PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptNIH Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
Int J Obes (Lond). Author manuscript; available in PMC Aug 1, 2010.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC2822132
NIHMSID: NIHMS151771
Mothers’ Negative Affectivity During Pregnancy and Food Choices for Their Infants
Sarah E. Hampson, Ph.D.,1,2 Serena Tonstad, Ph.D.,2,3 Lorentz M. Irgens, M.D., Ph.D.,4 Helle Margrete Meltzer, Ph.D.,5 and Margarete Vollrath, Ph.D.6,7
1 Oregon Research Institute, 1715 Franklin Blvd, Eugene, OR 97403, USA
2 Department of Nutrition, University of Oslo, Blindern, Oslo, Norway
3 Oslo University Hospital, Ullevål Department of Preventive Cardiology, Oslo, Norway
4 Medical Birth Registry of Norway, Locus for Registry Based Epidemiology, Department of Public Health and Primary Health Care, University of Bergen, Kalfarveien 31, NO 5031 Bergen Norway
5 Department of Food Safety and Nutrition, Division of Environmental Medicine, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, PBox 4404, Nydalen, 0403 Oslo, Norway
6 Department of Psychosomatics and Health Behavior, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, POB 4404 Nydalen, 0403 Oslo, Norway
7 Psychological Institute, University of Oslo, POB 1094 Blindern, 0317 Oslo, Norway
Corresponding Author: Sarah E. Hampson, PhD, Oregon Research Institute, 1715 Franklin Blvd, Eugene, OR 97403 USA, Phone: 541 484 2123, Fax: 541 484 1108, sarah/at/ori.org
Objective
To investigate whether maternal negative affectivity assessed in pregnancy is related to subsequent infant food choices.
Design
Cohort study.
Subjects
Mothers (N = 37, 919) and their infants participating in the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study conducted by the Norwegian Institute of Public Health.
Measurements
Maternal negative affectivity assessed pre-partum (SCL-5 at week 17 and 30 of pregnancy), introduction of solid foods by month 3, and feeding of sweet drinks by month 6 (by mothers’ reports).
Results
Mothers with higher negative affectivity were 64% more likely (95% CI 1.5–1.8) to feed sweet drinks by month 6, and 79% more likely (95% CI 1.6–2.0) to introduce solid foods by month 3. These odds decreased to 41% and 30%, respectively, after adjusting for mother’s age, body mass index, and education.
Conclusion
The maternal trait of negative affectivity is an independent predictor of infant feeding practices that may be related to childhood weight gain, overweight, and obesity.
Keywords: maternal feeding practices, negative affectivity, solid foods, sweet drinks
It is well-established that rates of childhood overweight and obesity are increasing in developed countries. 1,2. One of the many determinants of this increase may be mothers’ food choices for their infants. Personality traits influence individuals’ lifestyle behaviors, including diet, that contribute to their own obesity3,4 but little is known about the influence of personality traits on dietary choices made for others.
The personality trait of neuroticism is strongly correlated with anxiety and depression5, which are two of the main constituents of negative affect6. Although neuroticism is normally distributed in the population and is not an indicator of psychopathology, individuals with higher levels of neuroticism experience life as more threatening, problematic and distressing than more stable individuals7. They also tend to experience more pain and report more physical symptoms than those who are less neurotic8,9, and they are vulnerable to stress and prone to give up on goals10,11.
Previous studies of maternal personality and infant feeding have been limited by small samples and lack of prospective designs12,13,14. However, recent reports from the Norwegian Mothers and Child Cohort Study15 indicated that mothers with higher levels of negative affectivity were more likely to have stopped exclusively breastfeeding at 6 months16, and were more likely to be feeding their child an unhealthy diet at 18 months17.
Dietary choices made on behalf of infants have been identified as risk factors for childhood obesity including feeding sweet beverages and the early introduction of solid food18. Except for breastfeeding, recent evidence-based guidelines for infant feeding are lacking19. However, exclusive breastfeeding for the first 6 months is widely recommended20,21. The early introduction of solid foods has been related to infant weight gain in several recent studies22,23,24. Consumption of sweet beverages, independent of solid food consumed, is related to weight gain in rats25, but evidence for a causal relation between sweet drinks and weight gain in humans is more controversial26,27.
We hypothesized that, due to their lower threshold for stress, mothers with higher levels of negative affectivity would be more likely to use solid food and sweet drinks earlier than recommended to soothe their infants. This hypothesis was tested in a large, prospective population study of mothers and their infants. The present study extends past research by examining for the first time the association between maternal negative affectivity and. feeding practices for young infants during the first 6 months that may contribute to obesity.
Study design and participants
The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study is an ongoing longitudinal investigation of health determinants in a cohort comprising more than 100,000 pregnancies conducted by the Norwegian Institute of Public Health (www.fhi.no/morogbarn). The cohort was recruited from all except two of the maternity units in Norway with more than 100 births annually (n=50). The women received a postal invitation to join the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort together with their appointment cards for routine ultrasound scans in week 17 to 18 of gestation. The participation rate was 42.7%15. The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study releases updated versions of the data files once a year. The analyses reported here are based on the quality-controlled data files released in Spring 2007 (version 3). We included data from questionnaires at gestation weeks 17 and 30, and 6 months postpartum. Participation rates among those who gave informed consent initially were 95% at 17 weeks, 92% at 30 weeks, and 87% at 6 months postpartum. We also retrieved data from the Medical Birth Registry of Norway, which registers data on all births in Norway, including parental marital status, maternal health before and during pregnancy as well as birth and infant-related variables28.
For the present study, we selected mothers (N = 43,288) of singletons who completed questionnaires at gestation weeks 17 and 30, and at 6 months postpartum. We excluded all mothers where information was missing for maternal age, the child’s sex and birth weight, the age at which solid foods were introduced, and the frequency of feeding the child sweet drinks (N = 5,369). This resulted in a sample size of N = 37,919, representing 87.6 % of the entire dataset. We certify that all applicable institutional and governmental regulations concerning the ethical use of human volunteers were followed during this research.
Independent variables
Maternal negative affectivity, conceptualized as the combination of anxiety and depression, was measured in pregnancy using a 5-item version of the Hopkins Symptom Checklist (SCL-5) administered at weeks 17 and 30 of gestation. It was also measured at 6 months post-partum. The SCL-5 is a short form of the SCL-25, which measures only the anxiety and depression dimensions of the full checklist29. Anxiety and depression are major components of negative affect, which is part of the neuroticism construct5,6. The SCL-5 was developed on a large Norwegian community sample by factor analyzing the SCL-25 and selecting two high-loading items for anxiety and three for depression. The correlation between the SCL-5 and the SCL-25 was .92 30. The items were “Feeling fearful,” “Nervousness or shakiness inside,” “Feeling hopeless about the future,” “Feeling blue,” and “Worrying too much about things,” and were rated on a 4-point scale from not bothered (1) to very bothered (4). Scores were averaged across the two assessments in pregnancy (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.86) and correlated .56 with the assessment at 6 months post-partum, indicating that mothers’ negative affectivity was relatively stable across time.
Dependent Variables
At 6 months postpartum, mothers reported liquids and solid foods that they had fed to their child since birth. Four questions queried the frequency with which they fed their child sweetened drinks (“saft” i.e., berry or fruit-juice concentrate, with added sugar, that is mixed with water), other types of sweetened cordial, and fruit juice. The frequency of consumption was categorized as (1) never/seldom, (2) 1–3 times a week, (3) 4–6 times a week, and (4) at least once a day. From these four questions we constructed a dichotomous variable indicating whether the child was fed any of these sweet drinks at least 1–3 times a week.
From a list including 17 solid food items, including porridges and purees, mothers reported how often they fed each item to their child, and at what age it was fed to the child for the first time. We constructed a dichotomous variable indicating whether the child had been fed any of these foods at age 3 months or earlier.
Potential Confounders
Data on maternal age at delivery were retrieved from the Medical Birth Registry of Norway. The mothers’ body mass index (BMI) in kg/cm2 before pregnancy was calculated from self-reported data at the assessment at gestation week 17.
Mothers indicated their highest level of completed education at gestation week 17 using one of six categories covering educational levels from 9 years (secondary school) to 18+ years (university). Each unit of this variable, which was treated as a continuous variable coded from 1 to 6, corresponds to an increase of 2–3 years of education. This variable was reverse coded so that higher scores indicated less education.
Statistical Analysis
There were incorrect or missing values in 1–2% the cases for each confounding variable variable. Obviously incorrect values (such as a body weight of 570 kg) were replaced by missing values. Because listwise deletion of missing values would have reduced the sample size to around 28 000 cases, missing values were substituted by means of maximum likelihood imputation procedures31 using information from correlated variables in the expectation-maximization algorithm. Missing educational level was estimated by means of the mothers’ self-reported income and their reports on the fathers’ educational level. Missing values regarding maternal weight before pregnancy were estimated from mothers’ self-reported height and weight 6 months after pregnancy. Logistic regression modeling32 was used to calculate the effects of mothers’ negative affectivity and potential confounders on feeding their children sweet drinks at age 6 months and introducing solid foods at age 3 months.
Mothers’ mean age was 30 years and their mean pre-pregnancy BMIs were in the normal range (20–25). Their mean educational level corresponded to 14 years of education (see Table 1). At the categorical level, more than 50% of the mothers had a college or bachelor education (data not shown). Around one fifth of the mothers fed their child a sweet drink at least 1–3 times weekly at 6 months, and 7% had introduced solid foods to the child at 3 months. Compared to mothers excluded from the analyses because of missing data on key variables including the outcomes (N = 5,369), the 37,919 included mothers were slightly younger and heavier, but did not differ with respect to negative affectivity or educational level (see Table 1).
Table 1
Table 1
Characteristics (Means and SDs or Percentages) of the mothers included in the analyses (N=37, 919) compared to mothers excluded because of missing values on key variables (N=5,369)
The unadjusted and adjusted odds ratios for the association of negative affectivity with infant feeding practices are displayed in Table 2. The unadjusted odds ratios showed that mothers scoring higher in negative affectivity had 64% (sweet drinks) and 79% (solid foods) increased odds of using these feeding practices. These odds decreased to 41% and 30%, respectively, after adjusting for age, higher BMI before pregnancy, and lower levels of education, all of which increased the odds for these feeding practices. Lower educational level (r =0.14) and younger maternal age (r = 0.08) were associated with negative affectivity (p ≤ 0.05).
Table 2
Table 2
Associations of negative affectivity and confounding variables with feeding practices in mothers of 6 month- old infants
Previous studies of the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort showed that mothers with higher levels of negative affectivity were more likely to use potentially obesogenic feeding practices when their children were 6 months or older16,17. The present study demonstrated similar findings for children under age 6 months. One explanation for consistent effects of higher negative affectivity on infant feeding practices is that these mothers are more susceptible to stress and less able to tolerate signs of distress in their infants and hence may use solid foods and sweet drinks to soothe their infants. Fussy (difficult temperament) children are less likely to be predominantly breastfed for 6 months33. Mothers with higher negative affectivity may introduce solid foods earlier and give sweet drinks because they interpret apparent infant distress as signs of hunger.
Negative affectivity remained a significant predictor after controlling for other variables. Younger mothers, mothers with higher BMI, and those with lower educational achievement were more likely to make potentially obesogenic food choices for their infants. Younger mothers’ relative lack of child-rearing experience may have contributed to their early use of sweet drinks and solid foods. In this study as in others, negative affectivity was related to lower educational achievement34,35,36. Lower educational achievement may result in less attention to health information and less ability to respond to it. Breastfeeding is another variable that may influence food choices. When mothers stop exclusive breastfeeding during the first 6 months, they will not necessarily choose to feed sweet drinks or introduce solid foods. As additional analyses showed (not reported here), controlling for breastfeeding cessation at 3 months for solid foods and at 6 months for sweet drinks, resulted in only marginal reductions in the odds ratios for negative affect. Together, these findings indicate that negative affectivity, which is associated with obesognenic choices and effects for oneself, may also influence food choices for others.
Limitations
The measure of negative affectivity used here only assessed anxiety and depression and so was not a complete measure of neuroticism, nor were other maternal personality traits assessed. The feeding practices we studied may not be related to subsequent obesity. However, the use of solid foods and the use of sweet drinks prior to 6 months contradicts the recommendation to breastfeed exclusively for the first six months20,21. Sweet drinks may contribute to weight gain because they are less satiating so dietary compensation is weaker than for solid food forms of comparable nutrient content. In a descriptive study conducted in the US, beverages made up 84% of the daily food energy for infants aged 4 – 6 months, demonstrating the potential for excess calories if mothers give sweet drinks to their infants37. Studies of the effects of giving sweet beverages to infants under six months are lacking, although some studies have shown an association between consumption of fruit juice or sweetened beverages and obesity among older infants and children38,39. As yet, we do not know whether these feeding practices are associated with subsequent overweight in this sample, but we will be able to examine this in future studies. It should be noted that none of the children in our study attended external day care, which is associated with less breastfeeding and early introduction of solid food24, because in Norway parents can have a full year of paid maternity/paternity leave when a child is born.
Conclusions
These findings indicated that maternal negative affectivity is a risk factor for potentially obesogenic infant feeding practices. Maternal distress should be recognizable by health personnel and maternal depression and anxiety could be assessed by a brief self report measure such as the SCL-5 used here. Distressed mothers will be anxious, nervous and helpless, and express undue concern about their child’s health, growth and weight gain, and sleeping pattern. Health personnel should actively seek information from these mothers on their potential barriers to adherence to infant feeding recommendations, and invest more time in counseling and educating them. The benefits in terms of reduced childhood obesity could be considerable.
Acknowledgments
The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study is supported by the Norwegian Ministry of Health, NIH/NIEHS (grant no. N01-ES-85433), NIH/NINDS (grant no.1 UO1 NS 047537-01), and the Norwegian Research Council/FUGE (grant no. 151918/S10). Sarah E. Hampson’s contribution to the preparation of this article was partially supported by a grant number AG20048 from the National Institute on Aging, USA.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
1. Ogden CL, Carroll MD, McDowell MA, Flegal KM. High body mass index for age among U.S. children and adolescents, 2003–2006. JAMA. 2008;299(20):2401–2405. [PubMed]
2. Juliusson PB, Roelants M, Eide GE, Hauspie R, Waaler PE, Bjerknes R. Overweight and obesity in Norwegian children: Secular trends in weight-for-height and skinfolds. Acta Paediatr. 2007;6(9):1333–1337. [PubMed]
3. Hampson SE, Goldberg LR, Vogt TM, Dubanoski JP. Forty Years on: Teachers’ assessments of children’s personality traits predict self-reported health behaviors and outcomes at midlife. Health Psychol. 2006;25:57–64. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
4. Sullivan S, Cloninger CR, Przybeck TR, Klein S. Personality characteristics in obesity and relationship with successful weight loss. Int J Obesity. 2007;31:669–674. [PubMed]
5. Watson D, Walker LM. The long-term stability and predictive validity of trait measures of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996;70:567–577. [PubMed]
6. Watson D, Clark LA. Negative affectivity: The disposition to experience aversive emotional states. Psychol Bull. 1984;96:465–90. [PubMed]
7. Clark LA, Watson D. Temperament: An organizing paradigm for trait psychology. In: John OP, Robins RW, Pervin LA, editors. Handbook of Personality Theory and Research. 3. The Guilford Press; New York: 2008. pp. 265–286.
8. Costa PT, McCrae RR. Neuroticism, somatic complaints, and disease: Is the bark worse than the bite? J Pers. 1987;55:299–316. [PubMed]
9. Watson DJ, Pennebaker JW. Health complaints, stress, and distress: Exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psychol Rev. 1989;96:234–254. [PubMed]
10. Vollrath M, Torgersen S, Alnas R. Neuroticism, coping and change in MCMI-II clinical syndromes: Test of a mediator model. Scand J Psychol. 1998;39:15–24. [PubMed]
11. Vollrath M. Personality and stress. Scand J Psychol. 2001;42:335–47. [PubMed]
12. Cooper PJ, Murray L, Stein A. Psychosocial factors associated with early termination of breast feeding. J Psychosom Res. 1993;37:171–6. [PubMed]
13. Wagner CL, Wagner MT, Ebeling M, Chatman KG, Cohen M, Hulsey TC. The role of personality and other factors in a mother’s decision to initiate breastfeeding. J Hum Lact. 2006;22:16–26. [PubMed]
14. Wagner CL, Wagner MT. The breast or the bottle? Determinants of infant feeding behaviors. Clin Perinatal. 1999;26:505–25. [PubMed]
15. Magnus P, Irgens LM, Haug K, Nystad W, Skjærven R, Stoltenberg C. the MoBa study group. Cohort profile: The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study. Int J Epidemiol. 2006;35:1146–50. [PubMed]
16. Ystrom E, Niegel S, Knut-Inge K, Vollrath ME. The impact of maternal negative affectivity and general self-efficacy on breastfeeding: The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort study. J Pediatr. 2008;152:68–78. [PubMed]
17. Ystrom E, Niegel S, Vollrath ME. The impact of maternal negative affectivity on dietary patterns of 18-month-old children in the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort study. Matern Child Nutr. 2009 [PubMed]
18. Kumanyika SK. Environmental influences on childhood obesity: Ethnic and cultural influences in context. Physiol Behav. 2008;94:61–20. [PubMed]
19. Glinsmann WH, Bartholmey SJ, Coletta F. Dietary guidelines for infants: a timely reminder. Nutr Rev. 1996;54(2):50–57. [PubMed]
20. Kramer MS, Kakuma R. The Optimal Duration of Exclusive Breastfeeding. A Systematic Review. World Health Organization; Geneva: 2002.
21. [accessed April 24, 2008.]. http://www.unicef.org/nutrition/index_24811.html.
22. Grummer-Strawn LM, Scanlon KS, Fein SB. Infant feeding and feeding transitions during the first year of life. Pediatrics. 2008;22:S36–S42. [PubMed]
23. Jingxiong J, Rosenqvist U, Huishan W, Koletzko B, Guangli L, Jing H, Greiner T. Relationship of parental characteristics and feeding practices to overweight in infants and young children in Beijing, China. Public Health Nutr. 2009;12:973–979. [PubMed]
24. Kim J, Peterson KE. Association of infant child care with infant feeding practices and weight gain among US infants. Arch Pediat Adol Med. 2008;162(7):627–633. [PubMed]
25. Archer ZA, Corneloup J, Rayner DV, Barrett P, Moar KM, Mercer JG. Solid and liquid obesogenic diets induce obesity and counter-regulatory changes in hypothalamic gene expression in juvenile Sprague-Dawley rats. J Nutr. 2007;137:1483–90. [PubMed]
26. Mourao DM, Bressan J, Campbell WW, Mattes RD. Effects of food form on appetite and energy intake in lean and obese young adults. Int J Obesity. 2007;31:1688–95. [PubMed]
27. Pereira MA. The possible role of sugar-sweetened beverages in obesity etiology: A review of the evidence. Int J Obesity. 2006;30:S28–S36.
28. Irgens LM. The Medical Birth Registry of Norway. Epidemiological research and surveillance throughout 30 years. Acta Obstet et Gyn Scan. 2000;79(6):435–9. [PubMed]
29. Strand BH, Dalgard OS, Tambs K, Rognerud M. Measuring the mental health status of the Norwegian population: a comparison of the instruments SCL-25, SCL-10, SCL-5 and MHI-5 (SF-36) Nord J Psychiat. 2003;57:113–118. [PubMed]
30. Tambs K, Moum T. How well can a few questionnaire items indicate anxiety and depression? Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1993;87:364–367. [PubMed]
31. Schafer JL, Graham JW. Missing data: our view of the state of the art. Psychol Methods. 2002;7(2):147–177. [PubMed]
32. SPSS Inc. SPSS 16 for Windows. 2008.
33. Niegel S, Ystrom E, Hagtvet KA, Vollrath ME. Difficult temperament, breastfeeding, and their mutual prospective effects: the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2008;29(6):458–62. [PubMed]
34. Chamorro-Premuzic T, Furnham A. Personality predicts academic performance: evidence from two longitudinal university samples. J Res Pers. 2003;37:319–338.
35. Furnham A, Mitchell J. Personality, needs, social skills and academic achievement: A longitudinal study. Pers Indiv Differ. 1991;12(10):1067–1073.
36. Komarraju M, Kauro SJ, Schmeck RR. Role of the Big Five personality traits in predicting college students’ academic motivation and achievement. Learn Individ Differ. 2009;19:47–52.
37. Skinner JD, Ziegler P, Ponza M. Transitions in infants’ and toddlers’ beverage patterns. J Am Diet Assoc. 2004;104(1):s45–50. [PubMed]
38. Warner ML, Harley K, Bradman A, Vargas G, Eskanazi B. Soda consumption and overweight status of 20-year-old Mexican American children in California. Obesity. 2006;14:1966–1974. [PubMed]
39. Welch KR, Ariza JA, Wieczorek JL, Binns HJ. Characteristics of obese children aged 1–4 years at a referral clinic. J Natl Med Asssoc. 2008;100:884–891. [PubMed]
40. Haberman SJ. The analysis of residuals in cross-classified tables. Biometrics. 1973;29:205–220.