: diagnostic specificity and the role of attention. Psychiatry Research. 1997;66:121–130. [PubMed]
  • Ross RG, Olincy A, Harris JG, Radant AD, Hawkins M, Adler LE, et al. Evidence for bilineal inheritance of physiological indicators of risk in childhood-onset schizophrenia. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 1999;88:188–199. [PubMed]
  • Ross RG, Schaeffer J, Compagnon N, Heinlein S, Beresford C, Farley G. Creating School-age versions of semistructured interviews for the prodrome to schizophrenia: Lessons from case reviews. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2003;29:729–736. [PubMed]
  • Rund BR. A review of longitudinal studies of cognitive functions in schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1998;24:425–435. [PubMed]
  • Sanchez-Morla EM, Garcia-Jimenez MA, Barabash A, Martinez-Vizcaino V, Mena J, Cabranes-Diaz JA, et al. P50 sensory gating deficit is a common marker of vulnerability to bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2008;117:313–318. [PubMed]
  • Saykin AJ, Shtasel DL, Gur RE, Kester DB, Mozley LH, Stafiniak P, et al. Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia . Archives of General Psychiatry. 1994;51:124–131. [PubMed]
  • Schaeffer J, Ross RG. Childhood-onset schizophrenia: Premorbid and prodromal diagnosis and treatment histories. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41:538–545. [PubMed]
  • Schubert MH, Young KA, Hicks PB. Galantamine improves cognition in schizophrenic patients stabilized on risperidone. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;60:530–533. [PubMed]
  • Seidman LJ, Giuliano AJ, Smith CW, Stone WS, Glatt SJ, Meyer E, et al. Neuropsychological functioning in adolescents and young adults at genetic risk for schizophrenia and affective psychoses: results from the Harvard and Hillside Adolescent High Risk Studies. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2006;32:507–524. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Stevens KE, Adams C, Yonchek J, Hickel C, Danielson J, Kisley M. Permanent improvement in deficient sensory inhibition in DBA/2 mice with increased perinatal choline. Psychopharmacology. 2008a;198:413–420. [PubMed]
  • Stevens KE, Freedman R, Collins AC, Hall M, Leonard S, Marks MJ, et al. Genetic correlation of inhibitory gating of hippocampal auditory evoked response and alpha-bungarotoxin-binding nicotinic cholinergic receptors in inbred mouse strains. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1996;15:152–162. [PubMed]
  • Stevens K, Adams CE, Mellot TJ, Robbins E, Kisley MA. Perinatal choline deficiency produces abnormal sensory inhibition in Sprague-Dawley rats. Brain Research. 2008b;1237:84–90. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Stitzel JA, Farnham DA, Collins AC. Linkage of strain-specific nicotinic receptor alpha 7 subunit restriction fragment length polymorphisms with levels of alpha-bungarotoxin binding in the brain. Brain Research. 1996;43:30–40. [PubMed]
  • Thoma RJ, Hanon FM, Lanoue MD, Miller GA, Heller W, Edgar C, et al. Lateralization of auditory sensory gating and neuropsychological dysfunction in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:1595–1605. [PubMed]
  • Thompson PM, Vidal C, Giedd JN, Gochman P, Blumental J, Nicolson R, et al. Mapping adolescent brain change reveals dynamic wave of accelerated gray matter loss in very early-onset schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2001;98:11650–11655. [PubMed]
  • Tsuang DW, Skol A, Faraone SV, Bingham S, Young KA, Prabhudesai S, et al. Genetic linkage of the CHRNA7 region of chromosome 15 to schizophrenia in a large Veterans Affairs cooperative study sample. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2001;105:662–668. [PubMed]
  • Uteshev VV, Meyer EM, Papke RL. Regulation of neuronal function by choline and 4OH-GTS-21 through alpha7 nicotinic receptors. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2003;89:1797–1806. [PubMed]
  • Vidal CN, Rapoport JL, Hayashi KM, Geaga JA, Sui Y, McLemore LE, et al. Dynamically spreading frontal and cingulate deficits mapped in adolescents with schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;61:25–34. [PubMed]
  • Walss-Bass C, Liu W, Lew DF, Villegas R, Montero P, Dassori A, et al. A novel missense mutation in the transmembrane domain of neuregulin 1 is associated with schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;60:548–553. [PubMed]
  • Wegrzyn J, Wciorka J. P50 component of auditory evoked potentials in persons with schizophrenia and their first degree relatives. Psychiatia Polska. 2004;38:395–408. [PubMed]
  • Williams NM, Preece A, Spurlock G, Norton N, Williams HJ, Zammit S, et al. Support for genetic variation in neuregulin 1 and susceptibility to schizophrenia. Moelcular Psychiatry. 2003;8:485–487. [PubMed]
  • Xu J, Pato MT, Torre CD, Medeiros H, Carvalho c., Basile VS, et al. Evidence for linkage disequilibrium between the alpha 7-nicotinic receptor gene (CHRNA&) locus and schizophrenia in Azorean families. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2001;105:669–674. [PubMed]
  • Yang JZ, Si TM, Ruan Y, Ling YS, Han YH, Wang XL, et al. Association study of neuregulin 1 gene with schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry. 2003;8:706–709. [PubMed]
  • Yung AR, Phillips LJ, Yuen HP, Francey SM, McFarlane CA, Hallgren M, et al. Psychosis prediction: 12-month follow up of a high risk (“prodromal”) group. Schizophrenia Research. 2003;60:21–32. [PubMed]
  • Zeisel SH. Choline: needed for normal development of memory. Journal of the American College of Nutrition. 2000;19(5 Suppl):528S–531S. [PubMed]
  • Zeisel SH. Choline: Critical role during fetal development and dietary requirements in adults. Annual Review of Nutrition. 2006;26:229–250. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Zeisel SH, Mar MH, Zhou Z, da Costa KA. Pregnancy and lactation are associated with diminished concentrations of choline and its metabolites in rat liver. Journal of Nutrition. 1995;125:3049–3054. [PubMed]