PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptNIH Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
Health Expect. Author manuscript; available in PMC Oct 1, 2009.
Published in final edited form as:
Health Expect. Dec 2008; 11(4): 400–408.
doi:  10.1111/j.1369-7625.2008.00503.x
PMCID: PMC2689380
NIHMSID: NIHMS110581
Patients’ Perceptions of Safety if Interpersonal Continuity of Care were to be Disrupted
Nancy Pandhi, MD MPH,1,2 Jessica Schumacher, MS,2 Kathryn E. Flynn, PhD,3 and Maureen Smith, MD PhD MPH2
1University of Wisconsin, Department of Family Medicine
2University of Wisconsin, Department of Population Health Sciences
3Duke University, Center for Clinical and Genetic Economics
Correspondence should be addressed to: Nancy Pandhi MD MPH University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health Department of Family Medicine 777 S. Mills Street Madison, WI 53715 Telephone: 608−265−6448 Fax: 608−263−5813 E-mail: nancy.pandhi/at/fammed.wisc.edu (do not publish)
OBJECTIVE
To determine if patients vary in perceptions of safety if interpersonal continuity were to be disrupted. If so, which characteristics are associated with feeling unsafe?
BACKGROUND
The extent to which patients’ preference for continuity with a personal physician is due to perceptions of safety is unclear.
DESIGN
Observational study (Wisconsin Longitudinal Study Graduate and Sibling Survey)
SETTING AND PARTICIPANTS
6,827 respondents (most aged 63−66 years) who completed the 2003−2006 survey round
MAIN VARIABLES STUDIED
Age, gender, marital status, education, health insurance type, illnesses, medications, length of relationship with provider and place, personality type, decision making preference and trust in physician deliberation
MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES
Safety perception when visiting another doctor or clinic if own doctor were not available
RESULTS
12% of respondents felt unsafe. After adjustment, as compared to those who felt safe, those who felt unsafe were more likely to be women (Odds ratio=1.65, 95% Confidence Interval=1.35−2.01), have more chronic conditions (1.27, 1.08−1.50) and have a longer relationship with a usual provider: 5−9 years (1.53, 1.11−2.10) 10−14 years (1.41, 1.02−1.95) and 15 or more years (1.62, 1.20−2.17) compared to 0−4 years. Those who preferred active participation in decision making and had trust in their physician were less likely to feel safe (1.63, 1.10−2.41).
CONCLUSIONS
Certain older adults perceive being unsafe if not seeing their usual physician. Further research should investigate reasons for perceptions of safety if continuity were disrupted and any implications for care.
Keywords: continuity of patient care, doctor-patient relationship, patient safety, shared decision-making, survey research
Interpersonal continuity of care between a patient and a personal physician is positively associated with several patient-centered outcomes including satisfaction,1-4 trust5, 6 and adherence.7 However, patients vary in their preference for interpersonal continuity.8 In particular, seniors,9, 10 females,11 the less educated, Medicare/Medicaid recipients,10 those with more chronic conditions11 and those with longer relationships to their physician10, 12 prefer interpersonal continuity. Specific reasons given for this preference include a perception of comfort13 or familiarity with the physician,14, 15 better communication16, 17 and the physician's prior knowledge about them.14, 15
It has been suggested that discontinuity in care can lead to decreased patient safety by inhibiting a clinician's ability to anticipate and correct gaps in care.18 However, it is unclear the extent to which patients’ interpersonal continuity preference is due to perceptions that they are safer seeing the same physician. If a preference for interpersonal continuity is related to perceptions of safety, discontinuity with a physician may have severe consequences such as decreased adherence in addition to the safety concerns cited above. Learning about patients who feel unsafe when seeing physicians other than their own is increasingly important given the health system shift away from interpersonal continuity towards continuity provided by a team of physicians at a usual site19 or insurance changes that lead to disruptions in care.20
Patients’ perceptions of safety also are important because these perceptions differ from those of health care professionals and may have consequences for care. Although information regarding patients’ perceptions of safety is limited,21-24 studies suggest that patients conceive of safety broadly by considering areas such as access, care coordination and communication in addition to technical quality.23, 25-27 A 1997 Harris poll of 1,513 randomly selected households in the United States found that 42% of adults reported being personally involved in a situation where a medical mistake was made. One-third of those adults further believed that the medical mistake had a permanent negative effect on health.26 Patients describe physical, emotional and financial harm occurring from medical mistakes.28 Some patients who perceive experiencing a medical error change future behavior by taking extra measures to ensure their own safety.24, 26, 29 Others report then mistrusting the health care system and avoiding further interaction, or seeking out another provider.24, 28
This paper seeks to answer the following questions: 1) Is there variation in the extent to which patients feel unsafe if interpersonal continuity were to be disrupted? 2) If there is variation, which patient characteristics are associated with feeling unsafe? In addition to previous factors associated with general preference for continuity (e.g., age, gender, education, insurance, chronic conditions, length of patient-physician relationship), we examine the patient's length of relationship with their usual place of care as well as their decision making preferences and personality characteristics. We also examine whether the effect of these characteristics was mediated by trust in the usual physician.
Sample
We use data from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study (WLS) survey, a long-term cohort study of a one-third random sample (N=10,317) of men and women who graduated from Wisconsin high schools in the spring of 1957 and 7,638 of their randomly-selected brothers and sisters. Individuals were most recently surveyed in 1992−1994 and 2003−2006. In the 2003−2006 round of data collection, all surviving WLS participants were contacted via telephone whether or not they participated in the 1992 round of the survey. Respondents were traced and interviews were conducted and audio-recorded using computer-assisted techniques by the University of Wisconsin Survey Center. Interviews lasted approximately one hour. Among graduate survivors, the response rate for this survey was 80% and for siblings the response rate was 78%. Telephone interviews were followed by 54-page mail-out, mail-back surveys that take approximately 90 minutes to complete. Three mailings and one final telephone contact were made to encourage respondents to mail back their questionnaires. Among those who completed the telephone interview, a response rate of 88% was achieved for graduates and 81% for siblings.
We included graduate and sibling respondents who answered the question about safety. We excluded 8% of respondents who did not perceive they had a usual health care provider and therefore could not have interpersonal continuity. An additional 10% of respondents who were unsure of the length of relationship that they had with their provider were also excluded. The final sample size was 6,827. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the participating university.
Variables/Measures
All variables are obtained through patient self-report. The degree to which patients feel safe if interpersonal continuity were to be disrupted, the dependent variable, was indicated by response to the following item: “If my doctor were not available, I would feel safe visiting another doctor or clinic” (1=agree strongly to 5=disagree strongly).
Socio-demographic information included age, gender, marital status, education and health insurance type. Other variables relating to health and physician-patient relationship included chronic illness measured by the Duke Older Americans Resources and Services (OARS) schedule of common illnesses and conditions30 plus several additional chronic illnesses and conditions, the number of regularly taken prescription medications, the length of relationship with a usual provider and the length of relationship with a usual place of care. For conditions other than hypertension, diabetes, cancer (other than skin), coronary artery disease, stroke, joint pain, and chronic arthritis, information was collected on the extent to which the condition affected the respondent's life (1=Not at all to 5=A great deal). A variable was constructed that summed the number of conditions that affected the respondent's life “quite a bit” or “a great deal.”
Decision making preference was constructed from four variables that assessed respondent preferences during healthcare visits for information exchange (physician knowledge of patient medical history and physician disclosure of treatment choices), deliberation (discussion of treatment choices) and selection of treatment choice. Cluster analysis was performed on these items to create a typology of patient preference types.31 Four preference types characterized over 96% of respondents, all of which preferred high levels of information exchange with physicians. Deliberative autonomists were the most active type; they preferred discussion of treatment choices and personal control over important decisions. Deliberative delegators preferred discussion of treatment choices with doctor control over important decisions. Non-deliberative autonomists preferred little or no discussion of treatment choices with personal control over important decisions. Non-deliberative delegators were the least active type; they preferred little or no discussion of treatment choices and doctor control over important decisions.
We measured personality based on the widely-used five factor model.32 Twenty-nine items—a subset of the BFI-54 33—represented the five-factors: extraversion (6 items), agreeableness (6 items), conscientiousness (6 items), neuroticism (5 items) and openness-to-experience (6 items) with response categories measured on a 6-point scale from 1=agree strongly to 6=disagree strongly. Items within each factor were combined into a scale, and all scales were then standardized to have a mean of zero and standard deviation of one.
Patient's trust in physician deliberation was assessed using responses to the question, “If I had many treatment options, I worry about whether my doctor cares enough to discuss each one with me as long as I want” (1=agree strongly, 5=disagree strongly; disagree strongly/disagree=high trust, neutral=neutral and agree strongly/agree=low trust).
Statistical analysis
Logistic regression was used to estimate odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals. We compared individuals who would feel unsafe with disruption to interpersonal continuity (response of Agree Strongly/Agree) to those who would feel safe if interpersonal continuity were disrupted (response of Disagree strongly/Disagree) for each explanatory variable (socio-demographics, chronic health conditions, count of life-interfering conditions, a count of medications regularly taken, length of relationship with usual provider/place, decision-making preference type and personality factors), controlling for all other variables in the model.
Because we expected that the effect of these characteristics on feeling unsafe might differ by trust, we tested for interaction between trust in physician deliberation and the following: gender, marital status, personality and decision making preferences. We accounted for clustering of siblings within families by calculating confidence intervals and significance tests using the Stata ‘robust’ command, clustering family explicitly.34-36 Results were considered statistically significant at p-value < 0.05. Likelihood ratio tests were conducted to determine the statistical significance for groups of indicator variables. Data were analyzed using SAS 9.1 and Stata 9.0.37, 38
Respondents were predominantly in their early to mid sixties (Table 1). Most were married with at least a high school education and held private insurance. The majority had longstanding relationships with their usual place and provider of over five years. Hypertension, joint pain and arthritis were common conditions. Diabetes, cancer, coronary artery disease and stroke were less common conditions present in the sample. Over half of the respondents were taking at least one prescription medication regularly. Less than 20%, however, had other chronic conditions that interfered quite a bit or greatly with their daily lives. The majority of respondents were deliberators and therefore preferred discussion of treatment options with their physician.
Table 1
Table 1
Descriptive characteristics (N=6,827)
Variation existed in the extent to which patients felt unsafe were they to experience discontinuity. Figure 1 shows the percentage distribution of this variation; 67% of the sample agreed or strongly agreed that they would feel safe visiting someone other than their own doctor (N=4,555), 21% were neutral (N=1,404) and 12% disagreed or strongly disagreed with this statement (N=868).
Figure 1
Figure 1
Distribution of perceptions of safety if interpersonal continuity were to be disrupted
Table 2 indicates characteristics of individuals who felt unsafe (N=868) as opposed to safe (N=4,555) about visiting another doctor or clinic. Patients who feel unsafe were more likely to be female (Odds Ratio=1.65; 95% Confidence Interval: 1.35−2.01). Age, education, marital status and insurance type were not significantly associated with feeling unsafe.
Table 2
Table 2
Adjusted odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals (CI) for the relationship between patient characteristics and feeling unsafe about interpersonal continuity disruption (N=5,741)
The length of relationship with a provider but not a place was significantly associated with feeling unsafe versus safe with a break in interpersonal continuity. As compared to those who had been with their provider 0−4 years, those who had been with their provider for 5−9 years had 53% increased odds of feeling unsafe (95% CI: 1.11−2.10), those who had been with their provider10−14 years had 41% increased odds of feeling unsafe (95% CI: 1.02−1.95) and those who had been with their provider 15 or more years had 62% increased odds of feeling unsafe (95% CI: 1.20−2.17).
While having an increasing number of chronic conditions that interfered with life was significantly associated with feeling unsafe with a break in interpersonal continuity (OR=1.27; 95% CI: 1.08−1.50), only one specific chronic condition, coronary artery disease, was significantly associated with feeling unsafe (OR=1.49, 95% CI 1.18−1.88). As a sensitivity analysis, we repeated analyses with a variable that indicated the presence of any chronic condition. This variable was not significantly associated with feeling unsafe. Taking more medications, decision making preference type and personality type also were not significantly associated with safety.
Individuals who felt safe or unsafe about seeing someone other than their own physician varied in trusting their physician's deliberation. Of those who felt safe about seeing someone else, 49% perceived high trust, 22% were neutral and 29% had low trust (data not shown). Conversely, of those who felt unsafe about seeing someone else, 55% perceived high trust, 15% were neutral and 30% perceived low trust. There was a borderline statistically significant interaction between decision making preference and trust (p < 0.06), suggesting that the effect of decision making preference on feeling unsafe about a break in continuity might differ for patients who had weak versus high trust in their physician. As indicated in Table 3, among those with high trust in their physician, deliberative autonomists (the most active preference type) were more likely to feel unsafe if there were to experience discontinuity when compared to non-deliberative delegators (the least active preference type) (OR 1.63; 95% CI 1.10−2.41). A similar result was shown for deliberative delegators (OR 1.50; 95% CI 0.97−2.32) although this result was only borderline statistically significant.
Table 3
Table 3
Adjusted odds ratios (OR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) for the relationship between decision making preference and feeling unsafe without interpersonal continuity, by trust in physician deliberation (N=5,696)
To our knowledge, this is the first paper that examines patients’ sense of safety if they were to experience a break in interpersonal continuity. Our study supports the idea that interpersonal continuity is preferred by those in vulnerable subgroups8, 10 as we found that women, patients with more chronic conditions and patients with coronary artery disease would feel more unsafe if seeing someone other than their own doctor. Overall, decision making preference and personality were not associated with feeling unsafe, although those with a preference for active participation in decision making (i.e., deliberative autonomists) were more likely to feel unsafe if they had high trust in their physician's deliberation.
While other studies have indicated that a majority of individuals prefer interpersonal continuity,11, 39 we found that only 12% of the predominantly older adults in our study reported that they would feel unsafe if continuity were to be disrupted. This finding raises the question of whether, while interpersonal continuity is an ideal preferred by many individuals, actual perceptions of consequences due to discontinuity may be evident in only a smaller group of individuals. This magnitude in difference between individuals who prefer continuity and who actually perceive the consequence of feeling unsafe merits further study, particularly as interpersonal continuity is studied for tangible links to health benefit or cost reduction.
Although our study suggests that some individuals perceive disruption to continuity as unsafe, there is evidence that interpersonal continuity of care may not benefit patients in certain circumstances. Studies have found that interpersonal continuity did not improve completion of diabetes monitoring tests,40 or lower HgbA1c values in diabetic patients.41 Qualitative research performed with GPs in the United Kingdom found that the physicians felt obligated to maintain relationship with patients despite feeling powerless to improve clinical outcomes, even to the extent of colluding with unhealthy illness behaviors.42 Further research is needed about negative consequences of continuity and patient's perceptions of these outcomes in regards to safety.
One particularly interesting finding was that among individuals who had high trust in their physician's deliberation, those who preferred deliberation in decision making were more likely to feel unsafe about seeing someone other than their regular physician. This finding suggests that patients feel safer when discussing treatment choices with an individual that they are used to seeing, and is consistent with other literature that suggests that a reason for interpersonal continuity preference is improved communication.14, 16
Our study has several limitations. As our sample population was predominantly white adults from a single geographical area with at least a high school education, this work may have limited generalizability to other populations. Our study population was predominantly clustered in the older adult range, which may have limited the ability for age to have statistical significance in this study. Future research is needed to explore characteristics of safety-related interpersonal continuity preference in other groups. In addition, while safety is likely a multidimensional construct, we were limited to measuring safety in the unidimensional manner in which it was asked of participants in this study. Perceptions of safety are a complex issue worthy of further study. Nevertheless, our findings suggest that some patients link the doctor patient relationship to perceptions of safety. Finally, our question asked patients how they would hypothetically feel about safety if interpersonal continuity were to be disrupted. We were unable to further explore how much of this perception was based on actual experience. Further research is needed about patients’ perceptions of safety with and without continuity. In particular, qualitative research might address when and under what circumstances individuals feels unsafe without continuity.
Our main findings—that some individuals perceive not being safe if they were to experience interpersonal continuity disruption, and that those individuals who have high trust in their doctor would feel unsafe about seeing someone else if they have preferences for deliberation about treatment choices—have important implications for the design of health care visits. When possible, office policies and procedures should be designed so that individuals who prefer interpersonal continuity should be scheduled with their own physician. Given the Institute of Medicine's emphasis on shared physician-patient decision making,43, 44 disruptions to continuity in relationship should be minimized for those who prefer discussing treatment options with their own physician. Further research should identify reasons that patients feel unsafe about interpersonal continuity disruption and whether the perception of feeling unsafe is associated with subsequent negative health care outcomes.
Acknowledgements
Portions of these results were presented on 10/16/06 at the 2006 North American Primary Care Research Group Annual Meeting in Tucson, Arizona
Funding sources: This project was supported by the Community-Academic Partnerships core of the University of Wisconsin Institute for Clinical and Translational Research (UW ICTR) funded through an NIH Clinical and Translational Science Award (CTSA), grant number 1 UL1 RR025011
Footnotes
No conflicts of interest exist for the authors.
1. Hjortdahl P, Laerum E. Continuity of care in general practice: effect on patient satisfaction. British Medical Journal. 1992;304:1287–90. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
2. Smith CS. The impact of an ambulatory firm system on quality and continuity of care. Medical Care. 1995;33:221–6. [PubMed]
3. Wasson JH, Sauvigne AE, Mogielnicki RP, et al. Continuity of outpatient medical care in elderly men. A randomized trial. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1984;252:2413–7. [PubMed]
4. Weyrauch KF. Does continuity of care increase HMO patients’ satisfaction with physician performance? Journal of the American Board of Family Practice. 1996;9:31–6. [PubMed]
5. Mainous AG, 3rd, Baker R, Love MM, Gray DP, Gill JM. Continuity of care and trust in one's physician: evidence from primary care in the United States and the United Kingdom. Family Medicine. 2001;33:22–7. [PubMed]
6. Thom DH, Ribisl KM, Stewart AL, Luke DA. Further validation and reliability testing of the Trust in Physician Scale. The Stanford Trust Study Physicians. Medical Care. 1999;37:510–7. [PubMed]
7. Charney E, Bynum R, Eldredge D, et al. How well do patients take oral penicillin? A collaborative study in private practices. Pediatrics. 1967;40:188–95. [PubMed]
8. Pandhi N, Saultz JW. Patients’ perceptions of interpersonal continuity of care. Journal of the American Board of Family Medicine. 2006;19:390–7. [PubMed]
9. Brown JB, McWilliam CL, Mai V. Barriers and facilitators to seniors’ independence. Perceptions of seniors, caregivers, and health care providers. Canadian Family Physician. 1997;43:469–75. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
10. Nutting PA, Goodwin MA, Flocke SA, Zyzanski SJ, Stange KC. Continuity of primary care: to whom does it matter and when? Annals of Family Medicine. 2003;1:149–55. [PubMed]
11. Pereira AG, Pearson SD. Patient attitudes toward continuity of care. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2003;163:909–12. [PubMed]
12. Mainous AG, 3rd, Goodwin MA, Stange KC. Patient-physician shared experiences and value patients place on continuity of care. Annals of Family Medicine. 2004;2:452–4. [PubMed]
13. Pandhi N, Bowers B, Chen FP. A Comfortable Relationship: A Patient-derived Dimension of Ongoing Care. Family Medicine. 2007;39:266–7. [PubMed]
14. Gabel LL, Lucas JB, Westbury RC. Why do patients continue to see the same physician? Family Practice Research Journal. 1993;13:133–47. [PubMed]
15. Tarrant C, Windridge K, Boulton M, Baker R, Freeman G. How important is personal care in general practice? British Medical Journal. 2003;326:1310. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
16. Brown JB, Dickie I, Brown L, Biehn J. Long-term attendance at a family practice teaching unit. Qualitative study of patients’ views. Canadian Family Physician. 1997;43:901–6. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
17. Infante FA, Proudfoot JG, Powell Davies G, et al. How people with chronic illnesses view their care in general practice: a qualitative study. Medical Journal of Australia. 2004;181:70–3. [PubMed]
18. Cook RI, Render M, Woods DD. Gaps in the continuity of care and progress on patient safety. British Medical Journal. 2000;320:791–4. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
19. Stokes T, Tarrant C, Mainous AG, et al. Continuity of care: Is the personal doctor still important? A survey of general practitioners and family physicians in England and Wales, the United States, and the Netherlands. Annals of Family Medicine. 2005;3:353–9. [PubMed]
20. Kikano GE, Flocke SA, Gotler RS, Stange KC. ‘My insurance changed’: the negative effects of forced discontinuity of care. Family Practice Management. 2000;7:44–5. [PubMed]
21. Vincent CA, Coulter A. Patient safety: what about the patient? Quality and Safety in Health Care. 2002;11:76–80. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
22. Duclos CW, Eichler M, Taylor L, et al. Patient perspectives of patient-provider communication after adverse events. International Journal for Quality in Health Care. 2005;17:479–86. [PubMed]
23. Kuzel AJ, Woolf SH, Gilchrist VJ, et al. Patient reports of preventable problems and harms in primary health care. Annals of Family Medicine. 2004;2:333–40. [PubMed]
24. Elder NC, Jacobson CJ, Zink T, Hasse L. How experiencing preventable medical problems changed patients’ interactions with primary health care. Annals of Family Medicine. 2005;3:537–44. [PubMed]
25. Gallagher TH, Waterman AD, Ebers AG, Fraser VJ, Levinson W. Patients’ and physicians’ attitudes regarding the disclosure of medical errors. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2003;289:1001–7. [PubMed]
26. Louis Harris and Associates Public opinion of patient safety issues research findings. National Patient Safety Foundation at the AMA; North Adams, MA: 1997.
27. Burroughs TE, Waterman AD, Gallagher TH, et al. Patients’ concerns about medical errors during hospitalization. Joint Commission Journal on Quality and Patient Safety. 2007;33:5–14. [PubMed]
28. Van Vorst RF, Araya-Guerra R, Felzien M, et al. Rural community members’ perceptions of harm from medical mistakes: A High Plains Research Network (HPRN) study. Journal of the American Board of Family Medicine. 2007;20:135–43. [PubMed]
29. Nau DP, Erickson SR. Medication safety: patients’ experiences, beliefs, and behaviors. Journal of the American Pharmacists Association. 2005;45:452–7. [PubMed]
30. Duke University Center for the Study of Aging and Human Development Multidimensional functional assessment: the OARS methodology. Duke University; Durham, NC: 1978.
31. Flynn KE, Smith MA, Vanness D. A typology of preferences for participation in healthcare decision making. Social Science & Medicine. 2006;63:1158–69. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
32. McCrae RR, Costa PT., Jr Personality in Adulthood: A Five-Factor Theory Perspective. 2nd ed. Guilford Press; New York: 2003.
33. Srivastava S, John OP, Gosling SD, Potter J. Development of personality in early and middle adulthood: set like plaster or persistent change? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;84:1041–53. [PubMed]
34. Huber PJ. Proceedings of the Fifth Berkeley Symposium on Mathematical Statistics and Probability. University of California Press; Berkeley, CA: 1967. The behavior of maximum likelihood estimates under nonstandard conditions.
35. White H. A heteroskedasticity-consistent covariance matrix estimator and a direct test for heteroskedasticity. Econometrica. 1980;48:817–38.
36. Rogers WH. SG17: regression standard errors in clustered samples. Stata Technical Bulletin. 1993;13:19–23.
37. SAS Institute SAS Statistical Software. 8.2 ed. SAS Institute; Cary, NC: 2002.
38. StataCorp Stata Statistical Software. 8.0 ed. Stata Corporation; College Station, TX: 2003.
39. Lewis CL, Wickstrom GC, Kolar MM, et al. Patient preferences for care by general internists and specialists in the ambulatory setting. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2000;15:75–83. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
40. Gill JM, Mainous AG, 3rd, Diamond JJ, Lenhard MJ. Impact of provider continuity on quality of care for persons with diabetes mellitus. Annals of Family Medicine. 2003;1:162–70. [PubMed]
41. Hanninen J, Takala J, Keinanen-Kiukaanniemi S. Good continuity of care may improve quality of life in Type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Research and Clinical Practice. 2001;51:21–7. [PubMed]
42. Chew-Graham CA, May CR, Roland MO. The harmful consequences of elevating the doctor-patient relationship to be a primary goal of the general practice consultation. Family Practice. 2004;21:229–31. [PubMed]
43. Institute of Medicine Crossing the quality chasm: A new health system for the 21st century. National Academy Press; Washington, DC: 2001.
44. Institute of Medicine Improving Medical Education: Enhancing the Behavioral and Social Science Content of Medical School Curricula. National Academies Press; Washington, D.C.: 2004.