PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptHHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 1.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC2806224
NIHMSID: NIHMS168634

Persistent cognitive dysfunction after traumatic brain injury: A dopamine hypothesis

James W. Bales,a,b,c,d,e Amy K. Wagner,a,b,c,f Anthony E. Kline,a,b,c,d,f,g and C. Edward Dixona,b,c,e,f,*

Abstract

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) represents a significant cause of death and disability in industrialized countries. Of particular importance to patients the chronic effect that TBI has on cognitive function. Therapeutic strategies have been difficult to evaluate because of the complexity of injuries and variety of patient presentations within a TBI population. However, pharmacotherapies targeting dopamine (DA) have consistently shown benefits in attention, behavioral outcome, executive function, and memory. Still it remains unclear what aspect of TBI pathology is targeted by DA therapies and what time-course of treatment is most beneficial for patient outcomes. Fortunately, ongoing research in animal models has begun to elucidate the pathophysiology of DA alterations after TBI. The purpose of this review is to discuss clinical and experimental research examining DAergic therapies after TBI, which will in turn elucidate the importance of DA for cognitive function/dysfunction after TBI as well as highlight the areas that require further study.

Keywords: Cognitive dysfunction, Dopamine, Neurotransmission, Recovery of function, Traumatic brain injury

1. Introduction

1.1. Traumatic brain injury

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is the leading cause of death and disability in individuals less than 45 years of age in industrialized countries (Bruns and Hauser, 2003). Each year an estimated 1.4 million Americans experience a TBI and 80,000 to 90,000 suffer long-term substantial loss of function (Rutland-Brown et al., 2006). Clinical studies have shown that 10–15% of individuals with mild TBI have persistent cognitive and behavioral complaints. Outcomes from moderate TBI are much less favorable with some estimates suggesting that 50% of these individuals endure long-term injury-related disabilities (Kraus et al., 2005). This places an enormous economic burden on the U.S. healthcare system with an estimated cost of $9–10 billion in acute care and rehabilitation annually. This cost is in addition to lost earnings, social services, and the cost to family members who must care for TBI survivors. TBI also represents a global healthcare crisis with an estimated 2% of the world's population suffering from chronic symptoms of brain trauma, equating to more than 120 million individuals (NIH, 1998; Ragnarsson, 2002). For these reasons it has been a long sought goal of TBI researchers to understand the mechanisms of chronic disability after TBI to help develop treatment strategies that may assist patients with cognitive recovery.

However, researching chronic disability following TBI has posed a unique challenge to both clinical and experimental researchers. TBI is a highly variable and extremely complex phenomenon. Following the acute primary injury, which often consists of a focal contusion and more diffuse structural damage, there are a series of subsequent secondary responses, which include, but are not limited to, excitotoxicity, ischemia, oxidative stress, and ongoing structural and chemical alterations (Kochanek, 1993; DeKosky et al., 1998; Park et al., 2008). Traditionally, research in recovery of function after TBI has focused on preventing or manipulating early events in order to prevent chronic dysfunction. Drugs inhibiting apoptosis, blocking glutamate-induced excitotoxicity, or attenuating oxidative stress were designed to reduce cell loss with the premise that neuronal sparing would enhance recovery (Faden et al., 1989; Jennings et al., 2008). Unfortunately, the neuroprotective effects observed in the TBI laboratories have not translated successfully to the clinic (Gualtieri, 1988; Tolias and Bullock, 2004). In contrast, therapeutics used during the rehabilitative phase have shown more promise in addressing long-term disability, although they do not necessarily demonstrate the same level of neuroprotection as drugs designed to inhibit apoptosis or block excitotoxicity (Gualtieri, 1988; Rees et al., 2007).

The failure of translating experimental preventative strategies to clinical efficacy has raised the question about what events in TBI are crucial for long-term outcome. The development of clinically relevant small animal models has greatly assisted the understanding of both acute and chronic TBI-induced alterations in brain chemistry. The two most widely used models of TBI are fluid percussion (FP; Dixon et al., 1987) and controlled cortical impact (CCI; Lighthall, 1988; Lighthall et al., 1989; Dixon et al., 1991; Kline et al., 2001; Kline and Dixon, 2001). Both models produce clinically relevant brain pathology as well as behavioral and cognitive dysfunction in rats and mice (Dixon et al., 1987, 1991; Hamm et al., 1992, 1996a,b; Fox et al., 1998, 1999; Kline et al., 2002, 2007a,b; Wagner et al., 2002, 2004; Cheng et al., 2007, 2008; Hoffman et al., 2008a,b). Animal studies (Bramlett and Dietrich, 2002; Lifshitz et al., 2007) and human positron emission tomography (PET) imaging (Langfitt et al., 1986; Fontaine et al., 1999; Donnemiller et al., 2000) have shown that in addition to overt damage (e.g., cortical lesions and hippocampal cell loss), there exist areas of chronic dysfunction previously unappreciated, particularly in the striatum and thalamus, which are regions known to have important roles in cognitive, motor, and emotional processing (Vertes, 2006).

The aims of this review are to: (1) highlight the role of dopamine (DA) in cognition and its functional anatomy relevant to TBI; (2) outline clinical research that has demonstrated potential efficacy of DAergic medications in the treatment of TBI; (3) provide an overview of observed changes in DA signaling and anatomy in experimental models, and outline the importance that these alterations have on cognitive and behavioral deficits; and (4) assess future areas of DA systems research in TBI. This review is not meant to be an exhaustive discussion of DA and cognition, but rather is intended to highlight the role(s) DA plays in persistent cognitive dysfunction after TBI, which may provide insight into potential mechanisms and therapeutic targets for chronic cognitive dys-function. For recent reviews on DA cellular function and DAergic mediated cognition, see Verheij and Cools (2008) and El-Ghundi et al. (2007), respectively.

1.2. Introduction to dopamine in TBI

DA represents a unique signaling system within the central nervous system (CNS) due to its role as both a neurotransmitter and neuromodulator. Furthermore, DA receptors are abundantly expressed in brain areas known to be damaged after TBI, such as the frontal cortex and striatum, which are important for cognitive function (Seeman et al., 1978; Baron et al., 1985; McDonald et al., 2002; Chudasama and Robbins, 2006). The hippocampus, which is also critical for cognitive function does not have a high level of DA receptor expression, but is dependent on DA activity to modulate function (Lemon and Manahan-Vaughan, 2006; O'Carroll et al., 2006; Granado et al., 2008).

2. Dopaminergic functional anatomy: a TBI perspective

2.1. Persistent cognitive disability

Cognitive disorders experienced by TBI patients can present immediately after the initial injury or evolve during the subsequent months to years. Regardless of presentation, many patients live with sustained alterations in cognition and behavior for the rest of their lives (Millis et al., 2001). Non-pharmacological options for TBI patients experiencing cognitive and behavioral dysfunction are limited, with cognitive training paradigms often being the only consistent treatment provided. However, cognitive training effectiveness has not been fully validated, and program implementation can be variable (Turner-Stokes et al., 2005).

Persistent cognitive deficits can be categorized into one of three general domains: attention and processing speed, memory, and executive function (Gronwall, 1976; Levin and Grossman, 1978; Gronwall and Wrightson, 1981; Stuss et al., 1985; Binder, 1986, 1987; McMillan and Glucksman, 1987; Levin et al., 1988a,b; Gentilini et al., 1989; Stuss et al., 1989; Leininger et al., 1990; Binder, 1997; Binder et al., 1997; McMillan, 1997). Of these, memory difficulties are the most commonly reported and most difficult for patients and caregivers (Binder, 1987). In addition to cognitive difficulties, TBI survivors often experience behavioral difficulties characterized by enhanced emotional lability and alterations in affect (Fugate et al., 1997). Importantly, a prior TBI has been shown to be a risk factor for developing psychiatric and psychotic disorders (Koponen et al., 2002). Due to its often diffuse nature and the variety of cognitive disturbances experienced post-TBI, it has been difficult to localize a single disruption in neural function that could explain such an array of events. Indeed, persistent deficits experienced by TBI patients are probably due to a wide spectrum of different neural system dysfunctions. However, that does not preclude the possibility of utilizing a targeted therapeutic strategy to enhance cognitive recovery after TBI. DA is a particularly important system to study in this context because it is known to have an important role in physiological events relevant to cognition and in numerous systems also affected by TBI, including the hippocampus, striatum, and frontal cortex.

2.2. Dopamine and TBI: the effect on cognition

Fig. 1 is a depiction of the rat CNS demonstrating an overlay of injury processes that occur with a TBI and their direct relationship to DAergic pathways important to cognition. As illustrated, TBI can have widespread effects on brain anatomy and function within DAergic regions. What is not depicted in Fig. 1 is the indirect effect of TBI on DA signaling, including disruptions in glutamatergic cortico-striatal projections and striatal gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA)ergic outputs.

Fig. 1
Depiction of TBI induced damage to DA systems in rat brain demonstrating widespread disruption in DA structures. Areas involved in both ascending and descending DAergic signaling pathways are damaged following a TBI. Not pictured is the extensive damage ...

Ascending DAergic pathways in the CNS can be divided into two predominant systems: (1) the nigrostriatal pathway [substantia nigra (SN) innervating striatum], and (2) the mesocorticolimbic pathway [ventral tegmental area (VTA) projecting to the prefrontal cortex (PFC), hippocampus, amygdala, and nucleus accumbens (NAcc)] (Alexander and Crutcher, 1990; Graybiel, 1990). Projections from the mesocorticolimbic pathway are believed to be involved in modulating memory consolidation (Ploeger et al., 1991, 1994; Cools et al., 1993; Setlow and McGaugh, 1998; Coccurello et al., 2000), motivation (Mitchell and Gratton, 1994; Salamone, 1994; Baldo and Kelley, 2007), and drug reinforcement and addiction (Carelli, 2002; Schultz, 2004; Salamone et al., 2005; Berridge, 2006; Di Chiara and Bassareo, 2007; Ikemoto, 2007; Sutton and Beninger, 1999). Change in the release of DA in the mesocorticolimbic system is also associated with neuropsychiatric disorders, arousal, stress, and addiction (Tidey and Miczek, 1996; Viggiano et al., 2003; Sonuga-Barke, 2005). The nigrostriatal system is predominantly associated with voluntary movement (Hornykiewicz, 1966; Seeman and Niznik, 1990; Jackson and Westlind-Danielsson, 1994), but it has also been shown to be important for behavioral events including reward processing (Wickens et al., 2007) and acquisition of spatial learning and memory (Mura and Feldon, 2003). Research investigating Parkinson's disease (PD) has also shown that dysfunctional nigrostriatal signaling has implications for other cognitive functions including memory, executive function, and attention (Tamaru, 1997; Ridley et al., 2006).

The striatum, which includes the NAcc and caudate putamen, exists as part of an anatomic network that subserves functions associated with the dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC), but also receives inputs from numerous other brain areas including the hippo-campus and limbic cortex. The DLPFC has dense projections to the head of the caudate and there exist reciprocal pathways back to the DLPFC through the thalamus (Middleton and Strick, 2000). Due to this complex relationship with surrounding cortical and subcortical structures through DAergic projections, the striatum is in a prime location for mediating human cognition. Studies have demonstrated that both the striatum and DLPFC are important for executive function and working memory (WM) (Crosson, 2003).

In Huntington's disease (HD), projections from the caudate to the frontal lobes are disrupted and result in significant motor, attentional, and executive dysfunction (Brandt et al., 1988; Zakzanis and Kaplan, 1999). Experiments producing lesions in the striatum suggest that the caudate, in particular, plays a specific role in cognition. Damage to the caudate produces deficits that resemble damage to corresponding projection targets of the PFC (Divac et al., 1967). In nonhuman primates, metabolic activity within the striatum has also been linked to specific changes in WM task performance (Levy et al., 1997). In a recent review by Grahn et al. (2009) the authors examine the role of the basal ganglia relevant to learning and memory concluding that the goal-directed behaviors subserved by basal ganglia function are crucial to all forms of normal behavior.

Human neuroimaging studies also support the role of the striatum in cognition. PET studies using 18F-dopa in individuals with PD show a correlation between DA depletion and neuropsychological performance (Broussolle et al., 1999; Marie et al., 1999; Bruck et al., 2001; Duchesne et al., 2002). PET imaging studies in HD using C11-Raclopride indicate that striatal DA receptor subtype 2 (D2) binding is decreased and is sensitive to cognitive performance on a variety of tasks including executive function, attention, and WM (Backman et al., 1997; Lawrence et al., 1998). While striatal dysfunction is associated with cognitive sequelae of HD and PD, DAergic system dysfunction within the PFC has been strongly tied to attentional and cognitive symptoms associated with schizophrenia and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Heilman et al., 1991; Tassin, 1992; Knable et al., 1997; Tanaka, 2006).

These clinical studies strongly suggest that the striatum and PFC are functionally important for a variety of cognitive behaviors, and that the alterations in DA signaling appear to be the underlying cause of cognitive dysfunction in a variety of disease states. In TBI, it is known that both the striatum and PFC are vulnerable to damage. The known effects of TBI on the striatum include axonal degeneration (Ding et al., 2001), neuronal cell loss (Dunn-Meynell and Levin, 1997), and ischemia (Dietrich et al., 1994). Effects on the PFC include decreased glucose metabolism (Fontaine et al., 1999), changes in frontal lobe blood flow during memory tasks (Ricker et al., 2001), and hypoactivation with memory tasks (Sanchez-Carrion et al., 2008). Furthermore, experimental models of TBI have shown fairly robust effects on hippocampal neurons as demonstrated by significant loss in the CA2 and CA3 regions (Dixon et al., 1987; Hicks et al., 1993; Smith et al., 1994). While the hippocampus is not generally thought of in reference to DA signaling, it does have glutamatergic projections to the striatum that are important to the activity of GABAergic medium spiny neurons. Striatal medium spiny neurons also receive DAergic input from the VTA and SN (Meredith et al., 1990; Pennartz and Kitai, 1991). Furthermore, DA receptors in the hippocampus receive projections from the SN and have been shown to facilitate the maintenance of long-term potentiation (LTP), which is hypothesized to be the physiologic basis for memory formation and consolidation (Li et al., 2003; Lemon and Manahan-Vaughan, 2006).

2.3. Attention, processing speed, working memory, and executive function after TBI

Following TBI, patients often demonstrate confusion as well as an inability to concentrate. They are also distracted, have difficulties performing more than one task at a time, and require increased time to perform tasks (Gentilini et al., 1989; Draper and Ponsford, 2008). While these tasks are often broad in nature, they share a commonality in the form of attention processing. TBI patients consistently show impairments on measures of processing speed, including the Symbol Digit Modalities Task and Digit Symbol Coding. Attentional processing is believed to be a widely distributed cognitive function that involves both cortical and subcortical pathways including striatal and thalamic inputs and reticular activation. However, it is generally accepted that DA function plays a significant role in the ability to focus attention (Cohen and Servan-Schreiber, 1992; Wise et al., 1996; Brennan and Arnsten, 2008). In ADHD, DAergic drug treatments have been shown to be effective in treating attentional disorders (Solanto, 1998). This does not rule out other possible neural dysfunctions as being the underlying cause of attentional difficulties after TBI, but it does provide compelling reasons to examine DAergic function in TBI.

Attention processing can be difficult to localize given that there are multiple modalities of attentional function, including auditory, tactile, and visual attention (Arciniegas et al., 2000; Spence and Gallace, 2007; Adair and Barrett, 2008). Furthermore attention can refer to a wide variety of different cognitive processes that are both voluntary and involuntary and involve several brain systems including the parietal cortices, basal ganglia, PFC, and anterior cingulate cortex (Raz, 2004). In contrast, the anatomy of memory function is comparatively better understood with specific memory processing events (e.g., retrieval, consolidation) ascribed to specific brain regions (Izquierdo and Medina, 1997; Izquierdo et al., 1997). In particular, damage to the hippocampus has historically been associated with reproducible deficits in spatial and temporal memory processing (Buckley, 2005). In experimental TBI it is known that the hippocampus is exquisitely sensitive to both acute apoptotic events and excitotoxicity (Kotapka et al., 1991; Hicks et al., 1993; Dietrich et al., 1994; Smith et al., 1994). Regionalization of memory function and the reduction of hippocampal memory processing to spatial and temporal memory are simplistic views of memory processing in the CNS, but have helped guide research in the mechanisms of cognitive dysfunction following diffuse injuries such as TBI. However, at least one study (Lyeth et al., 1990) showed spatial memory impairments without clear neuronal loss in the hippocampus. Furthermore, strategies to reduce neuronal cell loss in the hippocampus using N-methyl-d-aspartic acid (NMDA) receptor antagonists and inhibitors of apoptosis have not always demonstrated behavioral improvements that correlate well with the level of neuronal sparing (Tolias and Bullock, 2004). These findings have challenged the practice of attributing cognitive dysfunction to discrete damage in specific brain regions after TBI and raised a new set of challenges for TBI research to look beyond anatomic damage and into functional studies.

Numerous studies have shown that after TBI there is a dysfunction in hippocampal LTP (Reeves et al., 1995; Sanders et al., 2000). The reasons for this impairment are not entirely clear. Falo et al. (2006, 2008) have suggested that the impairments may be due to changes in synaptic composition and dysfunction in normal molecular processes that influence synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus, including dysfunction in scaffolding proteins. What has not been examined is the role that DA may have in this process. It is known that blockade of D1/D5 receptors in the hippocampus eliminates late LTP and can even effect early LTP, suggesting that synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus depends upon a synergistic interaction between glutamate and DA (Frey et al., 1991; Granado et al., 2008). This raises the question about what other structures and what other events, besides hippocampal neuronal loss, could also be relevant to memory function after TBI.

The PFC and corticostriatal DA signaling system have, in addition to the hippocampus, been shown to be important for memory formation. The aspect of memory generally assigned to the PFC has been that of WM. WM commonly refers to those cognitive processes that provide the capacity to maintain and manipulate a limited amount of information over a brief period of time (Baddeley et al., 1986; Baddeley, 1992). WM incorporates aspects of divided attention, which is vulnerable to disruption after TBI (Stuss et al., 1985; Levin, 1990; Ponsford and Kinsella, 1992; McDowell et al., 1997). For example, a study of patients one year following treatment for severe TBI consistently found that these individuals were significantly impaired on the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test, a demanding task that recruits WM (Levin, 1990). WM deficits can be particularly disabling given the critical role of WM in overall intellectual functioning (Smith et al., 1996). WM processes are, arguably, an encoding process for long-term memory (Johnson, 1992). WM is also important for a wide variety of cognitive skills, such as problem solving, planning, and active listening (Jonides, 1995). Individuals with significant WM deficits have great difficulty recording features from a changing environment and keeping them in mind in order to guide behavior (Smith et al., 1996). As such, WM is a primary and critical component of all aspects of cognition, and impairment in this cognitive domain can be particularly disruptive for everyday functioning

TBI also causes an impairment in executive control or executive functioning, a critical aspect of cognition (Hanks et al., 1999; McDonald et al., 2002). Although individuals and clinicians often report or emphasize “memory” as being a primary functional concern, executive control dysfunction might actually be the most disabling aspect of cognitive compromise after brain injury (Mateer, 1999; Millis et al., 2001). It is known that impairments in executive control can compromise other aspects of cognition, such as memory for verbal information (Tremont et al., 2000) and visual information (Ricker et al., 1994; Lange et al., 2000; Ricker et al., 2001).

2.4. Emotional lability and behavioral alterations after TBI

Although not as well studied as cognitive deficits after TBI, it is widely recognized that TBI patients experience alterations in emotional control and general behavior (Oddy et al., 1985; Arciniegas et al., 2000). Dyer et al. (2006) reported that of three groups, TBI, spinal cord injury, and non-injured, the TBI group was more likely to be rated worse in areas of impulsivity and verbal aggressiveness. Patients with TBI are also known to have increased rates of depression (Seel et al., 2003; Jorge et al., 2004; Moldover et al., 2004). The mesocorticolimbic DA signaling pathway has been implicated in emotional and behavioral disturbances (Mega and Cummings, 1994). In fact, in schizophrenia, many of the observed affective mood disorders are proposed to be due to changes in DA signaling within the mesocorticolimbic signaling pathway (Abi-Dargham and Moore, 2003).

2.5. The contribution of animal models in understanding persistent cognitive dysfunction after TBI

FP and CCI rodent models of TBI have been used extensively to produce deficits reminiscent of those seen clinically. Both models produce spatial learning and WM deficits in adult rats (Lyeth et al., 1990; Smith et al., 1991; Hamm et al., 1992, 1993, 1996a,b; Hicks et al., 1993; Colicos et al., 1996; Dixon et al., 1996, 1997; Scheff et al., 1997; Kline et al., 2000, 2002, 2004, 2007a,b, 2008; Wagner et al., 2002) and mice (Smith et al., 1994; Fox et al., 1998; Whalen et al., 1999) as tested in the Morris water maze (MWM). One concern when utilizing the MWM paradigm of memory testing is that this task represents a particularly stressful environment to animals. Furthermore, the MWM task is both a learning and memory task, and it is possible that dysfunction in MWM performance is, in part, due to defects in learning and coping strategies associated with damage in thalamic structures (Markowitsch, 1982; Aggleton and Brown, 1999; Van der Werf et al., 2003). Importantly experimental TBI has also been shown to cause dysfunction in less stressful paradigms including open field exploration and radial arm maze tasks (Lyeth et al., 1990; Soblosky et al., 1996; Lindner et al., 1998; Enomoto et al., 2005; Wagner et al., 2007b). While open field exploration tasks typically assess mobility and anxiety, changes in the exploration of novel environments is considered an important indicator of learning and memory function in rats if other factors (anxiety, olfactory sensitivity, etc.) are properly controlled for (File, 2001; Christoffersen et al., 2008). Interestingly deficits post-TBI have also been observed in passive-avoidance tasks, which are typically considered to be a hippocampal independent measure of learning (Hamm et al., 1994; Hogg et al., 1998; Milman et al., 2005). Due to its versatility and consistent results the MWM task remains a useful measure of memory dysfunction following experimental TBI. However, it is important to consider its limitations and the contribution of other cognitive processes to MWM deficits.

Analysis of the affected brain regions in animal models coupled with clinical studies have helped identify the brain areas thought to be responsible for cognitive processes commonly affected by TBI. The PFC, hippocampus, striatum, and limbic structures have all been shown to be sensitive to damage after TBI (Dixon et al., 1987; Lighthall, 1988; Lighthall et al., 1989). The degree of damage to these structures depends in large part on the localization and severity of TBI. Mild TBI models may only show diffuse white matter damage, cortical cell loss, and some hippocampal cell loss (Hicks et al., 1993; Sanders et al., 2001), while more moderate to severe injuries show greater degrees of both cortical and subcortical neuronal death and damage to structures beyond the contusion site including ischemic alterations in subcortical structures (Dietrich et al., 1994; Hellmich et al., 2005). There are also differences in structural damage depending upon the location of the insult (Lighthall et al., 1989; Thompson et al., 2005).

Animal studies of spatial learning and WM, coupled with clinical research examining executive function, attention, and behavior have consistently demonstrated prolonged cognitive dysfunction after TBI. Due to its importance to WM, executive function, behavior and emotion, and psychosis, DA represents a promising avenue of research in TBI therapy. TBI also shares some similarities to PD and other DA disorders. Similar to PD, TBI patients can experience memory impairment, bradykinetic motor dysfunctions, and decreases in cognitive processing speed. TBI patients also exhibit attention difficulties similar to ADHD, such as easy distractibility. Unlike pure disorders in DA function, TBI has its own unique set of concerns including inflammation, white matter damage, disruption of other neurotransmitter systems, and a unique series of temporal events in DA effects that are unlike other insults. For these reasons, understanding the DAergic signaling system and its relation to brain function is an important step in the proper utilization of DA targeted treatment strategies.

3. Dopamine at the cellular level: gatekeeper of cognition

In the brain, DAergic neurons arise from the VTA and SN and project to the striatum, cortex, limbic system, and hypothalamus (Graybiel, 1990). DA influences on a number of physiologic functions including hormone secretion, movement control, motivation, emotion, and cognitive processing (Jackson and Westlind-Danielsson, 1994; Floresco and Magyar, 2006).

The unique effects of DA at each of its terminal sites are mediated by membrane receptors belonging to the large seven transmembrane domain (7TM) G-protein coupled receptors. Activation of DA receptors leads to alterations in intracellular second messengers either through formation or inhibition. Cloning experiments have identified five different DA receptors that are divided into two groups based upon their structural and pharmacological properties; D1-like and D2-like receptors (Bunzow et al., 1988; Dal Toso et al., 1989; Dearry et al., 1990; Zhou et al., 1990). Each of these receptor families have unique differences in regional CNS expression and unique intracellular signaling pathways that will be discussed briefly in the context of cognition. For a more complete review of the complex biochemical signaling pathways related to each receptor and the information gained from knockout mouse studies (D1–D5) see Gonon et al. (2000) and Tan et al. (2003).

In addition to different subsets of receptor populations throughout regions of the brain, DA effects are regulated through a complex control of DA release, re-uptake, and metabolism. Changes in a number of DA constituents, including the DA transporter (DAT) and tyrosine hydroxylase (TH), can effectively alter DA extracellular concentrations and its physiologic effect without directly affecting binding or receptor response.

3.1. DA release, metabolism, and the DA transporter

DA is synthesized first by the hydroxylation of the amino acid l-tyrosine to 3,4-dihydroxy-l-phenylalanine via the enzyme TH and is then decarboxylated by l-amino acid decarboxylase to DA. DA can be inactivated either by reuptake via the DAT and subsequent enzymatic breakdown by catechol-O-methyl transferase and monoamine oxidase or repackaged into vesicles for reuse (for further clarification of these systems and the role they may play in TBI injury see Section 4).

The DAT is particularly important to presynaptic DA regulation, and differences in DAT expression due to genetic polymorphisms have been implicated in a number of diseases including bipolar disorder and ADHD (Greenwood et al., 2001; Thapar et al., 2005; Swanson et al., 2007). Furthermore, DAT inhibition is the mechanism of action for a number of pharmaceuticals designed to enhance DA neurotransmission as well as drugs of abuse, such as cocaine (Hitri et al., 1994; Madras et al., 1994).

The DAT is a Na+/Cl-dependent plasma membrane neurotransmitter transporter containing twelve transmembrane domains with both the amino and the carboxyl termini located on the intracellular side of the membrane (Hersch et al., 1997). DAT terminates the action of vesicular DA release at the synapse via reuptake of extracellular DA (Torres et al., 2003) and acts as a reverse transporter of DA under basal conditions (Borland and Michael, 2004). Regional distribution of DAT has been found in areas of the brain with established DAergic circuitry including mesostriatal, mesolimbic, and mesocortical pathways (Ciliax et al., 1999). The rate at which DAT removes DA from the synapse has a profound effect on the amount of DA in the cell. This is best evidenced by the severe cognitive deficits, motor abnormalities, and hyperactivity seen in DAT knockout mice (Perona et al., 2008). DAT activity and expression has also been shown to change during normal aging (Bannon et al., 1992) and differs between males and females (Piccini, 2003).

There are many ways in which the DAT may be chronically regulated, including gene transcription, post-translational modifications, oligomerization, and trafficking (Doolen and Zahniser, 2001). Second messenger systems, in particular protein kinase C (PKC) activation, affect transporter activity (Huff et al., 1997). Activation of PKC via substrate and inhibitor binding appears to regulate DAT by altering its cellular distribution; specifically by altering levels of membrane bound DAT (Daniels and Amara, 1999). In addition, DA itself can regulate DAT via its interaction with the transporter or pre-synaptic autoreceptors (Williams and Galli, 2006). The DAT is also the target of several “DAT-blockers” including amphetamines and methylphenidate (MPD). These chemicals inhibit the action of DAT and, to a lesser extent, the other monoamine transporters. In contrast, amphetamines trigger a signal cascade thought to involve PKC or mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPK) that leads to the internalization of DAT molecules, which are normally expressed on the neuron's surface (Kahlig et al., 2004).

3.2. D1-like

The D1-like receptor family includes D1 and D5 receptors. For the purpose of this review, we will focus on the D1 receptor because the D5 receptor is less well studied in TBI. The distribution of the D1 receptor differs dependent upon the brain region that is being examined. Within the frontal cortex, D1 receptors are localized on post-synaptic dendrites of both pyramidal and non-pyramidal neurons. Similar structural localization of D1 receptors can be seen in the hippocampus and limbic cortex (Dearry et al., 1990; Monsma et al., 1990; Zhou et al., 1990). However, within the striatum, D1 receptors have been localized to non-synaptic dendritic spines suggesting that DA activity within the striatum is unique compared to cortical structures (Hersch et al., 1995; Caillé et al., 1996).

D1 receptor activation is classically associated with the stimulation of adenylate cyclase (AC) and subsequent activation of cAMP. However, it has also been demonstrated that D1 receptors are able to activate phosphoinositide hydrolysis (Undie et al., 1994) and inhibit arachidonic acid release (Schinelli et al., 1994). These varied receptor signaling mechanisms demonstrate the wide range of effects that D1 agonism or antagonism can have in the CNS. Activation of phosphoinositide hydrolysis is known to effect intracellular trafficking, cell growth, differentiation, and survival (Undie et al., 1994, 2000; Ming et al., 2006; Liu et al., 2008). Arachidonic acid release is important for inflammatory signaling and neuronal functioning such as LTP and synaptic plasticity (Tassoni et al., 2008). While both of these D1 receptor actions are important to DAergic function and have the potential to mediate insults to DA containing neurons affected by TBI, it is D1 receptor mediated control over intracellular cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP) signaling that represents perhaps the most pertinent aspect of D1 signaling to cognition. Manipulation of cAMP levels can affect cAMP response element binding (CREB) through both MAPK dependent and MAPK independent pathways (Greengard, 1976; Schulman, 1995; Waltereit and Weller, 2003). In DA containing neurons, there is an intracellular signaling molecule, DA and cAMP-regulated phosphoprotein mKDa 32 (DARPP-32), which is known to mediate a number of important cellular signaling events and is intimately involved in cAMP signaling (Ouimet et al., 1984; Walaas and Greengard, 1984; Hemmings and Greengard, 1986; Greengard et al., 1999).

D1 knockout mice demonstrate spatial learning deficits (El-Ghundi et al., 1999), and D1a knockout mice show broad impairments in initiation of activity learning of cue related tasks (Smith et al., 1998). However, the idea of receptors being impaired versus not impaired may be a simplistic viewpoint of DA interaction at the D1 receptor. A review by Williams and Castner (2006) describes the mathematical relationship of tight DAergic control and its relation to dysfunction. They point out that D1 receptor mediation of PFC function is often dependent upon both concentration and the temporal sequence of DA release. A series of papers examining DA signaling and calcium regulation describe a paradigm in which there exists a difference in the kinetics of CREB phosphorylation that is dependent upon the length of incubation with a D1 agonist (Liu and Graybiel, 1996, 1998a,b). Ruskin and Marshall (1997) have shown that D1 receptor mediated induction of Fos in striatal neurons is dependent on D2 activation. Goldman-Rakic (1995) has described at least three possible cellular mechanisms for DAergic control of WM in the PFC; (1) direct synaptic control over pyramidal neuron activity, (2) nonsynaptic interaction with DA receptors located on distal spines of PFC pyramidal neurons, or (3) indirect modulation through GABAergic interneurons. Frey et al. (1991) have demonstrated that blockade of D1 receptors in the hippocampus impairs hippocampal late LTP maintenance, which may be another reason DA dysfunction impairs memory. This alteration in hippocampal LTP associated with D1 receptor changes is believed to contribute to spatial memory deficiencies observed in D1 receptor knockout mice (Matthies et al., 1997; Granado et al., 2008).

Recent studies in PD (Cooper et al., 1991; Postle et al., 1997) have shown that WM dysfunction is often one of the first cognitive symptoms experienced by patients. This WM deficit is believed to be D1 receptor-mediated because D1 agonist treatment has been shown to alleviate impairments in young and aged non-human primates (Arnsten et al., 1994; Cai and Arnsten, 1997; Castner and Goldman-Rakic, 2004), as well as in other conditions characterized by prefrontal DA loss, such as chronic stress (Mizoguchi et al., 2000), chronic neuroleptic treatment (Castner et al., 2000), and 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP) treatment-induced delayed response performance (Schneider et al., 1994a,b). How hippocampal DA loss is associated with these processes is unclear, but in non-PD paradigms, the administration of D1 agonists has been shown to facilitate hippocampal LTP (Otmakhova and Lisman, 1996).

3.3. D2-like

The D2-like receptor family comprises D2, D3, and D4 receptors. For the purpose of this review, the focus will be on D2 receptors because D3 and D4 receptors are less well studied in TBI. D2 receptors are different from D1 in that they are associated both with terminal receptor activation and with presynaptic auto-receptor function and demonstrate a much greater expression in the striatum compared to other brain regions. However, like D1, D2 receptor expression in the striatum and frontal cortex are of particular interest in cognitive research. Interestingly, within the striatum, the pre and post-synaptic localization of the D2 receptors maintain a much closer proximity to actual synapses as opposed to the more diffuse location of D1 receptors on spines.

D2 receptor activation is more complex than D1 activation for a number of reasons. For example, D2 receptors can act through inhibitory G-proteins that lead to an inhibition of AC and cAMP (Cote et al., 1983; Onali et al., 1985; Weiss et al., 1985) or independently of cAMP pathways (Memo et al., 1986). D2 receptors have also been shown to inhibit phosphoinositide hydrolysis and subsequent Ca2+ mobilization (Vallar and Meldolesi, 1989; Picetti et al., 1997). An interesting caveat of D2 receptor activation is that in vitro studies utilizing co-immunoprecipitation methods and in vivo studies utilizing fluorescent and bioluminescent energy transfer (FRET and BRET) analyses have shown that many D2 receptor systems exist in heterodimerized pairs (Canals et al., 2003; Fuxe et al., 2005). D2 receptors have been shown to form heterodimers with adenosine A2a receptors and metabo-tropic glutamate-5 (Mglut-5) receptors (Diaz-Cabiale et al., 2002; Fuxe et al., 2003, 2005; Hillion et al., 2002; Ferré et al., 2008). A2a and Mglut-5 heterodimers are of particular interest for TBI research due to their role in striatal LTP and long-term depression (LTD) and subsequent plasticity of striatal connections (Ferré et al., 2002; Fuxe et al., 2003). The identification of heterodimer systems within the striatum indicate that D1 and D2 have control over CREB signaling within striatal neurons. Further, they modulate LTP and LTD dependent glutamatergic and adenosine transmitter systems (Calabresi et al., 2000, 2007; Centonze et al., 1999, 2003).

In PD, the contribution of neurotransmitters beyond DA is important to therapeutic manipulation as utilization of these other receptor systems provides an alternative way to manipulate DAergic signaling (Kase et al., 2000). D2–A2a interactions proposed by Fuxe et al. (2005) allow for precise alterations in intracellular signaling pathways that have been associated with cognitive function and striatal output. Mglut-5 receptors have also been implicated as important for control of DA signaling at the D2 receptor in a similar fashion to the A2a receptor.

3.4. DARPP-32

DA signaling cascades in the striatum are involved in numerous physiologic functions including synaptic plasticity, movement regulation, and even the modulation of other neurotransmitter systems (e.g., acetylcholine, calcium, glutamate, and GABA). DARPP-32 is a cytoplasmic phosphoprotein found in 95% of the medium spiny neurons in the striatum (Fig. 2), and it plays a central role in nearly all DA mediated events (Greengard et al., 1999; Calabresi et al., 2000; Svenningsson et al., 2005; Valjent et al., 2005). Two distinct phosphorylation sites, threonine-34 (T34) and threonine-75 (T75), make DARPP-32 a bifunctional signal transduction molecule that controls the activities of both protein phosphatase-1 (PP1) and protein kinase A(PKA) (Halpain et al., 1990; Nishi et al., 2002). The activity of both PP1 and PKA tightly regulates protein transcription related to numerous important cellular functions including neurotrophic factor production, regulation of synaptic plasticity, and cell homeostasis.

Fig. 2
Schematic of DARPP-32 signaling adapted from Nairn et al. (2004). DARPP-32 represents an important regulator of cell signaling within the medium spiny neurons of the striatum. Changes in phosphorylation state due to DA alterations effectively alter CREB ...

DA-induced phosphorylation of ionotropic glutamate and GABA receptors are attenuated in DARPP-32 knockout mice, suggesting that DARPP-32 plays an important role in regulating excitatory neurotransmission (Yan et al., 1999; Flores-Hernandez et al., 2000, 2002). Ethanol reinforcement, which is known to act through regulation of NMDA receptors, is also reduced in DARPP-32 knockout models (Risinger et al., 2001; Maldve et al., 2002). Studies assessing the behavioral affects of other drugs of abuse, including cocaine and morphine, have shown that DARPP-32 knockout mice demonstrate lower levels of psychomotor activation following drug administration compared to wild-type mice (Borgkvist et al., 2007). Further evidence using knockout mice indicates that DARPP-32 is necessary for the induction of striatal LTD and LTP, both important processes in models of memory acquisition and consolidation (Calabresi et al., 2007). Furthermore, after hypoxia-ischemia injury, DARPP-32 phosphorylation states were shown to be important to membrane potential, glutamate receptor activity, and oxidative stress (Yang et al., 2007).

DA acts through D1 receptor mediated increases in PKA to promote DARPP-32 phosphorylation at its T34 site, which leads to an inhibition of PP1. Additionally, DA, glutamate, and adenosine act on protein phosphatase 2B (PP2B), also known as calcineurin, and protein phosphatase 2A (PP2A) to decrease phosphorylation at T34 and increase phosphorylation at T75. A reduction in phosphorylation at T34 subsequently removes the inhibitory affect of DARPP-32 on PP1 (Greengard et al., 1999; Nairn et al., 2004; Svenningsson et al., 2004). The regulation of PP1 and PKA by DARPP-32 allows convergent DA, glutamate, and adenosine signaling to alter the phosphorylation state of NMDA receptor subunits, the sodium and potassium adenosine triphosphatase (Na/K ATPase), and members of the extracellular regulated kinase (ERK) pathway (Bertorello et al., 1990; Blank et al., 1997; Fienberg et al., 1998; Snyder et al., 1998; Dudman et al., 2003; Hakansson et al., 2004).

Given the wide range of cellular processes mediated by DA, its interaction with DARPP-32 in the striatum, and the effect that DA action at its different receptor systems have on PFC and hippocampal function, it is clear that even minor disturbances in DA function could have significant implications for CNS function.

4. Alterations in DAergic systems after TBI

The efficacy of DA receptor agonists suggests that TBI patients benefit from the promotion of central DAergic transmission. This could be a sign that DA release is suppressed after injury, that DA uptake is over active, or some combination of the two. Alternatively, it might be the case that DA activity remains normal after injury, but that basal DA activity is inadequate in the face of the injury-induced disruptions. Given that a few studies have also shown benefits with DA antagonists it must also be recognized that TBI possesses a complex series of temporally specific injuries to a wide range of different brain structures. It is important to acknowledge that any systemic treatment without a well-defined window of therapy could be both beneficial and detrimental to the recovery process. For this reason, understanding the effect of TBI on DA transmission is crucial for proper therapeutic management and for promoting optimal recovery.

Evidence that DA systems are altered in humans after TBI is predominantly based on reports that neurostimulants are beneficial in attenuating cognitive deficits (Goldstein, 2003; McAllister et al., 2004) and data showing altered DA transporter binding after TBI (Donnemiller et al., 2000). Donnemiller et al. (2000) used single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) to show that striatal DAT binding is decreased in patients 4–5 months after severe TBI, even in cases where no anatomical evidence of direct striatal injury exists.

After experimental TBI, alterations in catecholamine systems have been found in various brain regions and have been shown to be time-dependent (Huger and Patrick, 1979; Dunn-Meynell et al., 1994; McIntosh et al., 1994; Massucci et al., 2004). For example, regional increases in DA levels at acute time points after acceleration–deceleration brain injury were reported by Huger and Patrick (1979). A transient increase in DA in both the striatum, up to 6 h, and the hypothalamus, up to 24 h, have been identified utilizing microdialysis (McIntosh et al., 1994). Interestingly, in the McIntosh et al., 1994 study cortical tissue DA levels are actually depressed for up to 2 weeks post-FPI. In a CCI model of brain injury, there were significant increases in rat brain tissue DA levels and metabolism at 1 h in the contralateral frontal cortex and 1 day in the ipsilateral frontal cortex (Massucci et al., 2004). Tissue DA levels were also elevated in both the ipsilateral and contralateral striatum at 1 h compared to sham animals. Metabolism of DA was measured by the dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC)/DA ratios demonstrating a bilateral increase in DA metabolism at 1 hr in the striatum of CCI injured rats (Massucci et al., 2004). TH activity and catecholamine increases (both DA and NE) have also been seen in the prelimbic and infralimbic cortices, areas critical to PFC function, up to 2 weeks following CCI (Kobori et al., 2006). Increases in human cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) DA and its metabolites post-TBI have been shown to depend on both gender and genetic variations in the DAT. Furthermore, the systemic administration of DA as an inotropic agent in TBI patients was also associated with higher CSF DA (Wagner et al., 2007c). Increased DA metabolism may represent a compensatory response to increased tissue DA levels or may be due to further direct effects of TBI on DA regulation. The consequences of these changes can be either beneficial in that DA neurotransmission is restored after TBI or could potentially be deleterious due to DA-induced oxidative stress.

Fig. 3 shows a summary of noted changes in DA at the cellular level after TBI, which includes alterations in TH, DAT, and DA receptors.

Fig. 3
Schematic of alterations in DA cellular signaling following TBI in the PFC and striatum. Changes in DA at the cellular level both pre and post-synaptically have been observed in the PFC and striatum of rats. These alterations include changes in tissue ...

Given the importance of DA receptors to cognition, alterations in receptor expression has been another area of interest following TBI. Henry et al. (1997) reported a transient decrease in striatal D1 receptors immediately after injury,followedbyanincreaseat1day, and a subsequent return to pre-injury levels. No significant alterations in D2 receptor binding was reported in the study by Henry and colleagues. Direct analysis of DA D2 receptor protein via Western blots has shown no significant reduction in striatal D2 receptor or D1 receptor expression at 2 weeks in a rat model of TBI (Wagner et al., 2005, 2009). This finding suggests that DAergic dysfunction following TBI is not entirely mediated by changes in DA receptors.

In addition to temporal alterations in tissue DA levels there have also been changes observed in TH, the rate-limiting enzyme in catecholamine synthesis. TBI has been shown to increase TH protein in the rat frontal cortex at 28 days post-injury (Yan et al., 2001). Measured increases in TH protein are most likely due to enhanced synthesis as phospho-TH is also increased (Kobori et al., 2006). In contrast, DA beta hydroxylase protein levels were not altered after TBI suggesting that the increase in TH occurred predominantly in DAergic axons (Yan et al., 2001). The absence of a decrease in TH positive SN neurons further differentiates TBI from PD. Increases in TH protein have also been observed in the striatum with a similar temporal profile (Wagner et al., 2005, 2009; Yan et al., 2007).

DAT is a crucial protein in the regulation of DA neurotransmission, playing a central role in determining the duration of action of DA by rapidly taking up extracellular DA into pre-synaptic terminals after release (Horn, 1990; Gainetdinov et al., 1998). Studies in animals lacking expression of the DAT gene (Gainetdinov, 2008; Wu et al., 2007) suggest that this protein is perhaps the single most important determinant of the extraneuronal concentration and duration of DA. Differences in the number of uptake sites (Nirenberg et al., 1997; Sesack et al., 1998) in different brain regions provide DA with different extracellular lifetimes (Garris et al., 1994). Regional decreases in total DAT expression have been reported after CCI (Wagner et al., 2005, 2009). Alterations in DAT expression suggest that improvements in cognition and neurobehavioral recovery reported in experimental (Kline et al., 1994, 2000; Goldstein, 2003) and clinical (Whyte et al., 1997, 2004) TBI studies with the use of DAT inhibitors may, in part, confer their beneficial effects by increasing striatal extracellular DA in a post-injury environment where cortical influences on striatal DA neurotransmission may be impaired.

Recent work suggests that both frontal cortex and striatal decreases in total DAT expression post-TBI are gender specific and occur primarily in males (Wagner et al., 2005). Estrogen is known to have both a developmental and signaling role in DA systems so it is not surprising that TBI has different effects on DA signaling in males versus females. Specifically Wagner et al. (2005) showed that, when compared to gender-matched controls, male rats demonstrated a proportionally larger decrease in DAT expression compared to females. Furthermore, it was shown that environmental enrichment, which improves cognitive recovery (Hamm et al., 1996a; Passineau et al., 2001; Wagner et al., 2002; Kline et al., 2007b; Hoffman et al., 2008b) exerted bigger effects on post-injury DAT reductions in females compared to males. What remains unclear is whether the DAT changes that occur in males and females following TBI are beneficial or detrimental. Interestingly there is evidence that other members of the catecholamine metabolism system being altered post-TBI. Specifically there have been noted increases in catechol-O-methyl transferase expression 24 h post-TBI that persists for up to 14 days in the microglia of the injured hippocampus, suggesting a possible compensation for observed changes in DAT activity and providing further evidence of DA dysfunction (Redell and Dash, 2007).

Studies examining DA neurotransmission have demonstrated reduced evoked DA overflow and altered kinetics of DA clearance in the striatum when assessed utilizing fast scan cyclic voltammetry and using a medial forebrain bundle stimulation paradigm (Wagner et al., 2005). Recent work suggests that daily treatment with MPD for two weeks after CCI reverses deficits in DA neurotransmission. Interestingly, after two weeks of MPD treatment, there were no significant changes noted in DAT expression despite robust changes in DA neurotransmission and kinetics parameters (Wagner et al., 2009). Functional changes in DAT activity and trafficking, as well as other changes in DA receptor function may be responsible for the MPD mediated effects in DA neurotransmission observed.

What effect these alterations in DAT, evoked DA release, and TH increases have on DA signaling has just begun to be elucidated. Ongoing research into these molecular events will help researches develop targeted therapeutic strategies that specifically address TBI-induced deficits in DA signaling.

5. Dopamine in the clinic: the past and future role of DA agonists and antagonists

In 2006 the Neurotrauma Foundation (NTF) published an excellent review of current clinical recommendations for TBI management in both the acute and rehabilitative phases (Warden et al., 2006). As part of its review the NTF identified three drugs with DAergic effects as current viable options to assist with cognitive recovery. The identified pharmacotherapies were MPD, amantadine hydrochloride (AMH), and bromocriptine. MPD was recommended to enhance attentional function and speed of processing. Both MPD and AMH were considered reasonable options to enhance general cognitive function after TBI. Bromocriptine was recommended to enhance executive function after TBI. The NTF also acknowledged that there remains limited clinical evidence for long-term benefits with DAergic medications, but that initial clinical case reports and clinical studies have shown promise for stimulants (e.g., MPD), AMH, and bromocriptine. Both clinical data concerning the efficacy of DA agonists and a substantial amount of literature in animal models demonstrate improved functional recovery with DA agonists post-TBI. For the purpose of this review we are presenting research dealing with DA receptor agonists in adult TBI populations and corresponding animal studies.

5.1. CNs stimulants (amphetamine and methylphenidate)

5.1.1. Mechanism of action

Stimulants are often employed to assist in the rehabilitation of individuals with TBI. The most commonly used stimulants are amphetamine (AMPH) and MPD. Both AMPH and MPD have been used as drugs of choice for the treatment of ADHD. AMPH is a CNS stimulant that possesses two main mechanisms of action. AMPH acts on presynaptic nerve terminals to inhibit the reuptake of serotonin, norepinephrine (NE), and DA by acting as a substrate for monoamine transporters, including the DAT, causing a downregulation of transporter expression (Kahlig and Galli, 2003). AMPH also increases monoamine secretion via exchange diffusion and reverse transport (Haracz et al., 1998; Volkow et al., 2002a,b; Fleckenstein et al., 2007). In contrast MPD's predominant mechanism of action is via blockade of the DAT (Volkow et al., 2002a,b, 1998). Cognitive benefits on attention observed with AMPH and MPD treatment have been associated with the effective increase in DA caused by their administration (Volkow et al., 2001, 2004; Schiffer et al., 2006).

5.1.2. AMPH and MPD in the clinic

The administration of AMPH in the clinic to treat TBI is not as widely practiced as is the accepted pharmacotherapeutic strategy of providing MPD. Thus, this section will focus almost entirely on MPD. However, we would be remiss if we did not mention a report by Evans et al. (1987) who administered d-AMPH following closed head TBI and found that the treatment enhanced processing speed and improved memory. Regarding MPD, multiple studies have demonstrated its effectiveness in treating cognitive dysfunction after brain trauma (Table 1). In a double blind study, Gualtieri (1988) found that MPD treatment improved performance on measures of nonverbal fluency and selective attention as well as self-report measures in a subset of TBI “responders”. Another clinical trial found that low dose MPD treatment following moderate to severe TBI improved functional outcome at day 30 over controls as measured through the Disability Rating Scale (DRS) (Plenger et al., 1996). Kaelin et al. (1996) showed a trend toward improved DRS and a significant improvement in attention with low does MPD treatment after TBI. Other recent studies have demonstrated beneficial effects of MPD on attention (Whyte et al., 1997) and information processing speed (Whyte et al., 2004) in individuals with TBI. However, a few studies have shown no effect on cognitive outcomes with MPD treatment (Mooney and Haas, 1993; Speech et al., 1993; Tiberti et al., 1998). It must be noted that for the Speech et al. (1993) study, the authors noted that statistical power was low, and in Tiberti et al. (1998) study, the outcome measurement was memory function following organic amnesia, which may not be as profoundly influenced as attentional processing.

Table 1
Cognitive outcomes with the use of methylphenidate in traumatic brain injury.

To better understand the mechanism of action responsible for the cognitive benefits gained with CNS stimulant treatment, specifically MPD, animal studies have examined both the behavioral and biochemical aspects of MPD treatment post-TBI.

5.1.3. AMPH and MPD in animal models

Peak levels of MPD in the plasma and brain following intravenous injection occur within 20 min in awake animals and correspond with peak striatal DA levels as measured by microdialysis (Huff and Davies, 2002). Intraperitoneal and oral administration of MPD in rodents shows peak striatal DA levels after 40 min (Gerasimov et al., 2000). It has also been demonstrated that MPD has a shorter half-life in rats than in humans (Kuczenski and Segal, 2005). In order to overcome these limitations, doses are often larger to maintain drug levels at a therapeutic target over longer time intervals. Regardless of these limitations, experimental models utilizing a MPD treatment paradigm have been able to demonstrate cognitive benefit after both cortical ablation and TBI injuries (Kline et al., 1994, 2000). Specifically, a single administration of MPD followed by significant symptom relevant experience (i.e., beam walking experience) enhanced recovery of motor function following sensorimotor cortex lesions (Kline et al., 1994). Moreover, daily MPD treatments beginning as late as 24 h after TBI in rats reveal significantly less spatial memory performance deficits versus saline treatment (Kline et al., 2000). Wagner et al. (2009) showed that daily treatment with MPD (5 mg/kg) post-CCI resulted in increased DA overflow and Vmax. There was no associated effect of MPD treatment on DAT localization, or DA receptor expression, however there was a significant increase in cfos expression with MPD treatment.

Interestingly, while the exact mechanisms of MPD benefit on cognition after TBI are still not understood, gender differences have been observed. Wagner et al. (2007a) showed that injured males treated with MPD had no change in active behaviors and displayed significant improvements in a MWM task indicating that daily treatment with MPD results in enhanced cognition without noticeable motor enhancement. However, MPD treated female rats did not show significant improvement on the MWM task, but did exhibit increased swim speed, which was not observed in males. The researchers note that the dosing regimen was based on previous studies using male rats (5 mg/kg; Kline et al., 2000), and a lower dosing of MPD for injured females may be required to augment learning and memory pathways without inducing stereotypical behaviors that may interfere with attention and learning processes. The finding of increased motor sensitivity to MPD in females may be due to hormonal influences on regional DAT densities post-TBI and/or differences in DAT modification/function (Bosse et al., 1997). In a previous paper Wagner et al. (2005) demonstrated gender specific alterations in DAT expression after TBI. Furthermore, estrogen is known to exert effects on DAergic neuron development (Kipp et al., 2006) and has neuroprotective properties independent of other drug treatments (Gibson et al., 2008). Estrogen has also been shown to act as a signaling molecule within the DA system (Kuppers and Beyer, 1999; Kuppers et al., 2000).

AMPH use in experimental models of TBI and selective cortical injury models has also been shown to accelerate recovery. The positive benefits of AMPH have been reported in FP (Dhillon et al., 1998) and selective lesion studies (Feeney et al., 1981; Hovda et al., 1989; M'Harzi et al., 1988; Chudasama et al., 2005). AMPH treatment has been shown to reduce the accumulation of free fatty acids and lactate following FP in the cortex and hippocampus (Dhillon et al., 1998) and attenuate decreases in cerebral glucose utilization (Queen et al., 1997). However the most interesting effect of AMPH treatment may be in its ability to induce hippocampal brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) following brain injury (Griesbach et al., 2008). This is not surprising given that AMPH treatment is known to induce use dependent plasticity and synaptogenesis (Butefisch et al., 2002) and has been strongly linked to plastic alterations following brain injury (Goldstein, 2003; Ramic et al., 2006). Interestingly, when combined with exercise, AMPH treatment no longer increased BDNF (Griesbach et al., 2008). This finding suggests that the positive benefits of AMPH treatment may be associated with its ability to enhance plastic responses in an injured brain and that combinational therapies are not necessarily more beneficial.

An important caveat to AMPH studies is that while AMPH treatment does increase levels of all monoamines (Fleckenstein et al., 2007), the beneficial effects of AMPH on motor recovery have only been reproduced by intraventricular administration of NE (Boyeson and Feeney, 1990). This does not rule out a positive role for DA facilitation with AMPH treatment on other cognitive processes, but simply suggests that DA mediated benefits on motor recovery may not be due simply to increases in DA release. This is supported by evidence that DA antagonists, such as haloperidol (which will be discussed later), can block the beneficial effects of AMPH treatment (Feeney et al., 1982; Hovda and Feeney, 1985).

5.2. Amantadine hydrochloride

5.2.1. Mechanism of action

AMH is a water soluble salt that has the capability of crossing all cellular membranes including those of the CNS. AMH was originally used as an antiviral agent for influenza type A. Subsequent studies showed it to be effective in treating PD and multiple sclerosis (Godwin-Austen et al., 1970; Rinne et al., 1972; Cohen and Fisher, 1989). Though the mechanism of action for AMH treatment of PD and multiple sclerosis is not completely understood, biochemical studies have demonstrated that AMH increases extracellular DA concentrations by blocking reuptake and by facilitating the synthesis of DA (Von Voigtlander and Moore, 1971; Bak et al., 1972; Gianutsos et al., 1985). In addition to acting at pre-synaptic targets, AMH has been demonstrated to act post-synaptically by increasing post-synaptic DA receptor density (Gianutsos et al., 1985) or altering their conformation (Allen, 1983). Evidence of a post-synaptic mechanism is clinically promising because the mechanisms of actions may not depend solely on the presence of surviving pre-synaptic terminals. Because the mechanism of action of AMH differs from other DA releasing drugs (see Gualtieri et al., 1989 for review), it is likely that the DAergic effects of AMH are a combination of pre-synaptic and post-synaptic effects.

5.2.2. AMH in the clinic

AMH has been found to be effective at treating cognitive dysfunction post-TBI in both clinical trials and case reports (Table 2). While the level of evidence for AMH benefit is not as well developed as for MPD in adult TBI, clinical studies suggest general cognitive improvements with AMH administration after TBI. Zafonte et al. (1998) and Wu and Garmel (2005) reported improved scores on the activities of daily living scales in case reports of patients treated with AMH. Case reports (Chandler et al., 1988; Kraus and Maki, 1997) also indicate general improvements in global functioning. In patients demonstrating indications of diffuse axonal injury after TBI, AMH appeared to be effective in improving cognition independent of the timing of administration (Meythaler et al., 2002). Kraus et al. (2005) showed that improvements in executive function measurements correlated with increases in left PFC glucose metabolism in TBI patients receiving AMH treatment. As reported with other pharmacothera-pies (e.g., MPD) AMH did not confer benefits in all studies conducted (Schneider et al., 1999; Hughes et al., 2005). However, in a review of reports of AMH use after TBI, Sawyer et al. (2008) concluded that it appears to safely improve both arousal and cognition.

Table 2
Cognitive outcomes with the use of Amantadme in traumatic brain injury.

5.2.3. AMH in animal models

The mechanisms of AMH effects are still poorly understood making animal studies of its effects an important step in improving clinical use. In humans, AMH has variable absorption rates and steady state plasma concentrations are typically reached within 4 to 7 days. Doses of AMH given to patients are also somewhat variable due to a lack of correlation between plasma concentrations and therapeutic effects (Aoki and Sitar, 1988). In vitro studies have shown that doses required to affect DA uptake are higher than those used clinically with no significant difference in DA kinetics until concentrations of 40–80 mg/kg were given (Baldessarini et al., 1972; Brown and Redfern, 1976; Page et al., 2000). In brain injury patients the optimal dose of AMH has ranged from 50 to 400 mg/day given orally (Gualtieri et al., 1989). Unfortunately, AMH has not been as extensively researched in experimental models of TBI as CNS stimulants have. However, one study using daily treatment of AMH (10 mg/kg) did show significantly improved spatial memory performance compared to saline treated rats following TBI (Dixon et al., 1999).

5.3. Bromocriptine

5.3.1. Mechanism of action

Bromocriptine is a specific D2 receptor agonist that possesses a rather complex mechanism of action. At high doses (above 10 mg/kg) bromocriptine binds to both highly sensitive presynaptic D2 autoreceptors and less sensitive postsynaptic D2 receptors causing an expected inhibition in DA release and metabolism. Interestingly at lower doses (2.5 and 5 mg/kg) bromocriptine has been shown via microdialysis to increase extracellular DA levels in rats (Brannan et al., 1993). However, even at low doses of bromocriptine there is an associated delayed reduction in DA metabolites consistent with autoreceptor activation (Brannan et al., 1993; Pagliari et al., 1995). In vitro studies have suggested that at low concentrations bromocriptine can act as a partial D2 antagonist (Lieberman and Goldstein, 1985), which may explain an initial increase in DA release. In vivo studies have not been able to provide conclusive evidence of bromocriptine antagonist activity. However, low doses of bromocriptine are characteristically associated with inhibition of DA neuronal firing (Jackson et al., 1990). Consequently, the reason behind this concentration dependent effect of bromocriptine remains unclear. It may be a consequence of D2 receptor location, affinity, or bromocriptine's activity as a partial D1 antagonist and mixed agonist-antagonist at D2 receptors (Lieberman and Goldstein, 1985; Tan and Jankovic, 2001). What has been shown consistently is that bromocriptine requires DA in order to produce any behavioral effects (Jackson et al., 1988), and alterations in DA concentration effectively alter bromocriptine's activity at pre versus post-synaptic D2 receptors (Maruya et al., 2003). Furthermore, a single administration of bromocriptine can cause an alteration in D2 receptor binding for further treatments (Jackson et al., 1988). These are important caveats to consider in TBI research evaluating bromocriptine as a potential treatment strategy.

5.3.2. Bromocriptine in the clinic

In humans bromocriptine has an oral availability of approximately 30–40% and reaches peak levels about 1–2 h after administration. Following discontinuation, bromocriptine remains in the system for up 12 h. Low dose treatments for PD range from 5 to 30 mg/day while high dose treatments for more advanced PD are within 31–100 mg/day (Lieberman and Goldstein, 1985; Deleu et al., 2002). Bromocriptine is less well studied in clinical research compared to AMH and MPD (Table 3). Past case reports (Ben Smail et al., 2006; Karli et al., 1999) showed improvements in executive function after administering bromocriptine. McDowell et al. (1998) also demonstrated improvements in executive function with a single 2.5 mg bromocriptine administration. Improvements in digit span, list learning, and motivation in bromocriptine treated patients (maximum of 10 mg/day) that persisted for at least two weeks after bromocriptine withdrawal were also reported (Powell et al., 1996). Bromocriptine did not appear to be effective in addressing moderate to severe TBI patients’ attentional difficulties during a postacute phase of recovery at a dose of 5 mg twice daily (Whyte et al., 2008). However, the study employed a relatively high dose of steady state bromocriptine at 10 mg/day for a more prolonged treatment timeline than previously studied in TBI (Whyte et al., 2008). It may be that the dosing of bromocriptine in head trauma patients needs to be specifically titrated given DAergic alterations caused by TBI. It is also possible that higher doses of bromocriptine for prolonged periods negatively impacts DA kinetics in this patient population.

Table 3
Cognitive outcomes with the use of bromocriptine in traumatic brain injury.

5.3.3. Bromocriptine in animal models

In rats, plasma concentrations of bromocriptine peak at 15–30 min with delayed maximal D2 binding occurring for a period of up to 3 h (Maurer et al., 1983; Atsumi et al., 2003). Dosing for rats is generally lower than that for humans and is based on the findings from behavioral studies showing that higher doses (10–40 mg/kg) induce motor activation in normal rats (Jackson et al., 1988; Brannan et al., 1993). Rats receiving delayed (i.e., 24 h post-injury) and chronic (i.e., daily for 18 days) pharmacological treatment with bromocriptine (5 mg/kg) exhibited both enhanced WM and acquisition of spatial learning in a MWM task (Kline et al., 2002). In a follow up study, Kline et al. (2004) demonstrated that bromocriptine-treated rats exhibited enhanced spatial learning as they were more adept at locating a hidden platform in a MWM task and also displayed increased hippocampal neuronal protection following TBI compared to vehicle-treated controls. Furthermore, the data showed that bromocriptine attenuated TBI-induced oxidative stress (Kline et al., 2004).

5.4. Other DAergic drugs

The administration of selegiline (l-deprenyl) once daily for seven days beginning 24 h following FP injury has been reported to improve cognitive function in the MWM and enhance neuroplasticity (Zhu et al., 2000). l-Deprenyl is used to enhance the action of DA by inhibiting its main catabolic enzyme in the brain, monoamine oxidase-B. Additionally, Newburn and Newburn (2005) showed that selegiline has potential clinical benefits in the treatment of post-TBI apathy.

Atomoxetine administered at a dose of 1 mg/kg one day following lateral fluid percussion injury in rats showed improvement in Morris water maze performance compared to vehicle (Reid and Hamm, 2008). Atomoxetine is typically used as a non-stimulant drug for treatment of ADHD. While its mechanism of action is predominantly through inhibition of the NE transporter it has been shown to increase extracellular DA in the PFC (Bymaster et al., 2002).

5.5. The negative impact of DA antagonists following TBI

Antipsychotic drugs, in particular haloperidol and risperidone, have been administered to TBI patients to treat agitation and psychotic symptoms that may be related to the injury. Unfortunately there are very few clinical studies on cognitive effects in TBI patients following antipsyhcotic administration, which has limited conclusions on potential consequences of antipsychotic use in TBI populations (Elovic et al., 2008). However, animal studies have demonstrated negative consequences of antipsychotic administration following TBI, in particular with administration of typical antipsychotics.

Haloperidol and risperidone have multiple CNS effects, but one of the predominant effects is a strong central DA receptor inhibition that can produce akinesia and pseudoparkinsonism. Given this profound DAergic component of these antipsychotics, the question about what effect they might have on the recovery process after TBI is important. Animal models have demonstrated that antipsychotics impair the recovery process and in some instances exacerbate the TBI-induced behavioral deficits. For instance, Feeney and colleagues demonstrated that even a single administration of haloperidol provided after TBI to adult rodents delayed motor recovery (Feeney et al., 1982). Moreover, administration of haloperidol after the rats were recovered, as indicated by normal beam-walking, led to a reinstatement of the deficits (Feeney et al., 1982). Similar findings were reported by Goldstein and Bullman (2002). Other studies have shown that antipsychotic drugs after brain trauma not only impair motor recovery, but also cognitive function. Wilson et al. (2003) showed that haloperidol led to slower acquisition of spatial learning in a water maze task. Interestingly there was no impairment noted with the administration of the atypical antipsychotic olanzapine following TBI by Wilson et al. (2003). One possible explanation is the relatively low activity of olanzapine at the D2 receptor relative to haloperidol (Tauscher et al., 2004).

Recent studies from our laboratory have demonstrated that prolonged exposure to the typical and atypical antipsychotics, haloperidol and risperidone, respectively, after TBI impairs motor recovery and hinders the acquisition of spatial learning and memory retention (Kline et al., 2007a, 2008; Hoffman et al., 2008a). Risperidone and haloperidol also impaired performance in uninjured controls (Hoffman et al., 2008a,b; Kline et al., 2008). Haloperidol has also been shown to block the positive benefits of AMPH treatment (Hovda and Feeney, 1985). Interestingly, while single or multiple low doses of risperidone and haloperidol appear to be innocuous to recovery after TBI, chronic high-dose treatments are uniformly detrimental (Kline et al., 2007a,b).

A number of studies have also shown positive improvements in WM and spatial memory with both early (Tang et al., 1997) and late (Kobori and Dash, 2006) administration of DA antagonists. For example, Kobori and Dash (2006) demonstrated that a single administration of the DA D1 antagonist (SCH23390) at 14 days post-injury in rats improved WM for up to a week. Tang et al. (1997) showed an improvement in functional recovery with D2 receptor specific antagonists given immediately post-injury and a synergistic effect when combined with D1 receptor antagonism in mice. Given that both haloperidol and risperidone have a higher affinity for the D2 receptors (Cohen, 1994; Reimold et al., 2007), it may be that specific blockade of D2 receptors is the event most associated with negative outcomes when antagonized at later time-points. This is not unreasonable as it has been shown that D1 receptor activation is also important for inflammatory and immunological responses including phosphoinositide hydrolysis and arachidonic acid release as discussed previously in this review. A potential explanation is that inhibition of D1 receptors after TBI beneficially affects the injury response while DA agonists at later time-points provide cognitive benefits.

6. Future avenues of DA research in TBI: DA regulation, signaling, and structural plasticity

There remains a significant amount of work in TBI research to explore completely the realities and consequences of DAergic dysfunction after TBI. Are the observed alterations in DAT and TH protein levels a result of injury and ongoing biochemical damage or are they a response to initial changes in DA levels in the cortex and subcortical layers? Furthermore, although no overt cellular damage has been identified within nigrostriatal and mesocortical DAergic pathways, there remains the possibility of axonal disruptions and biochemical alterations. Interestingly, in the PD literature it has been suggested that there exist subtle changes in oxidative stress related to DA signaling within the SN occurring prior to significant cell loss.

Animal models of TBI consistently produce widespread excitotoxic damage and increased amounts of oxidative stress in a number of different brain regions (Palmer et al., 1993; Rao et al., 1999). DA is known to possess excitotoxic properties (Olney et al., 1990), and DAergic fibers have been shown to modulate striatal glutamatergic excitotoxicity (Chapman et al., 1989; Filloux and Wamsley, 1991). The initial increases in DA observed post-TBI may precipitate excitotoxic disruption and oxidative damage to DAergic cellular function that leads to the observed alterations in DA kinetics and decreased evoked DA release at later time-points.

Observed changes in DAT expression in TBI also raise an intriguing set of considerations. As discussed in the section concerning DAT activity, the DAT is regulated by a number of genetic factors and shows variation in activity and expression with both age and gender. Alterations in DAT expression can alter the kinetics of DA release as demonstrated in DAT knockdown models (Zhuang et al., 2001), as can changes in DAT cellular localization (Pristupa et al., 1998). Decreases in evoked DA overflow Vmax following CCI may be explained by either changes in expression or changes in membrane bound DAT associated with DAT trafficking (Wagner et al., 2005, 2009). Given that a number of the current DA receptor agonist therapies act through a DAT mediated mechanism, it is necessary to fully understand the role of DAT changes in TBI in order to provide efficacious DA therapies.

In addition to documented biochemical alterations in DA signaling following TBI, there remains the possibility of structural changes. TBI is known to cause diffuse white matter injury and significant axonal disruptions throughout the CNS (Smith et al., 2003). However, it remains unclear if similar effects can be observed within DAergic systems. Increases in TH staining in both the PFC and striatum may represent regrowth of DAergic fibers that occur as a consequence of DA synapse or axonal pathology that occurred acutely following TBI.

Regrowth or collateral sprouting of catecholaminergic axons has already been demonstrated in experimentally induced lesions of adult CNS neurons (Katzman et al., 1971; Gilad and Reis, 1979; Fritschy and Grzanna, 1992). Moreover, in both PD patients and animals with experimental Parkinsonism, neural transplants or the supply of neurotrophic factors may promote regrowth of DAergic fibers in the striatum and reverse lesion-induced behavioral deficits (Kordower et al., 1991; Kopin, 1993; Tomac et al., 1995). The occurrence of spontaneous regrowth of DAergic fibers after partial nigrostriatal denervation has already been suggested (Onn et al., 1986) and a long-term increase in the amount of striatal TH has been observed after 6-hydroxydopamine injection in the SN pars compacta (Pasinetti et al., 1991; Blanchard et al., 1995). It is thus possible that the TBI-induced expression of TH in the nigrostriatal system might share similar mechanisms. However, further studies evaluating DA turnover, TH activity, DOPAC/DA and DA/TH in the nigrostriatal system after TBI are needed to confirm whether there is such a compensatory mechanism after TBI.

Furthermore the striatum is a heterogeneous region and a varied profile of DA kinetics has been reported within different areas of the dorsal (caudate and putamen) and ventral (NAcc) striatum (May and Wightman, 1989a,b; Bergstrom et al., 2001). It is possible that caudate subregions are differentially affected by CCI. Differences may also exist in damage to the NAcc core versus the shell. Subregion damage could also differentially affect PFC function given the regionally specific cortical connections that exist within basal ganglia anatomy (Grahn et al., 2009). While many studies have also identified alterations in the PFC associated with TBI, future studies will likely need to evaluate DA neurotransmission in multiple DA regions outside the caudate putamen, as well as subregions within the caudate to characterize fully the effects of trauma on these heterogeneous structures.

DAergic dysfunction after TBI may not be limited to neurotransmitter release, concentration, and metabolism. Preliminary research in our group suggests that TBI can cause alterations in DARPP-32 phosphorylation altering a number of important intracellular signaling molecules. The importance of DARPP-32 to DAergic synaptic plasticity and modulation of other neurotransmitter systems has broad reaching implications for medium spiny neuron function in the striatum and represents another possible level of DA dysfunction following TBI.

Alterations in receptor expression also remain an area where further research is necessary. While studies to date have not demonstrated any overt alteration in DA receptors, there are a number of areas that require further consideration. D1 and D2 receptors depend upon a complex interplay with other receptor systems within the striatum and other DA systems. In particular, recent research in PD has identified a series of heterodimeric complexes of the D2 receptor with other neurotransmitter receptor systems including the adenosine A2a and Mglut5 receptors. It has been proposed in PD research that A2a antagonists can augment DAergic treatments. Interestingly, both A2a and Mglut5 receptors have also been shown to play heavily into DA's role on striatal plasticity and intracellular calcium signaling.

7. Summary

This review has sought to summarize the evidence that supports a DAergic hypothesis of cognitive dysfunction after TBI and provide a context for the use of DA targeted therapies during patient rehabilitation. A concise overview of DA's role in cognitive deficits relevant to TBI, the use of DA therapies in clinical populations to benefit cognitive recovery, and an overview of the alterations observed post-TBI in DA neurotransmission as determined by both clinical and experimental studies allows the construction of an early schematic for the effect of TBI on DA to help guide future endeavors.

Given that TBI causes damage to areas known to be involved in DAergic processing and that research in other disease states have established that DA is important for cognitive outcomes, it is logical that TBI should produce clinical outcomes similar to what is observed in other disorders of DAergic function. Indeed, the clinical picture of behavioral and cognitive dysfunctions after TBI shares a number of commonalities with other disorders of DA dysfunction, such as PD and HD.

To date, clinical studies have consistently demonstrated that pharmacotherapies that enhance DA post-TBI are beneficial to memory, attention, and executive function. However, clinical studies examining DAergic therapies have a number of limitations. Small patient populations, variations in treatment protocols, lack of proper controls including the absence of randomized clinical trials, and a poor definition of TBI make it difficult to identify who would benefit the most from DA enhancement therapy and the proper time-course of therapeutic intervention.

Ongoing animal and clinical studies have better characterized DAergic dysfunction following TBI, which is still difficult due to the diffuse nature and variable presentation of TBI. However, studies in both animals and humans have identified a series of temporally specific alterations in DA neurotransmission that occur after TBI (Fig. 3). An acute hyperactive phase, characterized by increases in tissue DA levels and an initial increase in D1 receptors, is subsequently followed by hypofunction in DA signaling characterized by decreases in evoked DA overflow and alterations in both DAT and TH expression. Animal studies demonstrating the benefit of D1 antagonists and D2 agonists would suggest that the critical event in DAergic dysfunction following TBI is related primarily to signaling through the DA D2 receptor population.

Understanding the temporal alterations in DA following TBI and the mechanism of dysfunction at a cellular and systems level will allow DAergic therapies to be better tailored to specifically address the character of dysfunction in TBI populations during recovery. Furthermore, given the importance of genetic differences in DA kinetics and the role of gender in DA signaling, it is important to utilize animal models of injury to better understand how these factors affect potential treatments. Doing so will help answer long standing questions in TBI rehabilitation in how to best optimize neuropharmacology strategies. Understanding the role of DA in cognitive recovery following TBI also adds another layer of consideration to the use of acute phase medicines that may affect CNS DA systems.

8. Conclusions

Clinical studies coupled with animal research have clearly demonstrated that DA targeted therapies represent an important clinical option in the treatment of memory, learning, and executive function deficiencies that persist following a TBI. However, clinical studies have so far failed to identify the most beneficial dose and time period of administration for DAergic therapy. Furthermore, conclusions from DA enhancement therapeutic trials are further complicated by the nature of TBI itself. TBI is a complex disease with multiple primary and secondary etiologies. Poor stratification of patients, often based almost exclusively upon Glascow coma scale, has made it difficult to identify the patient populations most likely to benefit from targeted DA therapy. Even considering these limitations there remains enough evidence to support further studies into the role of DA in persistent deficits following TBI. We propose three goals for ongoing animal and clinical studies in DAergic signaling following TBI.

First, there needs to be a coherent understanding of temporal alterations in DA signaling that occurs following TBI. Efforts should be made to understand what occurs acutely, during recovery, and chronically. A better understanding of molecular events will allow clinicians and researchers to effectively analyze clinical successes and, more importantly, clinical failures. Without this basic understanding is difficult to ascertain why some promising DAergic therapies fail in TBI patients. Second, research in animal models is necessary to examine dosing, timecourse of administration, and possible combination therapies. The natural dichotomy in DA anatomy provided by D1 versus D2 receptor populations and other relevant neurotransmitter systems, provides a real opportunity to utilize adjunct therapies in addition to direct DA enhancing strategies. Third, clinical research in TBI patients must make efforts to stratify results based upon gender, genetic markers, in particular DAT expression profiles, and injury profiles. Doing so we will allow clinicians to identify which patients will be the most likely to benefit from DAergic therapy.

Abbreviations

AC
adenylate cyclase
ADHD
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
AMH
amantadine hydrochloride
AMPH
amphetamine
BDNF
brain derived neurotrophic factor
cAMP
cyclic adenosine monophosphate
CCI
controlled cortical impact
CNS
central nervous system
CREB
cAMP responsive element binding
CSF
cerebrospinal fluid
DA
dopamine
DARPP-32
dopamine and cAMP-regulated phosphoprotein mKDa 32
DAT
dopamine transporter
DLPFC
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
DOPAC
dihydroxyphenylacetic acid
DRS
disability rating scale
ERK
extracellular regulated kinase
FP
fluid percussion
GABA
gamma amino-butyric acid
HD
Huntington's disease
LTD
long-term depression
LTP
long-term potentiation
MAPK
mitogen activated protein kinase
Mglut-5
metabotropic glutamate receptor-5
MPD
methylphenidate
MWM
Morris water maze
NAcc
nucleus accumbens
NE
norepinephrine
NMDA
N-methyl-d-aspartic acid
NTF
neurotrauma foundation
PD
Parkinson's disease
PET
positron emission tomography
PFC
prefrontal cortex
PKA
protein kinase A
PKC
protein kinase C
PP1
protein phosphatase-1
PP2A
protein phosphatase 2A
PP2B
protein phosphatase 2B (calcineurin)
SPECT
single photon emission computed tomography
SN
substantia nigra
T34
threonine-34
T75
threonine-75
TBI
traumatic brain injury
TH
tyrosine hydroxylase
VTA
ventral tegmental area
WM
working memory

References

  • Abi-Dargham A, Moore H. Prefrontal DA transmission at D1 receptors and the pathology of schizophrenia. Neuroscientist. 2003;9:404–416. [PubMed]
  • Adair JC, Barrett AM. Spatial neglect: clinical and neuroscience review: a wealth of information on the poverty of spatial attention. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008;1142:21–43. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Aggleton JP, Brown MW. Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal-anterior thalamic axis. Behav. Brain Sci. 1999;22:425–444. [PubMed]
  • Alexander GE, Crutcher MD. Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing. Trends Neurosci. 1990;13:266–271. [PubMed]
  • Allen RM. Role of amantadine in the management of neuroleptic-induced extrapyramidal syndromes: overview and pharmacology. Clin. Neuropharmacol. 1983;6(Suppl 1):S64–S73. [PubMed]
  • Aoki FY, Sitar DS. Clinical pharmacokinetics of amantadine hydrochloride. Clin. Pharmacokinet. 1988;14:35–51. [PubMed]
  • Arciniegas DB, Topkoff J, Silver JM. Neuropsychiatric aspects of traumatic brain injury. Curr. Treat. Opt. Neurol. 2000;2:169–186. [PubMed]
  • Arnsten AF, Cai JX, Murphy BL, Goldman-Rakic PS. Dopamine D1 receptor mechanisms in the cognitive performance of young adult and aged monkeys. Psychopharmacology. 1994;116:143–151. [PubMed]
  • Atsumi M, Kawakami J, Sugiyama E, Kotaki H, Sawada Y, Sato H, Yamada Y, Iga T. Pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic analyses, based on dopamine D2-receptor occupancy of bromocriptine, of bromocriptine-induced contralateral rotations in unilaterally 6-OHDA-lesioned rats. Synapse. 2003;50:110–116. [PubMed]
  • Backman L, Robins-Wahlin TB, Lundin A, Ginovart N, Farde L. Cognitive deficits in Huntington's disease are predicted by dopaminergic PET markers and brain volumes. Brain. 1997;120:2207–2217. [PubMed]
  • Baddeley A. Working memory. Science. 1992;255:556–559. [PubMed]
  • Baddeley A, Logie R, Bressi S, Della Sala S, Spinnler H. Dementia and working memory. Q. J. Exp. Psych. A: Hum. Exp. Psychol. 1986;38:603–618. [PubMed]
  • Bak IJ, Hassler R, Kim JS, Kataoka K. Amantadine actions on acetylcholine and GABA in striatum and substantia nigra of rat in relation to behavioral changes. J. Neural Transm. 1972;33:45–61. [PubMed]
  • Baldessarini RJ, Lipinski JF, Chace KV. Effects of amantadine hydrochloride on catecholamine metabolism in the brain of the rat. Biochem. Pharmacol. 1972;21:77–87. [PubMed]
  • Baldo BA, Kelley AE. Discrete neurochemical coding of distinguishable motivational processes: insights from nucleus accumbens control of feeding. Psychopharmacology. 2007;191:439–459. [PubMed]
  • Bannon MJ, Poosch MS, Xia Y, Goebel DJ, Cassin B, Kapatos G. Dopamine transporter mRNA content in human substantia nigra decreases precipitously with age. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 1992;89:7095–7099. [PubMed]
  • Baron JC, Comar D, Zarifian E, Agid Y, Crouzel C, Loo H, Deniker P, Kellershohn C. Dopaminergic receptor sites in human brain: positron emission tomography. Neurology. 1985;35:16–24. [PubMed]
  • Ben Smail D, Samuel C, Rouy-Thenaisy K, Regnault J, Azouvi P. Bromocriptine in traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 2006;20:111–115. [PubMed]
  • Bergstrom BP, Schertz KE, Weirick T, Nafziger B, Takacs SA, Lopes KO, Massa KJ, Walker QD, Garris PA. Partial, graded losses of dopamine terminals in the rat caudate-putamen: an animal model for the study of compensatory adaptation in preclinical parkinsonism. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2001;106:15–28. [PubMed]
  • Berridge CW. Neural substrates of psychostimulant-induced arousal. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:2332–2340. [PubMed]
  • Bertorello AM, Hopfield JF, Aperia A, Greengard P. Inhibition by dopamine of (Na(+)+K+)ATPase activity in neostriatal neurons through D1 and D2 dopamine receptor synergism. Nature. 1990;347:386–388. [PubMed]
  • Binder LM. Persisting symptoms after mild head injury: a review of the postconcussive syndrome. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1986;8:323–346. [PubMed]
  • Binder LM. Neurobehavioral recovery after mild head injury. J. Neurosurg. 1987;67:785–787. [PubMed]
  • Binder LM. A review of mild head trauma. Part II: Clinical implications. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1997;19:432–457. [PubMed]
  • Binder LM, Rohling ML, Larrabee GJ. A review of mild head trauma. Part I: Meta-analytic review of neuropsychological studies. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1997;19:421–431. [PubMed]
  • Blanchard V, Chritin M, Vyas S, Savasta M, Feuerstein C, Agid Y, Javoy-Agid F, Raisman-Vozari R. Long-term induction of tyrosine hydroxylase expression: compensatory response to partial degeneration of the dopaminergic nigrostriatal system in the rat brain. J. Neurochem. 1995;64:1669–1679. [PubMed]
  • Blank T, Nijholt I, Teichert U, Kugler H, Behrsing H, Fienberg A, Greengard P, Spiess J. The phosphoprotein DARPP-32 mediates cAMP-dependent potentiation of striatal N-methyl-d-aspartate responses. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 1997;94:14859–14864. [PubMed]
  • Borgkvist A, Usiello A, Greengard P, Fisone G. Activation of the cAMP/PKA/DARPP-32 signaling pathway is required for morphine psychomotor stimulation but not for morphine reward. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:1995–2003. [PubMed]
  • Borland LM, Michael AC. Voltammetric study of the control of striatal dopamine release by glutamate. J. Neurochem. 2004;91:220–229. [PubMed]
  • Bosse R, Rivest R, Di Paolo T. Ovariectomy and estradiol treatment affect the dopamine transporter and its gene expression in the rat brain. Brain Res. Mol. Brain Res. 1997;46:343–346. [PubMed]
  • Boyeson MG, Feeney DM. Intraventricular norepinephrine facilitates motor recovery following sensorimotor cortex injury. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1990;35:497–501. [PubMed]
  • Bramlett HM, Dietrich WD. Quantitative structural changes in white and gray matter 1 year following traumatic brain injury in rats. Acta Neuropathol. 2002;103:607–614. [PubMed]
  • Brandt J, Folstein SE, Folstein MF. Differential cognitive impairment in Alzheimer's disease and Huntington's disease. Ann. Neurol. 1988;23:555–561. [PubMed]
  • Brannan T, Martinez-Tica J, Di Rocco A, Yahr MD. Low and high dose bromocriptine have different effects on striatal dopamine release: an in vivo study. J. Neural Transm. Park. Dis. Dement. Sect. 1993;6:81–87. [PubMed]
  • Brennan AR, Arnsten AF. Neuronal mechanisms underlying attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: the influence of arousal on prefrontal cortical function. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008;1129:236–245. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Broussolle E, Dentresangle C, Landais P, Garcia-Larrea L, Pollak P, Croisile B, Hibert O, Bonnefoi F, Galy G, Froment JC, Comar D. The relation of putamen and caudate nucleus 18F-Dopa uptake to motor and cognitive performances in Parkinson's disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 1999;166:141–151. [PubMed]
  • Brown F, Redfern PH. Studies on the mechanism of action of amantadine. Br. J. Pharmacol. 1976;58:561–567. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Bruck A, Portin R, Lindell A, Laihinen A, Bergman J, Haaparanta M, Solin O, Rinne JO. Positron emission tomography shows that impaired frontal lobe functioning in parkinson's disease is related to dopaminergic hypofunction in the caudate nucleus. Neurosci. Lett. 2001;311:81–84. [PubMed]
  • Bruns J, Jr., Hauser WA. The epidemiology of traumatic brain injury: a review. Epilepsia. 2003;44(Suppl 10):2–10. [PubMed]
  • Buckley MJ. The role of the perirhinal cortex and hippocampus in learning, memory, and perception. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2005;58:246–268. [PubMed]
  • Bunzow JR, Van Tol HH, Grandy DK, Albert P, Salon J, Christie M, Machida CA, Neve KA, Civelli O. Cloning and expression of a rat D2 dopamine receptor cDNA. Nature. 1988;336:783–787. [PubMed]
  • Butefisch CM, Davis BC, Sawaki L, Waldvogel D, Classen J, Kopylev L, Cohen LG. Modulation of use-dependent plasticity by d-amphetamine. Ann. Neurol. 2002;51:59–68. [PubMed]
  • Bymaster FP, Katner JS, Nelson DL, Hemrick-Luecke SK, Threlkeld PG, Heiligenstein JH, Morin SM, Gehlert DR, Perry KW. Atomoxetine increases extracellular levels of norepinephrine and dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rat: a potential mechanism for efficacy in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:699–711. [PubMed]
  • Cai JX, Arnsten AF. Dose-dependent effects of the dopamine D1 receptor agonists A77636 or SKF81297 on spatial working memory in aged monkeys. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 1997;283:183–189. [PubMed]
  • Caillé I, Dumartin B, Bloch B. Ultrastructural localization of D1 dopamine receptor immunoreactivity in rat striatonigral neurons and its relation with dopaminergic innervation. Brain Res. 1996;730:17–31. [PubMed]
  • Calabresi P, Gubellini P, Centonze D, Picconi B, Bernardi G, Chergui K, Svenningsson P, Fienberg AA, Greengard P. Dopamine and cAMP-regulated phosphoprotein 32 kDa controls both striatal long-term depression and long-term potentiation, opposing forms of synaptic plasticity. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:8443–8451. [PubMed]
  • Calabresi P, Picconi B, Tozzi A, Di Filippo M. Dopamine-mediated regulation of corticostriatal synaptic plasticity. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30:211–219. [PubMed]
  • Canals M, Marcellino D, Fanelli F, Ciruela F, de Benedetti P, Goldberg SR, Neve K, Fuxe K, Agnati LF, Woods AS, Ferre S, Lluis C, Bouvier M, Franco R. Adenosine A2A-dopamine D2 receptor-receptor heteromerization: qualitative and quantitative assessment by fluorescence and bioluminescence energy transfer. J. Biol. Chem. 2003;278:46741–46749. [PubMed]
  • Carelli RM. The nucleus accumbens and reward: neurophysiological investigations in behaving animals. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 2002;1:281–296. [PubMed]
  • Castner SA, Goldman-Rakic PS. Enhancement of working memory in aged monkeys by a sensitizing regimen of dopamine D1 receptor stimulation. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:1446–1450. [PubMed]
  • Castner SA, Williams GV, Goldman-Rakic PS. Reversal of antipsychotic-induced working memory deficits by short-term dopamine D1 receptor stimulation. Science. 2000;287:2020–2022. [PubMed]
  • Centonze D, Grande C, Saulle E, Martin AB, Gubellini P, Pavon N, Pisani A, Bernardi G, Moratalla R, Calabresi P. Distinct roles of D1 and D5 dopamine receptors in motor activity and striatal synaptic plasticity. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:8506–8512. [PubMed]
  • Centonze D, Gubellini P, Picconi B, Calabresi P, Giacomini P, Bernardi G. Unilateral dopamine denervation blocks corticostriatal LTP. J. Neurophysiol. 1999;82:3575–3579. [PubMed]
  • Chandler MC, Barnhill JL, Gualtieri CT. Amantadine for the agitated head-injury patient. Brain Inj. 1988;2:309–311. [PubMed]
  • Chapman AG, Durmuller N, Lees GJ, Meldrum BS. Excitotoxicity of NMDA and kainic acid is modulated by nigrostriatal dopaminergic fibres. Neurosci. Lett. 1989;107:256–260. [PubMed]
  • Cheng JP, Aslam HA, Hoffman AN, Zafonte RD, Kline AE. The neurobehavioral benefit conferred by a single systemic administration of 8-OH-DPAT after brain trauma is confined to a narrow therapeutic window. Neurosci. Lett. 2007;416:165–168. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Cheng JP, Hoffman AN, Zafonte RD, Kline AE. A delayed and chronic treatment regimen with the 5-HT1A receptor agonist 8-OH-DPAT after cortical impact injury facilitates motor recovery and acquisition of spatial learning. Behav. Brain Res. 2008;194:79–85. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Christoffersen GR, Simonyi A, Schatman TR, Clausen B, Clement D, Bjerre VK, Mark LT, Reinholdt M, Schmith-Rasmussen K, Zink LV. MGlu5 antagonism impairs exploration and memory of spatial and non-spatial stimuli in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2008;191:235–245. [PubMed]
  • Chudasama Y, Nathwani F, Robbins TW. d-Amphetamine remediates attentional performance in rats with dorsal prefrontal lesions. Behav. Brain Res. 2005;158:97–107. [PubMed]
  • Chudasama Y, Robbins TW. Functions of frontostriatal systems in cognition: comparative neuropsychopharmacological studies in rats, monkeys and humans. Biol. Psychol. 2006;73:19–38. [PubMed]
  • Ciliax BJ, Drash GW, Staley JK, Haber S, Mobley CJ, Miller GW, Mufson EJ, Mash DC, Levey AI. Immunocytochemical localization of the dopamine transporter in human brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 1999;409:38–56. [PubMed]
  • Coccurello R, Adriani W, Oliverio A, Mele A. Effect of intra-accumbens dopamine receptor agents on reactivity to spatial and non-spatial changes in mice. Psychopharmacology. 2000;152:189–199. [PubMed]
  • Cohen LJ. Risperidone. Pharmacotherapy. 1994;14:253–265. [PubMed]
  • Cohen JD, Servan-Schreiber D. Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychol. Rev. 1992;99:45–77. [PubMed]
  • Cohen RA, Fisher M. Amantadine treatment of fatigue associated with multiple sclerosis. Arch. Neurol. 1989;46:676–680. [PubMed]
  • Colicos MA, Dixon CE, Dash PK. Delayed, selective neuronal death following experimental cortical impact injury in rats: possible role in memory deficits. Brain Res. 1996;739:111–119. [PubMed]
  • Cools AR, Ellenbroek B, Heeren D, Lubbers L. Use of high and low responders to novelty in rat studies on the role of the ventral striatum in radial maze performance: effects of intra-accumbens injections of sulpiride. Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 1993;71:335–342. [PubMed]
  • Cooper JA, Sagar HJ, Jordan N, Harvey NS, Sullivan EV. Cognitive impairment in early, untreated Parkinson's disease and its relationship to motor disability. Brain. 1991;114:2095–2122. [PubMed]
  • Cote TE, Frey EA, Grewe CW, Kebabian JW. Evidence that the D-2 dopamine receptor in the intermediate lobe of the rat pituitary gland is associated with an inhibitory guanyl nucleotide component. J. Neural Transm. 1983;18:139–147. [PubMed]
  • Crosson BHK. Subcortical functions in cognition: toward a consensus. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2003;9:1027–1030.
  • Dal Toso R, Sommer B, Ewert M, Herb A, Pritchett DB, Bach A, Shivers BD, Seeburg PH. The dopamine D2 receptor: two molecular forms generated by alternative splicing. EMBO J. 1989;8:4025–4034. [PubMed]
  • Daniels GM, Amara SG. Regulated trafficking of the human dopamine transporter. Clathrin-mediated internalization and lysosomal degradation in response to phorbol esters. J. Biol. Chem. 1999;274:35794–35801. [PubMed]
  • Dearry A, Gingrich JA, Falardeau P, Fremeau RT, Jr., Bates MD, Caron MG. Molecular cloning and expression of the gene for a human D1 dopamine receptor. Nature. 1990;347:72–76. [PubMed]
  • DeKosky ST, Kochanek PM, Clark RS, Ciallella JR, Dixon CE. Secondary injury after head trauma: subacute and long-term mechanisms. Semin. Clin. Neuropsychiatry. 1998;3:176–185. [PubMed]
  • Deleu D, Northway MG, Hanssens Y. Clinical pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic properties of drugs used in the treatment of Parkinson's disease. Clin. Pharmacokinet. 2002;41:261–309. [PubMed]
  • Dhillon HS, Dose JM, Prasad RM. Amphetamine administration improves neurochemical outcome of lateral fluid percussion brain injury in the rat. 1998. pp. 231–237. [PubMed]
  • Di Chiara G, Bassareo V. Reward system and addiction: what dopamine does and doesn't do. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 2007;7:69–76. [PubMed]
  • Diaz-Cabiale Z, Vivo M, Del Arco A, O'Connor WT, Harte MK, Muller CE, Martinez E, Popoli P, Fuxe K, Ferre S. Metabotropic glutamate mGlu5 receptor-mediated modulation of the ventral striopallidal GABA pathway in rats. Interactions with adenosine A(2A) and dopamine D(2) receptors. Neurosci. Lett. 2002;324:154–158. [PubMed]
  • Dietrich WD, Alonso O, Halley M. Early microvascular and neuronal consequences of traumatic brain injury: a light and electron microscopic study in rats. J. Neurotrauma. 1994;11:289–301. [PubMed]
  • Ding Y, Yao B, Lai Q, McAllister JP. Impaired motor learning and diffuse axonal damage in motor and visual systems of the rat following traumatic brain injury. Neurol. Res. 2001;23:193–202. [PubMed]
  • Divac I, Rosvold HE, Szwarcbart MK. Behavioral effects of selective ablation of the caudate nucleus. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1967;63:184–190. [PubMed]
  • Dixon CE, Bao J, Long DA, Hayes RL. Reduced evoked release of acetylcholine in the rodent hippocampus following traumatic brain injury. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1996;53:679–686. [PubMed]
  • Dixon CE, Clifton GL, Lighthall JW, Yaghmai AA, Hayes RL. A controlled cortical impact model of traumatic brain injury in the rat. J. Neurosci. Methods. 1991;39:253–262. [PubMed]
  • Dixon CE, Kraus MF, Kline AE, Ma X, Yan HQ, Griffith RG, Wolfson BM, Marion DW. Amantadine improves water maze performance without affecting motor behavior following traumatic brain injury in rats. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 1999;14:285–294. [PubMed]
  • Dixon CE, Lyeth BG, Povlishock JT, Findling RL, Hamm RJ, Marmarou A, Young HF, Hayes RL. A fluid percussion model of experimental brain injury in the rat. J. Neurosurg. 1987;67:110–119. [PubMed]
  • Dixon CE, Ma X, Marion DW. Effects of CDP-choline treatment on neurobehavioral deficits after TBI and on hippocampal and neocortical acetylcholine release. J. Neurotrauma. 1997;14:161–169. [PubMed]
  • Donnemiller E, Brenneis C, Wissel J, Scherfler C, Poewe W, Riccabona G, Wenning GK. Impaired dopaminergic neurotransmission in patients with traumatic brain injury: a SPECT study using 123I-beta-CIT and 123I-IBZM. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. 2000;27:1410–1414. [PubMed]
  • Doolen S, Zahniser NR. Protein tyrosine kinase inhibitors alter human dopamine transporter activity in Xenopus oocytes. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 2001;296:931–938. [PubMed]
  • Draper K, Ponsford J. Cognitive functioning ten years following traumatic brain injury and rehabilitation. Neuropsychology. 2008;22:618–625. [PubMed]
  • Duchesne N, Soucy JP, Masson H, Chouinard S, Bedard MA. Cognitive deficits and striatal dopaminergic denervation in Parkinson's disease: a single photon emission computed tomography study using 123iodine-beta-CIT in patients on and off levodopa. Clin. Neuropharmacol. 2002;25:216–224. [PubMed]
  • Dudman JT, Eaton ME, Rajadhyaksha A, Macias W, Taher M, Barczak A, Kameyama K, Huganir R, Konradi C. Dopamine d1 receptors mediate CREB phosphorylation via phosphorylation of the NMDA receptor at Ser897-NR1. J. Neurochem. 2003;87:922–934. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Dunn-Meynell A, Pan S, Levin BE. Focal traumatic brain injury causes widespread reductions in rat brain norepinephrine turnover from 6 to 24 h. Brain Res. 1994;660:88–95. [PubMed]
  • Dunn-Meynell AA, Levin BE. Histological markers of neuronal, axonal and astrocytic changes after lateral rigid impact traumatic brain injury. Brain Res. 1997;761:25–41. [PubMed]
  • Dyer KF, Bell R, McCann J, Rauch R. Aggression after traumatic brain injury: analysing socially desirable responses and the nature of aggressive traits. Brain Inj. 2006;20:1163–1173. [PubMed]
  • El-Ghundi M, Fletcher PJ, Drago J, Sibley DR, O'Dowd BF, George SR. Spatial learning deficit in dopamine D(1) receptor knockout mice. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 1999;383:95–106. [PubMed]
  • El-Ghundi M, O'Dowd BF, George SR. Insights into the role of dopamine receptor systems in learning and memory. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;18:37–66. [PubMed]
  • Elovic EP, Jasey NN, Jr., Eisenberg ME. The use of atypical antipsychotics after traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2008;23:132–135. [PubMed]
  • Enomoto T, Osugi T, Satoh H, McIntosh TK, Nabeshima T. Pre-injury magnesium treatment prevents traumatic brain injury-induced hippocampal ERK activation, neuronal loss, and cognitive dysfunction in the radial-arm maze test. J. Neurotrauma. 2005;22:783–792. [PubMed]
  • Evans RW, Gualtieri CT, Patterson D. Treatment of chronic closed head injury with psychostimulant drugs: a controlled case study and an appropriate evaluation procedure. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1987;175:106–110. [PubMed]
  • Faden AI, Demediuk P, Panter SS, Vink R. The role of excitatory amino acids and NMDA receptors in traumatic brain injury. Science. 1989;244:798–800. [PubMed]
  • Falo MC, Fillmore HL, Reeves TM, Phillips LL. Matrix metalloproteinase-3 expression profile differentiates adaptive and maladaptive synaptic plasticity induced by traumatic brain injury. J. Neurosci. Res. 2006;84:768–781. [PubMed]
  • Falo MC, Reeves TM, Phillips LL. Agrin expression during synaptogenesis induced by traumatic brain injury. J. Neurotrauma. 2008;25:769–783. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Feeney DM, Gonzales A, Law WA. Amphetamine restores locomotor function after motor cortex injury in the rat. Proc. West. Pharmacol. Soc. 1981;24:15–17. [PubMed]
  • Feeney DM, Gonzalez A, Law WA. Amphetamine, haloperidol, and experience interact to affect rate of recovery after motor cortex injury. Science. 1982;217:855–857. [PubMed]
  • Ferré S, Karcz-Kubicha M, Hope BT, Popoli P, Burgueno J, Gutierrez MA, Casadó V, Fuxe K, Goldberg SR, Lluis C, Franco R, Ciruela F. Synergistic interaction between adenosine A2A and glutamate mGlu5 receptors: implications for striatal neuronal function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2002;99:11940–11945. [PubMed]
  • Ferré S, Quiroz C, Woods AS, Cunha R, Popoli P, Ciruela F, Lluis C, Franco R, Azdad K, Schiffmann SN. An update on adenosine A2A-dopamine D2 receptor interactions: implications for the function of G protein-coupled receptors. Curr. Pharm. Des. 2008;14:1468–1474. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Fienberg AA, Hiroi N, Mermelstein PG, Song W, Snyder GL, Nishi A, Cheramy A, O'Callaghan JP, Miller DB, Cole DG, Corbett R, Haile CN, Cooper DC, Onn SP, Grace AA, Ouimet CC, White FJ, Hyman SE, Surmeier DJ, Girault J, Nestler EJ, Greengard P. DARPP-32: regulator of the efficacy of dopaminergic neurotransmission. Science. 1998;281:838–842. [PubMed]
  • File SE. Factors controlling measures of anxiety and responses to novelty in the mouse. Behav. Brain Res. 2001;125:151–157. [PubMed]
  • Filloux F, Wamsley JK. Dopaminergic modulation of excitotoxicity in rat striatum: evidence from nigrostriatal lesions. Synapse. 1991;8:281–288. [PubMed]
  • Fleckenstein AE, Volz TJ, Riddle EL, Gibb JW, Hanson GR. New insights into the mechanism of action of amphetamines. Ann. Rev. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 2007;47:681–698. [PubMed]
  • Floresco SB, Magyar O. Mesocortical dopamine modulation of executive functions: beyond working memory. Psychopharmacology. 2006;188:567–585. [PubMed]
  • Flores-Hernandez J, Cepeda C, Hernandez-Echeagaray E, Calvert CR, Jokel ES, Fienberg AA, Greengard P, Levine MS. Dopamine enhancement of NMDA currents in dissociated medium-sized striatal neurons: role of D1 receptors and DARPP-32. J. Neurophysiol. 2002;88:3010–3020. [PubMed]
  • Flores-Hernandez J, Hernandez S, Snyder GL, Yan Z, Fienberg AA, Moss SJ, Greengard P, Surmeier DJ. D(1) dopamine receptor activation reduces GABA(A) receptor currents in neostriatal neurons through a PKA/DARPP-32/PP1 signaling cascade. J. Neurophysiol. 2000;83:2996–3004. [PubMed]
  • Fontaine A, Azouvi P, Remy P, Bussel B, Samson Y. Functional anatomy of neuropsychological deficits after severe traumatic brain injury. Neurology. 1999;53:1963–1968. [PubMed]
  • Fox GB, Fan L, Levasseur RA, Faden AI. Sustained sensory/motor and cognitive deficits with neuronal apoptosis following controlled cortical impact brain injury in the mouse. J. Neurotrauma. 1998;15:599–614. [PubMed]
  • Fox GB, LeVasseur RA, Faden AI. Behavioral responses of c57BL/6, FVB/N, and 129/SvEMS mouse strains to traumatic brain injury: implications for gene targeting approaches to neurotrauma. J. Neurotrauma. 1999;16:377–389. [PubMed]
  • Frey U, Matthies H, Reymann KG. The effect of dopaminergic D1 receptor blockade during tetanization on the expression of long-term potentiation in the rat ca1 region in vitro. Neurosci. Lett. 1991;129:111–114. [PubMed]
  • Fritschy JM, Grzanna R. Degeneration of rat locus coeruleus neurons is not accompanied by an irreversible loss of ascending projections. Evidence for reestablishment of forebrain innervation by surviving neurons. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1992;648:275–278. [PubMed]
  • Fugate LP, Spacek LA, Kresty LA, Levy CE, Johnson JC, Mysiw WJ. Definition of agitation following traumatic brain injury. I. A survey of the brain injury special interest group of the American academy of physical medicine and rehabilitation. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1997;78:917–923. [PubMed]
  • Fuxe K, Agnati LF, Jacobsen K, Hillion J, Canals M, Torvinen M, Tinner-Staines B, Staines W, Rosin D, Terasmaa A, Popoli P, Leo G, Vergoni V, Lluis C, Ciruela F, Franco R, Ferre S. Receptor heteromerization in adenosine A2A receptor signaling: relevance for striatal function and Parkinson's disease. Neurology. 2003;61:S19–S23. [PubMed]
  • Fuxe K, Ferre S, Canals M, Torvinen M, Terasmaa A, Marcellino D, Goldberg SR, Staines W, Jacobsen KX, Lluis C, Woods AS, Agnati LF, Franco R. Adenosine A2A and dopamine D2 heteromeric receptor complexes and their function. J. Mol. Neurosci. 2005;26:209–220. [PubMed]
  • Gainetdinov RR. Dopamine transporter mutant mice in experimental neuropharmacology. Naunyn-Schmiedebergs Arch. Pharmacol. 2008;377:301–313. [PubMed]
  • Gainetdinov RR, Jones SR, Fumagalli F, Wightman RM, Caron MG. Reevaluation of the role of the dopamine transporter in dopamine system homeostasis. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 1998;26:148–153. [PubMed]
  • Garris PA, Ciolkowski EL, Wightman RM. Heterogeneity of evoked dopamine overflow within the striatal and striatoamygdaloid regions. Neuroscience. 1994;59:417–427. [PubMed]
  • Gentilini M, Barbieri C, De Renzi E, Faglioni P. Space exploration with and without the aid of vision in hemisphere-damaged patients. Cortex. 1989;25:643–651. [PubMed]
  • Gerasimov MR, Franceschi M, Volkow ND, Gifford A, Gatley SJ, Marsteller D, Molina PE, Dewey SL. Comparison between intraperitoneal and oral methylphenidate administration: a microdialysis and locomotor activity study. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 2000;295:51–57. [PubMed]
  • Gianutsos G, Chute S, Dunn JP. Pharmacological changes in dopaminergic systems induced by long-term administration of amantadine. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 1985;110:357–361. [PubMed]
  • Gibson CL, Gray LJ, Bath PM, Murphy SP. Progesterone for the treatment of experimental brain injury; a systematic review. Brain. 2008;131:318–328. [PubMed]
  • Gilad GM, Reis DJ. Collateral sprouting in central mesolimbic dopamine neurons: biochemical and immunocytochemical evidence of changes in the activity and distribution of tyrosine hydroxylase in terminal fields and in cell bodies of a 10 neurons. Brain Res. 1979;160:17–26. [PubMed]
  • Godwin-Austen RB, Frears CC, Bergmann S, Parkes JD, Knill-Jones RP. Combined treatment of Parkinsonism with l-dopa and amantadine. Lancet. 1970;2:383–385. [PubMed]
  • Goldman-Rakic PS. Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron. 1995;14:477–485. [PubMed]
  • Goldstein LB. Neuropharmacology of TBI-induced plasticity. Brain Inj. 2003;17:685–694. [PubMed]
  • Goldstein LB, Bullman S. Differential effects of haloperidol and clozapine on motor recovery after sensorimotor cortex injury in rats. Neurorehabil. Neural Rep. 2002;16:321–325. [PubMed]
  • Gonon F, Burie JB, Jaber M, Benoit-Marand M, Dumartin B, Bloch B. Geometry and kinetics of dopaminergic transmission in the rat striatum and in mice lacking the dopamine transporter. Prog. Brain Res. 2000;125:291–302. [PubMed]
  • Grahn JA, Parkinson JA, Owen AM. The role of the basal ganglia in learning and memory: neuropsychological studies. Behav. Brain Res. 2009;199:53–60. [PubMed]
  • Granado N, Ortiz O, Suarez LM, Martin ED, Cena V, Solis JM, Moratalla R. D1 but not D5 dopamine receptors are critical for LTP, spatial learning, and LTP-induced arc and zif268 expression in the hippocampus. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18:1–12. [PubMed]
  • Graybiel AM. The basal ganglia and the initiation of movement. Rev. Neurol. (Paris) 1990;146:570–574. [PubMed]
  • Greengard P. Possible role for cyclic nucleotides and phosphorylated membrane proteins in postsynaptic actions of neurotransmitters. Nature. 1976;260:101–108. [PubMed]
  • Greengard P, Allen PB, Nairn AC. Beyond the dopamine receptor: the DARPP-32/protein phosphatase-1 cascade. Neuron. 1999;23:435–447. [PubMed]
  • Greenwood TA, Alexander M, Keck PE, McElroy S, Sadovnick AD, Remick RA, Kelsoe JR. Evidence for linkage disequilibrium between the dopamine transporter and bipolar disorder. Am. J. Med. Gen. 2001;105:145–151. [PubMed]
  • Griesbach GS, Hovda DA, Gomez-Pinilla F, Sutton RL. Voluntary exercise or amphetamine treatment, but not the combination, increases hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor and synapsin I following cortical contusion injury in rats. Neuroscience. 2008;154:530–540. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Gronwall D. Performance changes during recovery from closed head injury. Proc. Aust. Assoc. Neurol. 1976;13:143–147. [PubMed]
  • Gronwall D, Wrightson P. Memory and information processing capacity after closed head injury. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1981;44:889–895. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Gualtieri CT. Pharmacotherapy and the neurobehavioural sequelae of traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 1988;2:101–129. [PubMed]
  • Gualtieri T, Chandler M, Coons TB, Brown LT. Amantadine: a new clinical profile for traumatic brain injury. Clin. Neuropharmacol. 1989;12:258–270. [PubMed]
  • Hakansson K, Lindskog M, Pozzi L, Usiello A, Fisone G. DARPP-32 and modulation of cAMP signaling: involvement in motor control and levodopa-induced dyskinesia. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 2004;10:281–286. [PubMed]
  • Halpain S, Girault JA, Greengard P. Activation of NMDA receptors induces dephosphorylation of DARPP-32 in rat striatal slices. Nature. 1990;343:369–372. [PubMed]
  • Hamm RJ, Dixon CE, Gbadebo DM, Singha AK, Jenkins LW, Lyeth BG, Hayes RL. Cognitive deficits following traumatic brain injury produced by controlled cortical impact. J. Neurotrauma. 1992;9:11–20. [PubMed]
  • Hamm RJ, Lyeth BG, Jenkins LW, O'Dell DM, Pike BR. Selective cognitive impairment following traumatic brain injury in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 1993;59:169–173. [PubMed]
  • Hamm RJ, Pike BR, O'Dell DM, Lyeth BG. Traumatic brain injury enhances the amnesic effect of an NMDA antagonist in rats. J. Neurosurg. 1994;81:267–271. [PubMed]
  • Hamm RJ, Temple MD, O'Dell DM, Pike BR, Lyeth BG. Exposure to environmental complexity promotes recovery of cognitive function after traumatic brain injury. J. Neurotruama. 1996a;13:41–47. [PubMed]
  • Hamm RJ, Temple MD, Pike BR, O'Dell DM, Buck DL, Lyeth BG. Working memory deficits following traumatic brain injury in the rat. J. Neurotrauma. 1996b;13:317–323. [PubMed]
  • Hanks RA, Rapport LJ, Millis SR, Deshpande SA. Measures of executive functioning as predictors of functional ability and social integration in a rehabilitation sample. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1999;80:1030–1037. [PubMed]
  • Haracz JL, Tschanz JT, Wang Z, Griffith KE, Rebec GV. Amphetamine effects on striatal neurons: implications for models of dopamine function. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1998;22:613–622. [PubMed]
  • Heilman KM, Voeller KK, Nadeau SE. A possible pathophysiologic sub-strate of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Child Neurol. 1991;6(Suppl):S76–S81. [PubMed]
  • Hellmich HL, Capra B, Eidson K, Garcia J, Kennedy D, Uchida T, Parsley M, Cowart J, DeWitt DS, Prough DS. Dose-dependent neuronal injury after traumatic brain injury. Brain Res. 2005;1044:144–154. [PubMed]
  • Hemmings HC, Jr., Greengard P. DARPP-32, a dopamine-regulated phosphoprotein. Prog. Brain Res. 1986;69:149–159. [PubMed]
  • Henry JM, Talukder NK, Lee AB, Jr., Walker ML. Cerebral trauma-induced changes in corpus striatal dopamine receptor subtypes. J. Invest. Surg. 1997;10:281–286. [PubMed]
  • Hersch SM, Ciliax BJ, Gutekunst CA, Rees HD, Heilman CJ, Yung KK, Bolam JP, Ince E, Yi H, Levey AI. Electron microscopic analysis of D1 and D2 dopamine receptor proteins in the dorsal striatum and their synaptic relationships with motor corticostriatal afferents. J. Neurosci. 1995;15:5222–5237. [PubMed]
  • Hersch SM, Yi H, Heilman CJ, Edwards RH, Levey AI. Subcellular localization and molecular topology of the dopamine transporter in the striatum and substantia nigra. J. Comp. Neurol. 1997;388:211–227. [PubMed]
  • Hicks RR, Smith DH, Lowenstein DH, Saint Marie R, McIntosh TK. Mild experimental brain injury in the rat induces cognitive deficits associated with regional neuronal loss in the hippocampus. J. Neurotrauma. 1993;10:405–414. [PubMed]
  • Hillion J, Canals M, Torvinen M, Casado V, Scott R, Terasmaa A, Hansson A, Watson S, Olah ME, Mallol J, Canela EI, Zoli M, Agnati LF, Ibanez CF, Lluis C, Franco R, Ferre S, Fuxe K. Coaggregation, cointernalization, and codesensitization of adenosine A2A receptors and dopamine D2 receptors. J. Biol. Chem. 2002;277:18091–18097. [PubMed]
  • Hitri A, Casanova MF, Kleinman JE, Wyatt RJ. Fewer dopamine transporter receptors in the prefrontal cortex of cocaine users. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1994;151:1074–1076. [PubMed]
  • Hoffman AN, Cheng JP, Zafonte RD, Kline AE. Administration of haloperidol and risperidone after neurobehavioral testing hinders the recovery of traumatic brain injury-induced deficits. Life Sci. 2008a;83:602–607. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Hoffman AN, Malena RR, Westergom BP, Luthra P, Cheng JP, Aslam HA, Zafonte RD, Kline AE. Environmental enrichment-mediated functional improvement after experimental traumatic brain injury is contingent on task-specific neurobehavioral experience. Neurosci. Lett. 2008b;431:226–230. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Hogg S, Sanger DJ, Moser PC. Mild traumatic lesion of the right parietal cortex in the rat: characterisation of a conditioned freezing deficit and its reversal by dizocilpine. Behav. Brain Res. 1998;93:157–165. [PubMed]
  • Horn AS. Dopamine uptake: a review of progress in the last decade. Prog. Neurobiol. 1990;34:387–400. [PubMed]
  • Hornykiewicz O. Dopamine (3-hydroxytyramine) and brain function. Pharmacol. Rev. 1966;18:925–964. [PubMed]
  • Hovda DA, Feeney DM. Haloperidol blocks amphetamine induced recovery of binocular depth perception after bilateral visual cortex ablation in cat. Proc. West. Pharmacol. Soc. 1985;28:209–211. [PubMed]
  • Hovda DA, Sutton RL, Feeney DM. Amphetamine-induced recovery of visual cliff performance after bilateral visual cortex ablation in cats: measurements of depth perception thresholds. Behav. Neurosci. 1989;103:574–584. [PubMed]
  • Huff JK, Davies MI. Microdialysis monitoring of methylphenidate in blood and brain correlated with changes in dopamine and rat activity. J. Pharm. Biomed. Anal. 2002;29:767–777. [PubMed]
  • Huff RA, Vaughan RA, Kuhar MJ, Uhl GR. Phorbol esters increase dopamine transporter phosphorylation and decrease transport Vmax. J. Neurochem. 1997;68:225–232. [PubMed]
  • Huger F, Patrick G. Effect of concussive head injury on central catecholamine levels and synthesis rates in rat brain regions. J. Neurochem. 1979;33:89–95. [PubMed]
  • Hughes S, Colantonio A, Santaguida PL, Paton T. Amantadine to enhance readiness for rehabilitation following severe traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 2005;19:1197–1206. [PubMed]
  • Ikemoto S. Dopamine reward circuitry: two projection systems from the ventral midbrain to the nucleus accumbens-olfactory tubercle complex. Brain Res. Rev. 2007;56:27–78. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Izquierdo I, Medina JH. Memory formation: the sequence of biochemical events in the hippocampus and its connection to activity in other brain structures. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 1997;68(3):285–316. [PubMed]
  • Izquierdo I, Quillfeldt JA, et al. Sequential role of hippocampus and amygdala, entorhinal cortex and parietal cortex in formation and retrieval of memory for inhibitory avoidance in rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1997;9(4):786–793. [PubMed]
  • Jackson DM, Jenkins OF, Ross SB. The motor effects of bromocriptine—a review. Psychopharmacology. 1988;95:433–446. [PubMed]
  • Jackson DM, Martin LP, Larsson LG, Cox RF, Waszczak BL, Ross SB. Behavioural, biochemical and electrophysiological studies on the motor depressant and stimulant effects of bromocriptine. Naunyn-Schmiedebergs Arch. Pharmacol. 1990;342:290–299. [PubMed]
  • Jackson DM, Westlind-Danielsson A. Dopamine receptors: molecular biology, biochemistry and behavioural aspects. Pharmacol. Ther. 1994;64:291–370. [PubMed]
  • Jennings JS, Gerber AM, Vallano ML. Pharmacological strategies for neuroprotection in traumatic brain injury. Mini Rev. Med. Chem. 2008;8:689–701. [PubMed]
  • Johnson M. Mem: mechanisms for recollection. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1992;4:268–280. [PubMed]
  • Jonides J. Working memory and thinking. In: Smith EE, Osheron DN, editors. Invitation to Cognitive Science. MIT Press; Cambridge, MA: 1995. pp. 215–265.
  • Jorge RE, Robinson RG, Moser D, Tateno A, Crespo-Facorro B, Arndt S. Major depression following traumatic brain injury. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2004;61:42–50. [PubMed]
  • Kaelin DL, Cifu DX, Matthies B. Methylphenidate effect on attention deficit in the acutely brain-injured adult. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1996;77:6–9. [PubMed]
  • Kahlig KM, Galli A. Regulation of dopamine transporter function and plasma membrane expression by dopamine, amphetamine, and cocaine. Eur. J Pharmacol. 2003;479:153–158. [PubMed]
  • Kahlig KM, Javitch JA, Galli A. Amphetamine regulation of dopamine transport. Combined measurements of transporter currents and transporter imaging support the endocytosis of an active carrier. J. Biol. Chem. 2004;279:8966–8975. [PubMed]
  • Karli DC, Burke DT, Kim HJ, Calvanio R, Fitzpatrick M, Temple D, Macneil M, Pesez K, Lepak P. Effects of dopaminergic combination therapy for frontal lobe dysfunction in traumatic brain injury rehabilitation. Brain Inj. 1999;13:63–68. [PubMed]
  • Kase H, Richardson PJ, Jenner P, editors. Adenosine Receptors and Parkinson's Disease. Academic Press; Orlando: 2000. pp. 1–265.
  • Katzman R, Bjorklund A, Owman C, Stenevi U, West KA. Evidence for regenerative axon sprouting of central catecholamine neurons in the rat mesencephalon following electrolytic lesions. Brain Res. 1971;25:579–596. [PubMed]
  • Kim YH, Ko MH, Na SY, Park SH, Kim KW. Effects of single-dose methylphenidate on cognitive performance in patients with traumatic brain injury: a double-blind placebo-controlled study. Clin. Rehabil. 2006;20:24–30. [PubMed]
  • Kipp M, Karakaya S, Pawlak J, Araujo-Wright G, Arnold S, Beyer C. Estrogen and the development and protection of nigrostriatal dopaminergic neurons: concerted action of a multitude of signals, protective molecules, and growth factors. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2006;27:376–390. [PubMed]
  • Kline AE, Chen MJ, Tso-Olivas DY, Feeney DM. Methylphenidate treatment following ablation-induced hemiplegia in rat: experience during drug action alters effects on recovery of function. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1994;48:773–779. [PubMed]
  • Kline AE, Dixon CE. Contemporary in vivo models of brain trauma and a comparison of injury responses. In: Miller LP, Hayes RL, editors. Head Trauma: Basic, Preclinical, and Clinical Directions. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; New York, NY: 2001. pp. 65–84.
  • Kline AE, Hoffman AN, Cheng JP, Zafonte RD, Massucci JL. Chronic administration of antipsychotics impede behavioral recovery after experimental traumatic brain injury. Neurosci. Lett. 2008;448:263–267. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Kline AE, Massucci JL, Ma X, Zafonte RD, Dixon CE. Bromocriptine reduces lipid peroxidation and enhances spatial learning and hippocampal neuron survival in a rodent model of focal brain trauma. J. Neurotrauma. 2004;21:1712–1722. [PubMed]
  • Kline AE, Massucci JL, Marion DW, Dixon CE. Attenuation of working memory and spatial acquisition deficits after a delayed and chronic bromocriptine treatment regimen in rats subjected to traumatic brain injury by controlled cortical impact. J. Neurotrauma. 2002;19:415–425. [PubMed]
  • Kline AE, Massucci JL, Zafonte RD, Dixon CE, DeFeo JR, Rogers EH. Differential effects of single versus multiple administrations of haloperidol and risperidone on functional outcome after experimental brain trauma. Crit. Care Med. 2007a;35:919–924. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Kline AE, Wagner AK, Westergom BP, Malena RR, Zafonte RD, Olsen AS, Sozda CN, Luthra P, Panda M, Cheng JP, Aslam HA. Acute treatment with the 5-HT(1A) receptor agonist 8-OH-DPAT and chronic environmental enrichment confer neurobehavioral benefit after experimental brain trauma. Behav. Brain Res. 2007b;177:186–194. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Kline AE, Yan HQ, Bao J, Marion DW, Dixon CE. Chronic methylphenidate treatment enhances water maze performance following traumatic brain injury in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 2000;280:163–166. [PubMed]
  • Kline AE, Yu J, Horváth E, Marion DW, Dixon CE. The selective 5-HT1A receptor agonist repinotan HCL attenuates histopathology and spatial learning deficits following traumatic brain injury in rats. Neuroscience. 2001;106:547–555. [PubMed]
  • Knable MB, Egan MF, Heinz A, Gorey J, Lee KS, Coppola R, Weinberger DR. Altered dopaminergic function and negative symptoms in drug-free patients with schizophrenia. [123i]-iodobenzamide SPECT study. Br. J. Psychiatry. 1997;171:574–577. [PubMed]
  • Kobori N, Clifton GL, Dash PK. Enhanced catecholamine synthesis in the prefrontal cortex after traumatic brain injury: implications for prefrontal dysfunction. J. Neurotrauma. 2006;23:1094–1102. [PubMed]
  • Kobori N, Dash PK. Reversal of brain injury-induced prefrontal glutamic acid decarboxylase expression and working memory deficits by D1 receptor antagonism. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:4236–4246. [PubMed]
  • Kochanek PM. Ischemic and traumatic brain injury: pathobiology and cellular mechanisms. Crit. Care Med. 1993;21:S333–S335. [PubMed]
  • Kopin IJ. The pharmacology of Parkinson's disease therapy: an update. Ann. Rev. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 1993;33:467–495. [PubMed]
  • Koponen S, Taiminen T, Portin R, Himanen L, Isoniemi H, Heinonen H, Hinkka S, Tenovuo O. Axis I and II psychiatric disorders after traumatic brain injury: a 30-year follow-up study. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2002;159:1315–1321. [PubMed]
  • Kordower JH, Cochran E, Penn RD, Goetz CG. Putative chromaffin cell survival and enhanced host-derived TH-fiber innervation following a functional adrenal medulla autograft for Parkinson's disease. Ann. Neurol. 1991;29:405–412. [PubMed]
  • Kotapka MJ, Gennarelli TA, Graham DI, Adams JH, Thibault LE, Ross DT, Ford I. Selective vulnerability of hippocampal neurons in acceleration-induced experimental head injury. J. Neurotrauma. 1991;8:247–258. [PubMed]
  • Kraus MF, Maki P. The combined use of amantadine and l-dopa/carbidopa in the treatment of chronic brain injury. Brain Inj. 1997;11:455–460. [PubMed]
  • Kraus MF, Smith GS, Butters M, Donnell AJ, Dixon E, Yilong C, Marion D. Effects of the dopaminergic agent and NMDA receptor antagonist amantadine on cognitive function, cerebral glucose metabolism and D2 receptor availability in chronic traumatic brain injury: a study using positron emission tomography (PET). Brain Inj. 2005;19:471–479. [PubMed]
  • Kuczenski R, Segal DS. Stimulant actions in rodents: implications for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder treatment and potential substance abuse. Biol. Psychiatry. 2005;57:1391–1396. [PubMed]
  • Kuppers E, Beyer C. Expression of estrogen receptor-alpha and beta mRNA in the developing and adult mouse striatum. Neurosci. Lett. 1999;276:95–98. [PubMed]
  • Kuppers E, Ivanova T, Karolczak M, Beyer C. Estrogen: a multifunctional messenger to nigrostriatal dopaminergic neurons. J. Neurocytol. 2000;29:375–385. [PubMed]
  • Lange G, Waked W, Kirshblum S, DeLuca J. Organizational strategy influence on visual memory performance after stroke: cortical/subcortical and left/right hemisphere contrasts. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2000;81:89–94. [PubMed]
  • Langfitt TW, Obrist WD, Alavi A, Grossman RI, Zimmerman R, Jaggi J, Uzzell B, Reivich M, Patton DR. Computerized tomography, magnetic resonance imaging, and positron emission tomography in the study of brain trauma. Preliminary observations. J. Neurosurg. 1986;64:760–767. [PubMed]
  • Lawrence AD, Weeks RA, Brooks DJ, Andrews TC, Watkins LH, Harding AE, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. The relationship between striatal dopamine receptor binding and cognitive performance in Huntington's disease. Brain. 1998;121:1343–1355. [PubMed]
  • Lee H, Kim SW, Kim JM, Shin IS, Yang SJ, Yoon JS. Comparing effects of methylphenidate, sertraline and placebo on neuropsychiatric sequelae in patients with traumatic brain injury. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 2005;20:97–104. [PubMed]
  • Leininger BE, Gramling SE, Farrell AD, Kreutzer JS, Peck EA., 3rd Neuropsychological deficits in symptomatic minor head injury patients after concussion and mild concussion. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1990;53:293–296. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Lemon N, Manahan-Vaughan D. Dopamine D1/D5 receptors gate the acquisition of novel information through hippocampal long-term potentiation and long-term depression. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:7723–7729. [PubMed]
  • Levin HS. Memory deficit after closed head injury. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1990;12:129–153. [PubMed]
  • Levin HS, Goldstein FC, High WM, Jr., Eisenberg HM. Disproportionately severe memory deficit in relation to normal intellectual functioning after closed head injury. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1988a;51:1294–1301. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Levin HS, Goldstein FC, High WM, Jr., Williams D. Automatic and effortful processing after severe closed head injury. Brain Cogn. 1988b;7:283–297. [PubMed]
  • Levin HS, Grossman RG. Behavioral sequelae of closed head injury. A quantitative study. Arch. Neurol. 1978;35:720–727. [PubMed]
  • Levy R, Friedman HR, Davachi L, Goldman-Rakic PS. Differential activation of the caudate nucleus in primates performing spatial and nonspatial working memory tasks. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:3870–3882. [PubMed]
  • Li S, Cullen WK, Anwyl R, Rowan MJ. Dopamine-dependent facilitation of LTP induction in hippocampal ca1 by exposure to spatial novelty. Nat. Neurosci. 2003;6:526–531. [PubMed]
  • Lieberman AN, Goldstein M. Bromocriptine in Parkinson disease. Pharmacol. Rev. 1985;37:217–227. [PubMed]
  • Lifshitz J, Kelley BJ, Povlishock JT. Perisomatic thalamic axotomy after diffuse traumatic brain injury is associated with atrophy rather than cell death. J. Neuropath. Exp. Neurol. 2007;66:218–229. [PubMed]
  • Lighthall JW. Controlled cortical impact: a new experimental brain injury model. J. Neurotrauma. 1988;5:1–15. [PubMed]
  • Lighthall JW, Dixon CE, Anderson TE. Experimental models of brain injury. J. Neurotrauma. 1989;6:83–97. [PubMed]
  • Lindner MD, Plone MA, Cain CK, Frydel B, Francis JM, Emerich DF, Sutton RL. Dissociable long-term cognitive deficits after frontal versus sensorimotor cortical contusions. J. Neurotrauma. 1998;15:199–216. [PubMed]
  • Liu FC, Graybiel AM. Spatiotemporal dynamics of CREB phosphorylation: transient versus sustained phosphorylation in the developing striatum. Neuron. 1996;17:1133–1144. [PubMed]
  • Liu FC, Graybiel AM. Dopamine and calcium signal interactions in the developing striatum: control by kinetics of CREB phosphorylation. Adv. Pharmacol. 1998a;42:682–686. [PubMed]
  • Liu FC, Graybiel AM. Region-dependent dynamics of cAMP response element-binding protein phosphorylation in the basal ganglia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 1998b;95:4708–4713. [PubMed]
  • Liu J, Wang F, Huang C, Long LH, Wu WN, Cai F, Wang JH, Ma LQ, Chen JG. Activation of phosphatidylinositol-linked novel D(1) dopamine receptor contributes to the calcium mobilization in cultured rat prefrontal cortical astrocytes. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 2008 doi:10.1007/s10571-008-9323-9. [PubMed]
  • Lyeth BG, Jenkins LW, Hamm RJ, Dixon CE, Phillips LL, Clifton GL, Young HF, Hayes RL. Prolonged memory impairment in the absence of hippocampal cell death following traumatic brain injury in the rat. Brain Res. 1990;526:249–258. [PubMed]
  • Madras BK, Elmaleh DR, Meltzer PC, Liang AY, Brownell GL, Brownell AL. Positron emission tomography of cocaine binding sites on the dopamine transporter. NIDA Res. Monogr. 1994;138:57–69. [PubMed]
  • Maldve RE, Zhang TA, Ferrani-Kile K, Schreiber SS, Lippmann MJ, Snyder GL, Fienberg AA, Leslie SW, Gonzales RA, Morrisett RA. DARPP-32 and regulation of the ethanol sensitivity of NMDA receptors in the nucleus accumbens. Nat. Neurosci. 2002;5:641–648. [PubMed]
  • Marie RM, Barre L, Dupuy B, Viader F, Defer G, Baron JC. Relationships between striatal dopamine denervation and frontal executive tests in Parkinson's disease. Neurosci. Lett. 1999;260:77–80. [PubMed]
  • Markowitsch HJ. Thalamic mediodorsal nucleus and memory: a critical evaluation of studies in animals and man. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1982;6:351–380. [PubMed]
  • Maruya H, Watanabe Y, Okita M, Lawlor GF, Utsumi H, Niitsuma T. Inhibitory effects of D2 agonists by striatal injection on excessive release of dopamine and hyperactivity induced by Bay K 8644 in rats. Neuroscience. 2003;118:1091–1098. [PubMed]
  • Massucci JL, Kline AE, Ma X, Zafonte RD, Dixon CE. Time dependent alterations in dopamine tissue levels and metabolism after experimental traumatic brain injury in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 2004;372:127–131. [PubMed]
  • Mateer CA. Executive function disorders: rehabilitation challenges and strategies. Sem. Clin. Neuropsychiatry. 1999;4:50–59. [PubMed]
  • Matthies H, Becker A, Schroeder H, Kraus J, Hollt V, Krug M. Dopamine D1-deficient mutant mice do not express the late phase of hippocampal long-term potentiation. Neuroreport. 1997;8:3533–3535. [PubMed]
  • Maurer G, Schreier E, Delaborde S, Nufer R, Shukla AP. Fate and disposition of bromocriptine in animals and man. II. Absorption, elimination and metabolism. Eur. J. Drug Metab. Pharmacokinet. 1983;8:51–62. [PubMed]
  • May LJ, Wightman RM. Effects of D-2 antagonists on frequency-dependent stimulated dopamine overflow in nucleus accumbens and caudate-putamen. J. Neurochem. 1989a;53:898–906. [PubMed]
  • May LJ, Wightman RM. Heterogeneity of stimulated dopamine overflow within rat striatum as observed with in vivo voltammetry. Brain Res. 1989b;487:311–320. [PubMed]
  • McAllister TW, Flashman LA, Sparling MB, Saykin AJ. Working memory deficits after traumatic brain injury: catecholaminergic mechanisms and prospects for treatment—a review. Brain Inj. 2004;18:331–350. [PubMed]
  • McDonald BC, Flashman LA, Saykin AJ. Executive dysfunction following traumatic brain injury: neural substrates and treatment strategies. NeuroRehabilitation. 2002;17:333–344. [PubMed]
  • McDowell S, Whyte J, D'Esposito M. Working memory impairments in traumatic brain injury: evidence from a dual-task paradigm. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35:1341–1353. [PubMed]
  • McDowell S, Whyte J, D'Esposito M. Differential effect of a dopaminergic agonist on prefrontal function in traumatic brain injury patients. Brain. 1998;121:1155–1164. [PubMed]
  • McIntosh TK, Yu T, Gennarelli TA. Alterations in regional brain catecholamine concentrations after experimental brain injury in the rat. J. Neurochem. 1994;63:1426–1433. [PubMed]
  • McMillan TM. Minor head injury. Curr. Opinion Neurol. 1997;10:479–483. [PubMed]
  • McMillan TM, Glucksman EE. The neuropsychology of moderate head injury. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1987;50:393–397. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Mega MS, Cummings JL. Frontal-subcortical circuits and neuropsychiatric disorders. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 1994;6:358–370. [PubMed]
  • Memo M, Missale C, Carruba MO, Spano PF. D2 dopamine receptors associated with inhibition of dopamine release from rat neostriatum are independent of cyclic AMP. Neurosci. Lett. 1986;71:192–196. [PubMed]
  • Meredith GE, Wouterlood FG, Pattiselanno A. Hippocampal fibers make synaptic contacts with glutamate decarboxylase-immunoreactive neurons in the rat nucleus accumbens. Brain Res. 1990;513:329–334. [PubMed]
  • Meythaler JM, Brunner RC, Johnson A, Novack TA. Amantadine to improve neurorecovery in traumatic brain injury-associated diffuse axonal injury: a pilot double-blind randomized trial. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2002;17:300–313. [PubMed]
  • M'Harzi M, Willig F, Costa JC, Delacour J. d-Amphetamine enhances memory performance in rats with damage to the fimbria. Physiol. Behav. 1988;42:575–579. [PubMed]
  • Middleton FA, Strick PL. Basal ganglia output and cognition: evidence from anatomical, behavioral, and clinical studies. Brain Cogn. 2000;42:183–200. [PubMed]
  • Millis SR, Rosenthal M, Novack TA, Sherer M, Nick TG, Kreutzer JS, High WM, Jr., Ricker JH. Long-term neuropsychological outcome after traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2001;16:343–355. [PubMed]
  • Milman A, Rosenberg A, Weizman R, Pick CG. Mild traumatic brain injury induces persistent cognitive deficits and behavioral disturbances in mice. J. Neurotrauma. 2005;22:1003–1010. [PubMed]
  • Ming Y, Zhang H, Long L, Wang F, Chen J, Zhen X. Modulation of Ca2+ signals by phosphatidylinositol-linked novel D1 dopamine receptor in hippocampal neurons. J. Neurochem. 2006;98:1316–1323. [PubMed]
  • Mitchell JB, Gratton A. Involvement of mesolimbic dopamine neurons in sexual behaviors: implications for the neurobiology of motivation. Rev. Neurosci. 1994;5:317–329. [PubMed]
  • Mizoguchi K, Yuzurihara M, Ishige A, Sasaki H, Chui DH, Tabira T. Chronic stress induces impairment of spatial working memory because of prefrontal dopaminergic dysfunction. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:1568–1574. [PubMed]
  • Moldover JE, Goldberg KB, Prout MF. Depression after traumatic brain injury: a review of evidence for clinical heterogeneity. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2004;14:143–154. [PubMed]
  • Monsma FJ, Jr., Mahan LC, McVittie LD, Gerfen CR, Sibley DR. Molecular cloning and expression of a D1 dopamine receptor linked to adenylyl cyclase activation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 1990;87:6723–6727. [PubMed]
  • Mooney GF, Haas LJ. Effect of methylphenidate on brain injury-related anger. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1993;74:153–160. [PubMed]
  • Mura A, Feldon J. Spatial learning in rats is impaired after degeneration of the nigrostriatal dopaminergic system. Move. Disord. 2003;18:860–871. [PubMed]
  • Nairn AC, Svenningsson P, Nishi A, Fisone G, Girault JA, Greengard P. The role of DARPP-32 in the actions of drugs of abuse. Neuropharmacology. 2004;47:14–23. [PubMed]
  • Newburn G, Newburn D. Selegiline in the management of apathy following traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 2005;19:149–154. [PubMed]
  • Nickels JL, Schneider WN, Dombovy ML, Wong TM. Clinical use of amantadine in brain injury rehabilitation. Brain Inj. 1994;8:709–718. [PubMed]
  • NIH Rehabilitation of persons with traumatic brain injury. 1998. pp. 1–41. NIH Consensus Statement 16. [PubMed]
  • Nirenberg MJ, Chan J, Pohorille A, Vaughan RA, Uhl GR, Kuhar MJ, Pickel VM. The dopamine transporter: comparative ultrastructure of dopaminergic axons in limbic and motor compartments of the nucleus accumbens. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:6899–6907. [PubMed]
  • Nishi A, Bibb JA, Matsuyama S, Hamada M, Higashi H, Nairn AC, Greengard P. Regulation of DARPP-32 dephosphorylation at PKA- and Cdk5-sites by NMDA and AMPA receptors: distinct roles of calcineurin and protein phosphatase-2A. J. Neurochem. 2002;81:832–841. [PubMed]
  • O'Carroll CM, Martin SJ, Sandin J, Frenguelli B, Morris RG. Dopaminergic modulation of the persistence of one-trial hippocampus-dependent memory. Learn. Mem. 2006;13:760–769. [PubMed]
  • Oddy M, Coughlan T, Tyerman A, Jenkins D. Social adjustment after closed head injury: a further follow-up seven years after injury. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1985;48:564–568. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Olney JW, Zorumski CF, Stewart GR, Price MT, Wang GJ, Labruyere J. Excitotoxicity of l-dopa and 6-OH-dopa: implications for Parkinson's and Huntington's diseases. Exp. Neurol. 1990;108:269–272. [PubMed]
  • Onali P, Olianas MC, Gessa GL. Characterization of dopamine receptors mediating inhibition of adenylate cyclase activity in rat striatum. Mol. Pharmacol. 1985;28:138–145. [PubMed]
  • Onn SP, Berger TW, Stricker EM, Zigmond MJ. Effects of intraventricular 6-hydroxydopamine on the dopaminergic innervation of striatum: histochemical and neurochemical analysis. Brain Res. 1986;376:8–19. [PubMed]
  • Otmakhova NA, Lisman JE. D1/D5 dopamine receptor activation increases the magnitude of early long-term potentiation at CA1 hippocampal synapses. J. Neurosci. 1996;16:7478–7486. [PubMed]
  • Ouimet CC, Miller PE, Hemmings HC, Jr., Walaas SI, Greengard P. DARPP-32, a dopamine- and adenosine 3′:5′-monophosphate-regulated phosphoprotein enriched in dopamine-innervated brain regions. III. Immunocytochemical localization. J. Neurosci. 1984;4:111–124. [PubMed]
  • Page G, Peeters M, Maloteaux JM, Hermans E. Increased dopamine uptake in striatal synaptosomes after treatment of rats with amantadine. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2000;403:75–80. [PubMed]
  • Pagliari R, Peyrin L, Crambes O. Differential regional and kinetics effects of piribedil and bromocriptine on dopamine metabolites: a brain microdialysis study in freely moving rats. J. Neural Transm. Gen. Sect. 1995;101:13–26. [PubMed]
  • Palmer AM, Marion DW, Botscheller ML, Swedlow PE, Styren SD, DeKosky ST. Traumatic brain injury-induced excitotoxicity assessed in a controlled cortical impact model. J. Neurochem. 1993;61:2015–2024. [PubMed]
  • Park E, Bell JD, Baker AJ. Traumatic brain injury: can the consequences be stopped? CMAJ. 2008;178:1163–1170. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Pasinetti GM, Kohama S, Reinhard JF, Jr., Cheng HW, McNeill TH, Finch CE. Striatal responses to decortication. I. Dopaminergic and astrocytic activities. Brain Res. 1991;567:253–259. [PubMed]
  • Passler MA, Riggs RV. Positive outcomes in traumatic brain injury-vegetative state: patients treated with bromocriptine. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2001;82:311–315. [PubMed]
  • Passineau MJ, Green EJ, Dietrich WD. Therapeutic effects of environmental enrichment on cognitive and tissue integrity following severe traumatic brain injury in rats. Exp. Neurol. 2001;168:373–384. [PubMed]
  • Pavlovskaya M, Hochstein S, Keren O, Mordvinov E, Groswasser Z. Methylphenidate effect on hemispheric attentional imbalance in patients with traumatic brain injury: a psychophysical study. Brain Inj. 2007;21:489–497. [PubMed]
  • Pennartz CM, Kitai ST. Hippocampal inputs to identified neurons in an in vitro slice preparation of the rat nucleus accumbens: evidence for feed-forward inhibition. J. Neurosci. 1991;11:2838–2847. [PubMed]
  • Perona MT, Waters S, Hall FS, Sora I, Lesch KP, Murphy DL, Caron M, Uhl GR. Animal models of depression in dopamine, serotonin, and norepinephrine transporter knockout mice: prominent effects of dopamine transporter deletions. Behav. Pharmacol. 2008;19:566–574. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Piccini PP. Dopamine transporter: basic aspects and neuroimaging. Move. Disord. 2003;18(Suppl 7):S3–S8. [PubMed]
  • Picetti R, Saiardi A, Abdel Samad T, Bozzi Y, Baik JH, Borrelli E. Dopamine D2 receptors in signal transduction and behavior. Crit. Rev. Neurobiol. 1997;11:121–142. [PubMed]
  • Plenger PM, Dixon CE, Castillo RM, Frankowski RF, Yablon SA, Levin HS. Subacute methylphenidate treatment for moderate to moderately severe traumatic brain injury: a preliminary double-blind placebo-controlled study. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1996;77:536–540. [PubMed]
  • Ploeger GE, Spruijt BM, Cools AR. Spatial localization in the Morris water maze in rats: acquisition is affected by intra-accumbens injections of the dopaminergic antagonist haloperidol. Behav. Neurosci. 1994;108:927–934. [PubMed]
  • Ploeger GE, Willemen AP, Cools AR. Role of the nucleus accumbens in social memory in rats. Brain Res. Bull. 1991;26:23–27. [PubMed]
  • Ponsford J, Kinsella G. Attentional deficits following closed-head injury. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1992;14:822–838. [PubMed]
  • Postle BR, Locascio JJ, Corkin S, Growdon JH. The time course of spatial and object learning in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35:1413–1422. [PubMed]
  • Powell JH, al-Adawi S, Morgan J, Greenwood RJ. Motivational deficits after brain injury: effects of bromocriptine in 11 patients. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1996;60:416–421. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Pristupa ZB, McConkey F, Liu F, Man HY, Lee FJ, Wang YT, Niznik HB. Protein kinase-mediated bidirectional trafficking and functional regulation of the human dopamine transporter. Synapse. 1998;30:79–87. [PubMed]
  • Queen SA, Chen MJ, Feeney DM. d-amphetamine attenuates decreased cerebral glucose utilization after unilateral sensorimotor cortex contusion in rats. Brain Res. 1997;777:42–50. [PubMed]
  • Ragnarsson KT. Results of the NIH consensus conference on “Rehabilitation of persons with traumatic brain injury”. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 2002;20:103–108. [PubMed]
  • Ramic M, Emerick AJ, Bollnow MR, O'Brien TE, Tsai SY, Kartje GL. Axonal plasticity is associated with motor recovery following amphetamine treatment combined with rehabilitation after brain injury in the adult rat. Brain Res. 2006;1111:176–186. [PubMed]
  • Rao VL, Dogan A, Bowen KK, Dempsey RJ. Traumatic injury to rat brain upregulates neuronal nitric oxide synthase expression and L-[3H]nitroarginine binding. J. Neurotrauma. 1999;16:865–877. [PubMed]
  • Raz A. Anatomy of attentional networks. Anat. Rec. B New Anat. 2004;281:21–36. [PubMed]
  • Redell JB, Dash PK. Traumatic brain injury stimulates hippocampal catechol-o-methyl transferase expression in microglia. Neurosci. Lett. 2007;413:36–41. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Rees L, Marshall S, Hartridge C, Mackie D, Weiser M. Cognitive interventions post acquired brain injury. Brain Inj. 2007;21:161–200. [PubMed]
  • Reeves TM, Lyeth BG, Povlishock JT. Long-term potentiation deficits and excitability changes following traumatic brain injury. Exp. Brain Res. 1995;106:248–256. [PubMed]
  • Reid WM, Hamm RJ. Post-injury atomoxetine treatment improves cognition following experimental traumatic brain injury. J. Neurotrauma. 2008;25:248–256. [PubMed]
  • Reimold M, Solbach C, Noda S, Schaefer JE, Bartels M, Beneke M, Machulla HJ, Bares R, Glaser T, Wormstall H. Occupancy of dopamine d(1), d (2) and serotonin (2a) receptors in schizophrenic patients treated with flupentixol in comparison with risperidone and haloperidol. Psychopharmacology. 2007;190:241–249. [PubMed]
  • Ricker JH, Hillary FG, DeLuca J. Functionally activated brain imaging (O-15 PET and fMRI) in the study of learning and memory after traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2001;16:191–205. [PubMed]
  • Ricker JH, Keenan PA, Jacobson MW. Visuoperceptual-spatial ability and visual memory in vascular dementia and dementia of the alzheimer type. Neuropsychologia. 1994;32:1287–1296. [PubMed]
  • Ridley RM, Cummings RM, Leow-Dyke A, Baker HF. Neglect of memory after dopaminergic lesions in monkeys. Behav. Brain Res. 2006;166:253–262. [PubMed]
  • Rinne UK, Sonninen V, Siirtola T. Treatment of Parkinson's disease with amantadine and l-Dopa. Eur. Neurol. 1972;7:228–240. [PubMed]
  • Risinger FO, Freeman PA, Greengard P, Fienberg AA. Motivational effects of ethanol in DARPP-32 knock-out mice. J. Neurosci. 2001;21:340–348. [PubMed]
  • Ruskin DN, Marshall JF. Differing influences of dopamine agonists and antagonists on Fos expression in identified populations of globus pallidus neurons. Neuroscience. 1997;81:79–92. [PubMed]
  • Rutland-Brown W, Langlois JA, Thomas KE, Xi YL. Incidence of traumatic brain injury in the United States, 2003. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2006;21:544–548. [PubMed]
  • Salamone JD. The involvement of nucleus accumbens dopamine in appetitive and aversive motivation. Behav. Brain Res. 1994;61:117–133. [PubMed]
  • Salamone JD, Correa M, Mingote SM, Weber SM. Beyond the reward hypothesis: alternative functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 2005;5:34–41. [PubMed]
  • Sanchez-Carrion R, Gomez PV, Junque C, Fernandez-Espejo D, Falcon C, Bargallo N, Roig-Rovira T, Ensenat-Cantallops A, Bernabeu M. Frontal hypoactivation on functional magnetic resonance imaging in working memory after severe diffuse traumatic brain injury. J. Neurotrauma. 2008;25:479–494. [PubMed]
  • Sanders MJ, Dietrich WD, Green EJ. Behavioral, electrophysiological, and histopathological consequences of mild fluid-percussion injury in the rat. Brain Res. 2001;904:141–144. [PubMed]
  • Sanders MJ, Sick TJ, Perez-Pinzon MA, Dietrich WD, Green EJ. Chronic failure in the maintenance of long-term potentiation following fluid percussion injury in the rat. Brain Res. 2000;861:79–86. [PubMed]
  • Saniova B, Drobny M, Kneslova L, Minarik M. The outcome of patients with severe head injuries treated with amantadine sulphate. J. Neural. Transm. 2004;111:511–514. [PubMed]
  • Saniova B, Drobny M, Lehotsky J, Sulaj M, Schudichova J. Biochemical and clinical improvement of cytotoxic state by amantadine sulphate. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 2006;26:1475–1482. [PubMed]
  • Sawyer E, Mauro LS, Ohlinger MJ. Amantadine enhancement of arousal and cognition after traumatic brain injury. Ann. Pharmacother. 2008;42:247–252. [PubMed]
  • Scheff SW, Baldwin SA, Brown RW, Kraemer PJ. Morris water maze deficits in rats following traumatic brain injury: lateral controlled cortical impact. J. Neurotrauma. 1997;14:615–627. [PubMed]
  • Schiffer WK, Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Alexoff DL, Logan J, Dewey SL. Therapeutic doses of amphetamine or methylphenidate differentially increase synaptic and extracellular dopamine. Synapse. 2006;59:243–251. [PubMed]
  • Schinelli S, Paolillo M, Corona GL. Opposing actions of D1- and D2- dopamine receptors on arachidonic acid release and cyclic AMP production in striatal neurons. J. Neurochem. 1994;62:944–949. [PubMed]
  • Schneider JS, Sun ZQ, Roeltgen DP. Effects of dihydrexidine, a full dopamine D-1 receptor agonist, on delayed response performance in chronic low dose MPTP-treated monkeys. Brain Res. 1994a;663:140–144. [PubMed]
  • Schneider JS, Sun ZQ, Roeltgen DP. Effects of dopamine agonists on delayed response performance in chronic low-dose MPTP-treated monkeys. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1994b;48:235–240. [PubMed]
  • Schneider WN, Drew-Cates J, Wong TM, Dombovy ML. Cognitive and behavioural efficacy of amantadine in acute traumatic brain injury: an initial double-blind placebo-controlled study. Brain Inj. 1999;13:863–872. [PubMed]
  • Schulman H. Protein phosphorylation in neuronal plasticity and gene expression. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1995;5:375–381. [PubMed]
  • Schultz W. Neural coding of basic reward terms of animal learning theory, game theory, microeconomics and behavioural ecology. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2004;14:139–147. [PubMed]
  • Seel RT, Kreutzer JS, Rosenthal M, Hammond FM, Corrigan JD, Black K. Depression after traumatic brain injury: a national institute on disability and rehabilitation research model systems multicenter investigation. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2003;84:177–184. [PubMed]
  • Seeman P, Niznik HB. Dopamine receptors and transporters in Parkinson's disease and schizophrenia. FASEB J. 1990;4:2737–2744. [PubMed]
  • Seeman P, Tedesco JL, Lee T, Chau-Wong M, Muller P, Bowles J, Whitaker PM, McManus C, Tittler M, Weinreich P, Friend WC, Brown GM. Dopamine receptors in the central nervous system. Fed. Proc. 1978;37:131–136. [PubMed]
  • Sesack SR, Hawrylak VA, Matus C, Guido MA, Levey AI. Dopamine axon varicosities in the prelimbic division of the rat prefrontal cortex exhibit sparse immunoreactivity for the dopamine transporter. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:2697–2708. [PubMed]
  • Setlow B, McGaugh JL. Sulpiride infused into the nucleus accumbens posttraining impairs memory of spatial water maze training. Behav. Neurosci. 1998;112:603–610. [PubMed]
  • Smith DH, Lowenstein DH, Gennarelli TA, McIntosh TK. Persistent memory dysfunction is associated with bilateral hippocampal damage following experimental brain injury. Neurosci. Lett. 1994;168:151–154. [PubMed]
  • Smith DH, Meaney DF, Shull WH. Diffuse axonal injury in head trauma. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2003;18:307–316. [PubMed]
  • Smith DH, Okiyama K, Thomas MJ, Claussen B, McIntosh TK. Evaluation of memory dysfunction following experimental brain injury using the Morris water maze. J. Neurotrauma. 1991;8:259–269. [PubMed]
  • Smith DR, Striplin CD, Geller AM, Mailman RB, Drago J, Lawler CP, Gallagher M. Behavioural assessment of mice lacking D1A dopamine receptors. Neuroscience. 1998;86:135–146. [PubMed]
  • Smith EE, Jonides J, Koeppe RA. Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. Cereb. Cortex. 1996;6:11–20. [PubMed]
  • Snyder GL, Fienberg AA, Huganir RL, Greengard P. A dopamine/D1 receptor/protein kinase A/dopamine- and cAMP-regulated phosphoprotein (Mr 32 kDa)/protein phosphatase-1 pathway regulates dephosphorylation of the NMDA receptor. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:10297–10303. [PubMed]
  • Soblosky JS, Tabor SL, Matthews MA, Davidson JF, Chorney DA, Carey ME. Reference memory and allocentric spatial localization deficits after unilateral cortical brain injury in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 1996;80:185–194. [PubMed]
  • Solanto MV. Neuropsychopharmacological mechanisms of stimulant drug action in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a review and integration. Behav. Brain Res. 1998;94:127–152. [PubMed]
  • Sonuga-Barke EJ. Causal models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: from common simple deficits to multiple developmental pathways. Biol. Psychiatry. 2005;57:1231–1238. [PubMed]
  • Speech TJ, Rao SM, Osmon DC, Sperry LT. A double-blind controlled study of methylphenidate treatment in closed head injury. Brain Inj. 1993;7:333–338. [PubMed]
  • Spence C, Gallace A. Recent developments in the study of tactile attention. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 2007;61:196–207. [PubMed]
  • Stuss DT, Ely P, Hugenholtz H, Richard MT, LaRochelle S, Poirier CA, Bell I. Subtle neuropsychological deficits in patients with good recovery after closed head injury. Neurosurgery. 1985;17:41–47. [PubMed]
  • Stuss DT, Stethem LL, Hugenholtz H, Picton T, Pivik J, Richard MT. Reaction time after head injury: fatigue, divided and focused attention, and consistency of performance. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1989;52:742–748. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Sutton MA, Beninger RJ. Psychopharmacology of conditioned reward: evidence for a rewarding signal at D1-like dopamine receptors. Psychopharmacology. 1999;144:95–110. [PubMed]
  • Svenningsson P, Nairn AC, Greengard P. DARPP-32 mediates the actions of multiple drugs of abuse. AAPS. 2005;7:E353–E360. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Svenningsson P, Nishi A, Fisone G, Girault JA, Nairn AC, Greengard P. Darpp-32: an integrator of neurotransmission. Ann. Rev. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 2004;44:269–296. [PubMed]
  • Swanson JM, Kinsbourne M, Nigg J, Lanphear B, Stefanatos GA, Volkow N, Taylor E, Casey BJ, Castellanos FX, Wadhwa PD. Etiologic subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: brain imaging, molecular genetic and environmental factors and the dopamine hypothesis. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2007;17:39–59. [PubMed]
  • Tamaru F. Disturbances in higher function in Parkinson's disease. Eur. Neurol. 1997;38(Suppl 2):33–36. [PubMed]
  • Tan EK, Jankovic J. Choosing dopamine agonists in Parkinson's disease. Clin. Neuropharmacol. 2001;24:247–253. [PubMed]
  • Tan S, Hermann B, Borrelli E. Dopaminergic mouse mutants: investigating the roles of the different dopamine receptor subtypes and the dopamine transporter. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 2003;54:145–197. [PubMed]
  • Tanaka S. Dopaminergic control of working memory and its relevance to schizophrenia: a circuit dynamics perspective. Neuroscience. 2006;139:153–171. [PubMed]
  • Tang YP, Noda Y, Nabeshima T. Involvement of activation of dopaminergic neuronal system in learning and memory deficits associated with experimental mild traumatic brain injury. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1997;9:1720–1727. [PubMed]
  • Tassin JP. Ne/da interactions in prefrontal cortex and their possible roles as neuromodulators in schizophrenia. J. Neural Transm. 1992;36:135–162. [PubMed]
  • Tassoni D, Kaur G, Weisinger RS, Sinclair AJ. The role of eicosanoids in the brain. Asia Pac. J. Clin. Nutr. 2008;17(Suppl 1):220–228. [PubMed]
  • Tauscher J, Hussain T, Agid O, Verhoeff NP, Wilson AA, Houle S, Remington G, Zipursky RB, Kapur S. Equivalent occupancy of dopamine d1 and d2 receptors with clozapine: differentiation from other atypical antipsychotics. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1620–1625. [PubMed]
  • Thapar A, O'Donovan M, Owen MJ. The genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Hum. Mol. Gen. 2005;14:R275–R282. (Spec No. 2) [PubMed]
  • Thompson HJ, Lifshitz J, Marklund N, Grady MS, Graham DI, Hovda DA, McIntosh TK. Lateral fluid percussion brain injury: a 15-year review and evaluation. J. Neurotrauma. 2005;22:42–75. [PubMed]
  • Tiberti C, Sabe L, Jason L, Leiguarda R, Starkstein S. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of methylphenidate in patients with organic amnesia. Eur. J. Neurol. 1998;5:297–299. [PubMed]
  • Tidey JW, Miczek KA. Social defeat stress selectively alters mesocorticolimbic dopamine release: an in vivo microdialysis study. Brain Res. 1996;721:140–149. [PubMed]
  • Tolias CM, Bullock MR. Critical appraisal of neuroprotection trials in head injury: what have we learned? NeuroRx. 2004;1:71–79. [PubMed]
  • Tomac A, Lindqvist E, Lin LF, Ogren SO, Young D, Hoffer BJ, Olson L. Protection and repair of the nigrostriatal dopaminergic system by GDNF in vivo. Nature. 1995;373:335–339. [PubMed]
  • Torres GE, Carneiro A, Seamans K, Fiorentini C, Sweeney A, Yao WD, Caron MG. Oligomerization and trafficking of the human dopamine transporter. Mutational analysis identifies critical domains important for the functional expression of the transporter. J. Biol. Chem. 2003;278:2731–2739. [PubMed]
  • Tremont G, Halpert S, Javorsky DJ, Stern RA. Differential impact of executive dysfunction on verbal list learning and story recall. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2000;14:295–302. [PubMed]
  • Turner-Stokes L, Disler PB, Nair A, Wade DT. Multi-disciplinary rehabilitation for acquired brain injury in adults of working age. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2005:CD004170. [PubMed]
  • Undie AS, Berki AC, Beardsley K. Dopaminergic behaviors and signal transduction mediated through adenylate cyclase and phospholipase C pathways. Neuropharmacology. 2000;39:75–87. [PubMed]
  • Undie AS, Weinstock J, Sarau HM, Friedman E. Evidence for a distinct D1-like dopamine receptor that couples to activation of phosphoinositide metabolism in brain. J. Neurochem. 1994;62:2045–2048. [PubMed]
  • Valjent E, Pascoli V, Svenningsson P, Paul S, Enslen H, Corvol JC, Stipanovich A, Caboche J, Lombroso PJ, Nairn AC, Greengard P, Herve D, Girault JA. Regulation of a protein phosphatase cascade allows convergent dopamine and glutamate signals to activate ERK in the striatum. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2005;102:491–496. [PubMed]
  • Vallar L, Meldolesi J. Mechanisms of signal transduction at the dopamine D2 receptor. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 1989;10:74–77. [PubMed]
  • Van der Werf YD, Scheltens P, Lindeboom J, Witter MP, Uylings HB, Jolles J. Deficits of memory, executive functioning and attention following infarction in the thalamus; a study of 22 cases with localised lesions. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:1330–1344. [PubMed]
  • Verheij MM, Cools AR. Twenty years of dopamine research: individual differences in the response of accumbal dopamine to environmental and pharmacological challenges. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2008;585:228–244. [PubMed]
  • Vertes RP. Interactions among the medial prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and midline thalamus in emotional and cognitive processing in the rat. Neuroscience. 2006;142:1–20. [PubMed]
  • Viggiano D, Vallone D, Ruocco LA, Sadile AG. Behavioural, pharmacological, morpho-functional molecular studies reveal a hyperfunctioning mesocortical dopamine system in an animal model of attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2003;27:683–689. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang G, Ding Y, Gatley SJ. Mechanism of action of methylphenidate: insights from PET imaging studies. J. Atten. Disord. 2002a;6(Suppl 1):S31–S43. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Gatley SJ, Logan J, Ding YS, Hitzemann R, Pappas N. Dopamine transporter occupancies in the human brain induced by therapeutic doses of oral methylphenidate. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1998;155:1325–1331. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Logan J, Franceschi D, Maynard L, Ding YS, Gatley SJ, Gifford A, Zhu W, Swanson JM. Relationship between blockade of dopamine transporters by oral methylphenidate and the increases in extracellular dopamine: therapeutic implications. Synapse. 2002b;43:181–187. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Telang F, Maynard L, Logan J, Gatley SJ, Pappas N, Wong C, Vaska P, Zhu W, Swanson JM. Evidence that methylphenidate enhances the saliency of a mathematical task by increasing dopamine in the human brain. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2004;161:1173–1180. [PubMed]
  • Von Voigtlander PF, Moore KE. Dopamine: release from the brain in vivo by amantadine. Science. 1971;174:408–410. [PubMed]
  • Wagner AK, Chen X, Kline AE, Li Y, Zafonte RD, Dixon CE. Gender and environmental enrichment impact dopamine transporter expression after experimental traumatic brain injury. Exp. Neurol. 2005;195:475–483. [PubMed]
  • Wagner AK, Drewencki L, Chen X, Santos FR, Khan AS, Harun R, Torres G, Michael AC, Dixon CE. Chronic methylphenidate treatment enhances striatal dopamine neurotransmission after experimental traumatic brain injury. J. Neurochem. 2009;108:986–997. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Wagner AK, Kline AE, Ren D, Willard LA, Wenger MK, Zafonte RD, Dixon CE. Gender associations with chronic methylphenidate treatment and behavioral performance following experimental traumatic brain injury. Behav. Brain Res. 2007a;181:200–209. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Wagner AK, Kline AE, Sokoloski J, Zafonte RD, Capulong E, Dixon CE. Intervention with environmental enrichment after experimental brain trauma enhances cognitive recovery in male but not female rats. Neurosci. Lett. 2002;334:165–168. [PubMed]
  • Wagner AK, Postal BA, Darrah SD, Chen X, Khan AS. Deficits in novelty exploration after controlled cortical impact. J. Neurotrauma. 2007b;24:1308–1320. [PubMed]
  • Wagner AK, Ren D, Conley YP, Ma X, Kerr ME, Zafonte RD, Puccio AM, Marion DW, Dixon CE. Sex and genetic associations with cerebrospinal fluid dopamine and metabolite production after severe traumatic brain injury. J. Neurosurg. 2007c;106:538–547. [PubMed]
  • Wagner AK, Willard LA, Kline AE, Wenger MK, Bolinger BD, Ren D, Zafonte RD, Dixon CE. Evaluation of estrous cycle stage and gender on behavioral outcome after experimental traumatic brain injury. Brain Res. 2004;998:113–121. [PubMed]
  • Walaas SI, Greengard P. DARPP-32, a dopamine- and adenosine 3′:5′-monophosphate-regulated phosphoprotein enriched in dopamine-innervated brain regions. I. Regional and cellular distribution in the rat brain. J. Neurosci. 1984;4:84–98. [PubMed]
  • Waltereit R, Weller M. Signaling from cAMP/PKA to MAPK and synaptic plasticity. Mol. Neurobiol. 2003;27:99–106. [PubMed]
  • Warden DL, Gordon B, McAllister TW, Silver JM, Barth JT, Bruns J, Drake A, Gentry T, Jagoda A, Katz DI, Kraus J, Labbate LA, Ryan LM, Sparling MB, Walters B, Whyte J, Zapata A, Zitnay G. Guidelines for the pharmacologic treatment of neurobehavioral sequelae of traumatic brain injury. J. Neurotrauma. 2006;23:1468–1501. [PubMed]
  • Weiss S, Sebben M, Garcia-Sainz JA, Bockaert J. D2-dopamine receptor-mediated inhibition of cyclic AMP formation in striatal neurons in primary culture. Mol. Pharmacol. 1985;27:595–599. [PubMed]
  • Whalen MJ, Clark RS, Dixon CE, Robichaud P, Marion DW, Vagni V, Graham SH, Virag L, Hasko G, Stachlewitz R, Szabo C, Kochanek PM. Reduction of cognitive and motor deficits after traumatic brain injury in mice deficient in poly(ADP-ribose) polymerase. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 1999;19:835–842. [PubMed]
  • Whyte J, Hart T, Schuster K, Fleming M, Polansky M, Coslett HB. Effects of methylphenidate on attentional function after traumatic brain injury. A randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1997;76:440–450. [PubMed]
  • Whyte J, Hart T, Vaccaro M, Grieb-Neff P, Risser A, Polansky M, Coslett HB. Effects of methylphenidate on attention deficits after traumatic brain injury: a multidimensional, randomized, controlled trial. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2004;83:401–420. [PubMed]
  • Whyte J, Vaccaro M, Grieb-Neff P, Hart T, Polansky M, Coslett HB. The effects of bromocriptine on attention deficits after traumatic brain injury: a placebo-controlled pilot study. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2008;87:85–99. [PubMed]
  • Wickens JR, Budd CS, Hyland BI, Arbuthnott GW. Striatal contributions to reward and decision making: making sense of regional variations in a reiterated processing matrix. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2007;1104:192–212. [PubMed]
  • Williams GV, Castner SA. Under the curve: critical issues for elucidating D1 receptor function in working memory. Neuroscience. 2006;139:263–276. [PubMed]
  • Williams JM, Galli A. The dopamine transporter: a vigilant border control for psychostimulant action. Handb. Exp. Pharmacol. 2006:215–232. [PubMed]
  • Wilson MS, Gibson CJ, Hamm RJ. Haloperidol, but not olanzapine, impairs cognitive performance after traumatic brain injury in rats. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2003;82:871–879. [PubMed]
  • Wise SP, Murray EA, Gerfen CR. The frontal cortex-basal ganglia system in primates. Crit. Rev. Neurobiol. 1996;10:317–356. [PubMed]
  • Wu N, Cepeda C, Zhuang X, Levine MS. Altered corticostriatal neurotransmission and modulation in dopamine transporter knock-down mice. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;98:423–432. [PubMed]
  • Wu TS, Garmel GM. Improved neurological function after amantadine treatment in two patients with brain injury. J. Emerg. Med. 2005;28:289–292. [PubMed]
  • Yan HQ, Kline AE, Ma X, Hooghe-Peters EL, Marion DW, Dixon CE. Tyrosine hydroxylase, but not dopamine beta-hydroxylase, is increased in rat frontal cortex after traumatic brain injury. Neuroreport. 2001;12:2323–2327. [PubMed]
  • Yan HQ, Ma X, Chen X, Li Y, Shao L, Dixon CE. Delayed increase of tyrosine hydroxylase expression in rat nigrostriatal system after traumatic brain injury. Brain Res. 2007;1134:171–179. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Yan Z, Feng J, Fienberg AA, Greengard P. D(2) dopamine receptors induce mitogen-activated protein kinase and cAMP response element-binding protein phosphorylation in neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 1999;96:11607–11612. [PubMed]
  • Yang ZJ, Torbey M, Li X, Bernardy J, Golden WC, Martin LJ, Koehler RC. Dopamine receptor modulation of hypoxic-ischemic neuronal injury in striatum of newborn piglets. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 2007;27:1339–1351. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Zafonte RD, Watanabe T, Mann NR. Amantadine: a potential treatment for the minimally conscious state. Brain Inj. 1998;12:617–621. [PubMed]
  • Zakzanis KK, Kaplan E. Neuropsychological Differential Diagnosis. Swets & Zeitlinger; Lisse, Netherlands: 1999.
  • Zhou QY, Grandy DK, Thambi L, Kushner JA, Van Tol HH, Cone R, Pribnow D, Salon J, Bunzow JR, Civelli O. Cloning and expression of human and rat D1 dopamine receptors. Nature. 1990;347:76–80. [PubMed]
  • Zhu J, Hamm RJ, Reeves TM, Povlishock JT, Phillips LL. Postinjury administration of l-deprenyl improves cognitive function and enhances neuroplasticity after traumatic brain injury. Exp. Neurol. 2000;166:136–152. [PubMed]
  • Zhuang X, Oosting RS, Jones SR, Gainetdinov RR, Miller GW, Caron MG, Hen R. Hyperactivity and impaired response habituation in hyperdopaminergic mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2001;98:1982–1987. [PubMed]