PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of procbThe Royal Society PublishingProceedings BAboutBrowse by SubjectAlertsFree Trial
 
Proc Biol Sci. 2009 November 22; 276(1675): 4035–4041.
Published online 2009 August 26. doi:  10.1098/rspb.2009.1147
PMCID: PMC2825779

Termites eavesdrop to avoid competitors

Abstract

Competition exclusion, when a single species dominates resources due to superior competitiveness, is seldom observed in nature. Termites compete for resources with deadly consequences, yet more than one species can be found feeding in the same wooden resource. This is especially surprising when drywood species, with colonies of a few hundred, are found cohabiting with subterranean species, with colonies of millions. Termites communicate vibro-acoustically and, as these signals can travel over long distances, they are vulnerable to eavesdropping. We investigated whether drywood termites could eavesdrop on vibration cues from subterranean species. We show, using choice experiments and recordings, that the drywood termite Cryptotermes secundus can distinguish its own species from the dominant competitor in the environment, the subterranean termite Coptotermes acinaciformis. The drywood termite was attracted to its own vibration cues, but was repelled by those of the subterranean species. This response increased with decreasing wood size, corresponding with both increased risk and strength of the cue. The drywood termites appear to avoid confrontation by eavesdropping on the subterranean termites; these results provide further evidence that vibro-acoustic cues are important for termite sensory perception and communication.

Keywords: aggression, competition, communication, eavesdropping, Isoptera, social information

1. Introduction

Competition exclusion, when a single species dominates resources due to superior competitiveness (Hardin 1960), is seldom observed in nature. For example, ant communities are well known for competition/dominance hierarchies (e.g. Fellers 1987; Hölldobler & Wilson 1990; Andersen 1992, 1997), yet a weakening or even a complete lack of competitive exclusion is usually observed. Like ants, termites (Order Isoptera) compete for resources, often with deadly consequences (Nel 1968; Wood & Lee 1971; Thorne & Haverty 1991). Colony size and soldier number are important features of competitive ability in ants (Hölldobler & Wilson 1990; Gordon & Kulig 1996) and are also important features for termite competitive ability; termite species with small colonies have been observed to yield territory and food, even be eliminated by, those with large colonies (Levings & Adams 1984; Leponce et al. 1997). Despite this, competitive exclusion is incomplete as more than one species can be found foraging simultaneously on the same food resource, even in relatively small pieces of food (Hill 1942; Roisin et al. 2006; Schuurman 2006).

The mechanism for avoiding competitive exclusion in termites has not been investigated and remains unknown. In ants, subordinate species avoid exclusion by foraging at temperatures (e.g. Bestelmeyer 2000) and times (e.g. Albrecht & Gotelli 2001) different from those preferred by dominant species. It is possible that subordinate termite species avoid dominant competitors using similar tactics, but this is unlikely because temperature variation is less marked where most termites are found (inside wood and soil) than where most ants are found (on the soil surface). For similar reasons, time and light differences are much less important.

Another possible mechanism may be eavesdropping. Eavesdropping, or social information, is when one species listens to the alarm calls given by other species to provide information about possible danger, which has been studied primarily in vertebrates such as monkeys (Seyfarth & Cheney 1990; Zuberbühler 2000) and birds (Otter et al. 1999; Templeton & Greene 2007). A similar ability of detecting heterospecific signals has been reported in bees, but the bees are detecting and responding to foraging signals instead of alarm signals, and in doing so improve their foraging efficiency (Nieh et al. 2004; Reader et al. 2005; Yokoi et al. 2007).

Eavesdropping may be more likely to evolve when differences between competitors are extreme. Perhaps the most extreme difference in competitive ability in termites found in the same location is between drywood termites such as Cryptotermes and the tree ‘piping’ subterranean termites Coptotermes. Both genera infest trees as they eat sound, dry wood; they are among the few genera that eat heartwood and so have become pests of buildings. Drywood termites appear to be especially vulnerable: Cryptotermes colonies originate in branches from winged reproductives and Cryptotermes queens do not enlarge their ovaries or abdomens, and so lay few eggs; thus their colonies are usually small (typically 200–300 individuals; Gay & Watson 1982; Korb & Katrantzis 2004; Korb & Schmidinger 2004). Cryptotermes colonies contain few (sometimes only one) defensive soldiers, which have relatively small jaws, instead relying on their phragmotic heads to block tunnels. Also, drywood termite colonies remain within the single tree or piece of wood as they are unable to tunnel through soil, thus preventing escape.

In comparison, Coptotermes are strong competitors. They are the dominant wood-eating termite in Australia; they infest more than 85 per cent of trees (Hill 1942; Werner et al. 2008). They originate from winged reproductives also, but Coptotermes queens in their ground-based colonies become physogastric, producing colonies of one million or more individuals, with tens of thousands of aggressive soldiers with long biting mandibles and sticky glue secretions (Evans et al. 1998, 1999). Coptotermes colonies forage on up to 20 trees simultaneously; they enter trees through their roots and ‘pipe’ the tree by eating the inner heartwood up to the branches (Hill 1942; Greaves 1959)—and so produce the raw material for the Aboriginal musical instrument, the didgeridoo (Tarnopolsky et al. 2005; Werner et al. 2008; figure 1). Despite the apparently overwhelming competitiveness of Coptotermes, Cryptotermes colonies can coexist in Coptotermes-infested trees (Hill 1942; Gay & Watson 1982).

Figure 1.

Schematic diagram of a tree infested by a colony of Cryptotermes (black) started in a dead branch stub by winged reproductives and Co. acinaciformis (white), foraging from their mound-nest into the trunk via the roots. The Co. acinaciformis have hollowed ...

We tested the hypothesis that Cryptotermes were detecting Coptotermes and avoiding them. Termites generate vibrations when they chew and they use these vibrations to determine wood size and the quality of the food source (Evans et al. 2005, 2007; Inta et al. 2007). Given that termites use vibration alarm signals generated by soldiers (Howse 1964; Röhrig et al. 1999; Inta et al. 2009), it seemed possible that species could detect other species using just vibration signals or cues from vibrations caused during feeding.

2. Material and methods

(a) Test species

We used the co-existing termite species Cryptotermes secundus and Coptotermes acinaciformis (Hill 1942; Gay & Watson 1982), and collected the Cr. secundus from trees and the Co. acinaciformis from mound nests in the field near Darwin, Northern Territory, Australia (latitude 12.4°S, longitude 130.9°E). We housed the termites in constant-temperature rooms (28°C and 80% RH) in Canberra. We used 5070 Cr. secundus ‘workers’ from 31 colonies (4290 as test termites and 780 as cue termites) and approximately 16 800 Co. acinaciformis workers from five colonies as cue termites. Although often referred to as workers, the drywood termites that help in the colony are not true workers, but totipotent nymphs that have delayed development (see Korb & Katrantzis 2004; Korb & Schmidinger 2004).

(b) Laboratory bioassays

We had three sizes of paired wooden blocks: 20, 160 and 400 mm long. We used these different block sizes to test the hypothesis that Cr. secundus detect and avoid Co. acinaciformis only when the perceived risk was high, with risk inversely correlated with distance to Co. acinaciformis. To ensure paired blocks would be as similar as possible, we cut them sequentially from lengths of seasoned, air-dried Pinus radiata wood of 20 × 20 mm cross section. The ‘test termites’ were those whose behaviour was under test. There were 15 test Cr. secundus termites in each replicate, which were held in a 15 mm long central chamber made from tape, aluminium foil and/or plastic sheeting between the just-cut surfaces of the paired blocks; thus, the test termites encountered two almost identical wooden faces.

The ‘cue termites’ were those that provided the cue, to which the test termites were exposed. We had four cue treatments: live Cr. secundus, recorded vibration cues of Cr. secundus, live Co. acinaciformis and recorded vibration cues of Co. acinaciformis. We also had a no-cue control. The cue treatment was applied to one wooden block in each pair. For the live termite cue treatments, we made cages from acrylic tube (20 mm diameter), aluminium foil and tape. A side chamber was placed on each free end (distal to the central chamber containing the test termites) of the paired wooden blocks; only one side chamber received termites, either 15 Cr. secundus workers or 300 Co. acinaciformis workers. Both side chambers in the Co. acinaciformis treatment had damp vermiculite clay to provide water (figure 2).

Figure 2.

Schematic diagram of the experiment apparatus. (a) No-cue control; (b) live termite cue treatments; (c) recorded cue treatments. Wooden blocks were paired to be as similar as possible. There were three sizes: 20, 160 and 400 mm long.

The different termite numbers were a consequence of availability and physiology. A large number of Co. acinaciformis was necessary because they die rapidly in small groups isolated from their colonies under these conditions; 300 was the smallest group size with any survivors at the end of the experiment, although there were often no survivors even for this group size. Cryptotermes secundus colony size is usually less than 300, so it was almost impossible to obtain numbers equal to those of Co. acinaciformis. However, survival is usually very high, perhaps due in part to their water requirements; they need only what they metabolize from their food. The numerical difference was unlikely to equate to a difference in vibration cue activity as the cut end of the wooden blocks (on which the termites were able to chew) could accommodate only around 20 termites. More importantly, the activity rates in the recorded cues were equal for all species and wood block size combinations (see §2c), thus providing a control for signal termite numbers.

We played the recorded cues as .wav files from CDs (Sony D-EJ100) through Philip Harris vibration generators (model F4H31134, Leicester, UK), which were attached to the free end (distal to the central cell) of one of the paired wooden blocks using an aluminium beam and a wood screw. The CDs played continuously (24 h d−1) for the duration of the experiment. The amplitude of the playback of the recorded cues was close to that of the cue termites, as measured by an accelerometer. We expected that the amplitude of the cue would give information primarily about distance of the receiving termite from the cue source, because of the attenuation of the signal amplitude of vibrations due to damping in the substrate. The acceleration frequency response of the vibration generator was relatively flat over the frequency range of interest (100 Hz to 10 kHz). The materials of the central chamber transmitted a negligible signal from the signal block side (more than 30 dB reduction in the acceleration from the block receiving the signal to the other block).

We ran the experiment for two weeks in a constant-environment room (29°C and 80% RH). Tunnelling data were analysed using Systat 9 (SPSS). We used only replicates with surviving termites in the analysis; these included for each treatment (20, 160 and 400 mm blocks, respectively): live Cr. secundus (15, 20, 17), recorded vibration cues of Cr. secundus (10, 24, 18), live Co. acinaciformis (15, 23, 20), recorded vibration cues of Co. acinaciformis (12, 21, 30) and no-cue control (12, 34, 17). Two-way and one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni-corrected post hoc comparisons were used to compare distance and proportion distance tunnelled. We determined preference by calculating 95 per cent confidence intervals of proportion distance tunnelled in the block receiving the cue; we considered those not overlapping 0.5 to have made a choice.

(c) Recorded cues

The recorded cue treatments were designed to control for any confounding effect of species and number of individual cue termites used in the live termite treatments. We recorded termites from six Cr. secundus colonies, and three Co. acinaciformis colonies feeding on wooden blocks of Pinus radiata of 20, 160 and 400 mm length under identical conditions in an anechoic chamber to minimize environmental noise and reflected cues. The termites were held in the wooden blocks for 1–5 days, and we recorded cues throughout this time. We used a Brüel & Kjaer 4370 accelerometer attached to the opposite end of the wooden blocks to the termites and a Brüel & Kjaer 2635 charge amplifier, and recorded the cues directly to the hard drive of a computer using a soundcard.

We assembled six composite recorded cues, each 10 min in duration, one for each species–block size combination. The recorded cues were composite because they were made from shorter recordings from several groups from each of the colonies for each species. We did this to prevent confounding species identity and individual colony identity and also to ensure the same level of activity in each recorded cue for each species–block size combination. There were two obvious types of activity in the cues: (i) long, dull-sounding ‘scratches’ made when the termites dragged their mandibles across the wooden surface and (ii) short, sharp ‘snaps’ made when wooden fibres broke. The recorded Cr. secundus cues had 71.1 ± 1.8 scratches min−1 (mean ± s.e.) and 8.8 ± 1.0 snaps min−1, and the recorded Co. acinaciformis cues had 70.7 ± 1.7 scratches min−1 and 9.2 ± 0.5 snaps min−1. The quantity of each activity (log transformed to meet homogeneity of variance assumptions) of the composite recorded cues were not significantly different when compared with a two-factor ANOVA with species and block size as treatments: scratches (species F1,54 = 0.018, p = 0.894; block size F2,54 = 0.259, p = 0.773, interaction F2,54 = 0.050, p = 0.951); snaps (species F1,54 = 1.658, p = 0.203; block size F2,54 = 1.044, p = 0.359, interaction F2,54 = 0.013, p = 0.987).

3. Results

Test Cr. secundus termites tunnelled a similar total distance (i.e. tunnel length in both paired blocks added) between cue treatments (approx. 12 mm), but tunnelled significantly further in the longest blocks (approx. 10 mm versus 13 mm; cue treatment F4,275 = 0.188, p = 0.944; length treatment F2,275 = 6.621, p = 0.002; figure 3). This result showed that the termites contributed the same feeding effort between cue treatments, but more effort when more food was available, in agreement with previous results (Lenz 1994; Evans & Gleeson 2006).

Figure 3.

Total length (mean ± s.e.) of tunnels excavated by test Cr. secundus in paired wooden blocks of three sizes receiving four different cue treatments and a no-cue control. Black, 20 mm; broad hatch, 160 mm; narrow hatch, 400 mm long wooden blocks. ...

To control for the effect of wood block size, we converted data to proportion of tunnelling in the block receiving the cue for the live termite and recorded cue treatments, or in a random block for the controls. There was a significant interaction (F4,271 = 2.066, p = 0.039) between cue type and block length; therefore, each block size was analysed separately. For 20 mm long wooden blocks, cue treatments were significantly different (F4,59 = 14.755, p < 0.001), with live Cr. secundus not significantly different from recorded Cr. secundus (p > 0.05) and live Co. acinaciformis not significantly different from recorded Co. acinaciformis (p > 0.05), but with both Cr. secundus treatments significantly different from both Co. acinaciformis treatments (p < 0.05; all post hoc comparisons Bonferroni corrected). The same situation was observed for 160 mm long wooden blocks: treatments were significantly different (F4,115 = 5.590, p < 0.001), with live Cr. secundus not significantly different from recorded Cr. secundus (p > 0.05) and live Co. acinaciformis not significantly different from recorded Co. acinaciformis (p > 0.05), but with both Cr. secundus treatments significantly different from both Co. acinaciformis treatments (p < 0.05; all post hoc comparisons Bonferroni corrected). The situation differed for 400 mm long wooden blocks: no treatments were significantly different (F4,97 = 0.283, p = 0.889; figure 4).

Figure 4.

Proportion (mean ± s.e.) of total tunnel length excavated by test Cr. secundus in paired wooden blocks receiving different cue treatments (random block for controls). (a) 20 mm wooden blocks; (b) 160 mm wooden blocks; (c) 400 mm wooden blocks. ...

The preference of the test Cr. secundus was confirmed by calculating 95 per cent confidence intervals on the proportion of tunnelling in blocks receiving the cue. When the 95 per cent confidence intervals did not overlap 0.5, the test termites were determined to have a preference. Cryptotermes secundus preferred 20 and 160 mm blocks with live Cr. secundus and recorded Cr. secundus cues; they avoided 20 mm and 160 blocks with live Co. acinaciformis and recorded Co. acinaciformis cues (figure 5). Test Cr. secundus had no preference in all control block pairs and all 400 mm blocks.

Figure 5.

95 per cent Confidence intervals of the proportion of the total tunnel length excavated by test Cr. secundus in the block receiving different signal treatments. A preference was observed when the 95 per cent CI did not include 0.5 (dashed line); attraction ...

4. Discussion

These results show clearly that termites were using vibration cues to distinguish their own species from others, at least at shorter distances. It was unlikely that the test Cr. secundus could have detected the cue termites by other means, e.g. chemically, because they were completely contained and distant from the test termites. However, this possibility could not be ruled out; yet the recorded cues of both species produced the same pattern as the live termites, thus confirming that the test Cr. secundus termites were responding to the vibration cues alone. The recorded cues produced a greater response than the live termites, perhaps because the vibration cues were played continuously, whereas live termites may have chewed on the wooden blocks less constantly. There are two lines of support for this suggestion. First, the recorded cues were composite, with shorter recordings of higher activity cut from longer recordings, and so were likely to have higher activity overall than the live cue termites. Second, the related species Coptotermes lacteus varied the numbers of foraging termites found in feeding stations over the 24 h day (Evans & Gleeson 2001), indicating that the vibration cues generated by chewing varied with foraging activity over the day.

The ability to detect vibrations made by feeding and perceive the species feeding could be considered a form of eavesdropping. This is distinct from eavesdropping in the usual sense of the term, when one prey species listen to the alarm calls of other prey species to detect the presence of predators (Seyfarth & Cheney 1990; Otter et al. 1999; Zuberbühler 2000; Templeton & Greene 2007), but it is analogous to the eavesdropping on foraging signals of competitors observed in bees. The sweat bee Halictus aerarius avoids flowers visited and marked by other conspecifics and heterospecific bee species. This increases their foraging efficiency as H. aerarius can avoid flowers emptied of nectar (Yokoi et al. 2007). Similarly, bumblebees and honeybees avoid the flowers visited by hoverflies (Reader et al. 2005). Perhaps the closest analogous behaviour is that of Melipona rufiventris, a stingless bee. This bee can detect and avoid the odour marking of a competitor stingless bee, Trigona spinipes, because T. spinipes attacks and usurps the food resources of M. rufiventris (Nieh et al. 2004). Thus, two ecologically subordinate species, the stingless bee M. rufiventris and the drywood termite Cr. secundus, can eavesdrop on and avoid their ecological dominants.

The ability to identify other termite species from vibration cues or other signals has not been identified before now. Vibration cues have several advantages: they are fast, operate over distance and do not require direct contact—a particularly useful advantage for cryptic animals—although operation over distance allows eavesdropping. The decrease in response with the increase in wooden block size may suggest that the termites are capable of evaluating risk (i.e. by avoiding competitors only when the threat is imminent). Alternatively, it is possible that the termites' ability to perceive the cue diminishes as the block size increases. The benefit in avoiding Co. acinaciformis was clear, even more so when they were close (in fact, the Co. acinaciformis tunnelled through the 20 mm block in two replicates and killed all the test Cr. secundus; these were not used in the analysis). However, the benefit in having attraction to conspecifics is less clear. One possibility may be the opportunity for outbreeding. Should Cr. secundus colonies be orphaned (i.e. the mother queen or father king, or both, die), the workers can mature into neotenic replacement reproductives. Without meeting another colony, newly reproductive brothers and sisters will be forced to mate. This seems plausible as Cr. secundus colonies accept unrelated individuals peacefully and genetic data suggests that around 25 per cent of field Cr. secundus colonies are from two or more colonies merging (Korb & Schneider 2007).

Perhaps it is not surprising that termites are using vibration cues and signals, because they are small, herbivorous insects, which, due to their size and physiological constraints, are well adapted to use substrate-borne vibration signals (Bennet-Clark 1998; Cocroft & Rodríguez 2005). It is not clear whether subterranean termites have this ability. Perhaps they do, but they have lower selective pressure to avoid competitors, due to their greater competitive ability and foraging in more than one wooden resource. Evidence to support this suggestion could come from similar experimental studies comparing species of subterranean termites that are relatively stronger and weaker competitors. Alternatively, it is possible that subterranean termites may be less attuned to vibration cues and signals than drywood termites because of the medium in which they live and nest. Soil has a much larger coefficient of attenuation than wood (Liu & Nagel 1993), so the detection range of acoustic emissions in soil can be low, as little as 5 cm (Mankin et al. 2000). If so, then drywood termites can exploit the same food resources as subterranean termites by being even more cryptic than subterranean species. While it has been shown here that drywood termites distinguish their own species from others using vibration cues through wood, exactly which features of these cues and signals the termites use to extract the information and how they process such information are yet to be determined; such information has yet to be determined even for birds (Templeton & Greene 2007; Magrath et al. 2009). More detailed study on the features of the vibration cues is required, as well as comparative studies with other subterranean species to determine how widespread this ability is in the Isoptera.

Acknowledgements

We thank A. Barrett, P. Gleeson and W. Whitby for their help with termite collection and bioassays. This research was partly supported under the Australian Research Council's Discovery Projects funding scheme (project number DP0449825) and Linkage Projects funding scheme (project number LP0669638).

References

  • Albrecht M., Gotelli N. J. 2001. Spatial and temporal niche partitioning in grassland ants. Oecologica 126, 134–141 (doi:10.1007/s004420000494)
  • Andersen A. N. 1992. Regulation of ‘momentary’ diversity by dominant species in exceptionally rich ant communities of the Australian seasonal tropics. Am. Nat. 140, 401–420 (doi:10.1086/285419) [PubMed]
  • Andersen A. N. 1997. Functional groups and patterns of organization in North American ant communities: a comparison with Australia. J. Biogeogr. 24, 433–460 (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2699.1997.00137.x)
  • Bennet-Clark H. C. 1998. Size and scale effects as constraints in insect sound communication. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 353, 407–419 (doi:10.1098/rstb.1998.0219)
  • Bestelmeyer B. T. 2000. The trade-off between thermal tolerance and behavioural dominance in a subtropical South American ant community. J. Anim. Ecol. 69, 998–1009 (doi:10.1046/j.1365-2656.2000.00455.x)
  • Cocroft R. B., Rodríguez R. L. 2005. The behavioural ecology of insect vibrational communication. Bioscience 55, 323–334 (doi:10.1641/0006-3568)
  • Evans T. A., Gleeson P. V. 2001. Seasonal and daily activity patterns of subterranean, wood-eating termite foragers. Aust. J. Zool. 49, 311–321 (doi:10.1071/ZO00083)
  • Evans T. A., Gleeson P. V. 2006. The effect of bait size, composition, presentation and inspection frequency on bait consumption in termites (Isoptera: Rhinotermitidae). Bull. Entomol. Res. 96, 85–90 (doi:10.1079/BER2005397) [PubMed]
  • Evans T. A., Lenz M., Gleeson P. V. 1998. Testing assumptions of mark-recapture protocols for estimating population size using Australian, mound-building, subterranean termites (Isoptera: Rhinotermitidae and Termitidae). Ecol. Entomol. 23, 139–159 (doi:10.1046/j.1365-2311.1998.00114.x)
  • Evans T. A., Lenz M., Gleeson P. V. 1999. Estimating population size and forager movement in a tropical subterranean termite (Isoptera: Rhinotermitidae). Environ. Entomol. 28, 823–830
  • Evans T. A., Lai J. C. S., Toledano E., McDowall L., Rakotonarivo S., Lenz M. 2005. Termites assess wood size by using vibration signals. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 3732–3737 (doi:10.1073/pnas.0408649102) [PubMed]
  • Evans T. A., Inta R., Lai J. C. S., Lenz M. 2007. Foraging vibration signals attract foragers and identify food size in the drywood termite, Cryptotermes secundus. Insectes Soc. 54, 374–382 (doi:10.1007/s00040-007-0958-1)
  • Fellers J. H. 1987. Interference and exploitation in a guild of woodland ants. Ecology 68, 1466–1478 (doi:10.2307/1939230)
  • Gay F. J., Watson J. A. L. 1982. The genus Cryptotermes in Australia (Isoptera: Kalotermitidae). Aust. J. Zool. Suppl. Series 88, 1–64
  • Gordon D. M., Kulig A. W. 1996. Founding, foraging, and fighting: colony size and the spatial distribution of harvester ant nests. Ecology 77, 2393–2409 (doi:10.2307/2265741)
  • Greaves T. 1959. Termites as forest pests. Australas. Forestry 23, 114–120
  • Hardin G. 1960. The competitive exclusion principle. Science 131, 1292–1298 (doi:10.1126/science.131.3409.1292) [PubMed]
  • Hill G. F. 1942. In Termites (Isoptera) of the Australian Region. Melbourne, Australia: CSIR Publishing
  • Hölldobler B., Wilson E. O. 1990. The Ants. Heidelberg-Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag
  • Howse P. E. 1964. The significance of the sound produced by the termite Zootermopsis angusticollis (Hagen). Anim. Behav. 12, 284–301 (doi:10.1016/0003-3472(64)90015-6)
  • Inta R., Lai J. C. S., Fu E. W., Evans T. A. 2007. Termites live in a material world: exploration of their ability to differentiate between food sources. J. R. Soc. Lond. Interf. 4, 735–744 (doi:0022-0493/09/0121-0126) [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Inta R., Evans T. A., Lai J. C. S. 2009. Effect of vibratory soldier alarm signals on the foraging behavior of subterranean termites (Isoptera: Rhinotermitidae). J. Econ. Entomol. 102, 121–126 (doi:10.1603/029.102.0117) [PubMed]
  • Korb J., Katrantzis S. 2004. Influence of environmental conditions on the expression of the sexual dispersal phenotype in a lower termite: implications for the evolution of workers in termites. Evol. Dev. 6, 342–352 (doi:10.1111/j.1525-142X.2004.04042.x) [PubMed]
  • Korb J., Schmidinger S. 2004. Help or disperse? Cooperation in termites influenced by food conditions. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 56, 89–95 (doi:10.1007/s00265-004-0757-x)
  • Korb J., Schneider K. 2007. Does kin structure explain the occurrence of workers in a lower termite? Evol. Ecol. 21, 817–828 (doi:10.1007/s10682-006-9153-5)
  • Lenz M. 1994. Food resources, colony growth and caste development in wood-feeding termites. In Nourishment and evolution in insect societies (eds Hunt J. H., Nalepa C. A., editors. ), pp. 159–209 Boulder, CO: Westview Press
  • Leponce M., Roisin Y., Pasteels J. M. 1997. Structure and dynamics of the arboreal termite community in New Guinean coconut plantations. Biotropica 29, 193–203 (doi:10.1111/j.1744-7429.1997.tb00024.x)
  • Levings S. C., Adams E. S. 1984. Intra- and inter-specific territoriality in Nasutitermes (Isoptera: Termitidae) in a Panamanian mangrove forest. J. Anim. Ecol. 53, 705–714
  • Liu C.-H., Nagel S. T. 1993. Sound in a granular material: disorder and nonlinearity. Phys. Rev. B 48, 15 646–15 650 (doi:10.1103/PhysRevB.48.15646) [PubMed]
  • Magrath R. D., Pitcher B. J., Gardner J. L. 2009. Recognition of other species’ aerial alarm calls: speaking the same language or learning another? Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 769–774 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1368) [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Mankin R. W., Brandhorst-Hubbard J., Flanders K. L., Zhang M., Crocker R. L., Lapointe S. L., McCoy C. W., Fisher J. R., Weaver D. K. 2000. Eavesdropping on insects hidden in soil and interior structures of plants. J. Econ. Entomol. 93, 1173–1182 (doi:10.1603/0022-0493) [PubMed]
  • Nel J. J. C. 1968. Aggressive behaviour of the harvester termites Hodotermes mossambicus (Hagen) and Trinervitermes trinervoides (Sjostedt). Insectes Soc. 15, 145–156 (doi:10.1007/BF02223463)
  • Nieh J. C., Barreto L. S., Contrera F. A. L., Imperatriz-Fonseca V. L. 2004. Olfactory eavesdropping by a competitively foraging stingless bee, Trigona spinipes. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 271, 1633–1640 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2717) [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Otter K., McGregor P. K., Terry A. M. R., Burford F. R. L., Peake T. M., Dabelsteen T. 1999. Do female great tits (Parus major) assess males by eavesdropping? A field study using interactive song playback. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 266, 1305–1309 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0779)
  • Reader T., MacLeod I., Elliott P. T., Robinson O. J., Manica A. 2005. Inter-order interactions between flower-visiting insects: foraging bees avoid flowers previously visited by hoverflies. J. Insect Behav. 18, 51–57 (doi:10.1007/s10905-005-9346-8)
  • Röhrig A., Kirchner W. H., Leuthold R. H. 1999. Vibrational alarm communication in the African fungus-growing termite genus Macrotermes Isoptera, Termitidae. Insectes Soc. 46, 71–77
  • Roisin Y., Dejean A., Corbara B., Orivel J., Samaniego M., Leponce M. 2006. Vertical stratification of the termite assemblage in a neotropical rainforest. Oecologia 149, 301–311 (doi:10.1007/s00442-006-0449-5) [PubMed]
  • Schuurman G. 2006. Foraging and distribution patterns in a termite assemblage dominated by fungus-growing species in semi-arid northern Botswana. J. Trop. Ecol. 22, 277–287 (doi:10.1017/S0266467405003044)
  • Seyfarth R. M., Cheney D. L. 1990. The assessment by vervet monkeys of their own and another species’ alarm calls. Anim. Behav. 40, 754–764 (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80704-3)
  • Tarnopolsky A., Fletcher N., Hollenberg L., Lange B., Smith J., Wolfe J. 2005. Acoustics: the vocal tract and the sound of a didgeridoo. Nature 436, 39 (doi:10.1038/43639a) [PubMed]
  • Templeton C. N., Greene E. 2007. Nuthatches eavesdrop on variations in heterospecific chickadee mobbing alarm calls. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 5479–5482 (doi:10.1073/pnas.0605183104) [PubMed]
  • Thorne B. L., Haverty M. I. 1991. A review of intracolony, intraspecific, and interspecific agonism in termites. Sociobiology 19, 115–145
  • Werner P. A., Prior L. D., Forner J. 2008. Growth and survival of termite-piped Eucalyptus tetrodonta and E. miniata in northern Australia: implications for harvest of trees for didgeridoos. Forest Econ. Manage. 256, 328–334 (doi:10.1016/j.foreco.2008.04.027)
  • Wood T. G., Lee K. E. 1971. Abundance of mounts and competition among colonies of some Australian termite species. Pedobiologia 11, 341–356
  • Yokoi T., Goulson D., Fujisaki K. 2007. The use of heterospecific scent marks by the sweat bee Halictus aerarius. Naturwissenschaften 94, 1021–1024 (doi:10.1007/s00114-007-0285-4) [PubMed]
  • Zuberbühler K. 2000. Interspecies semantic communication in two forest primates. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 267, 713–718 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1061) [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Articles from Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society