PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of jpnSubmit a ManuscriptEmail AlertsAbout JPNJournal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience
 
J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2008 May; 33(3): 187–195.
PMCID: PMC2441882

Is fetal brain monoamine oxidase inhibition the missing link between maternal smoking and conduct disorders?

Abstract

Smoking is the leading cause of preventable illness in the world today. Prenatal cigarette smoke exposure (PCSE) is a particularly insidious form because so many of its associated health effects befall the unborn child and produce behavioural outcomes that manifest themselves only years later. Among these are the associations between PCSE and conduct disorders, which have been mostly ascribed to the deleterious effects of nicotine on the fetal brain. Here we hypothesize that inhibition of brain monoamine oxidase (MAO) during fetal brain development, secondary to maternal cigarette smoking and in addition to nicotine, is a likely contributor to this association. MAOs play a central role in monoaminergic balance in the brain, and their inhibition during fetal development — but not during adult life — is known to result in an aggressive phenotype in laboratory animals. This paper provides theoretical and experimental support for the notion that cigarette smoke–induced inhibition of MAO in the fetal brain, particularly when it occurs in combination with polymorphisms in the MAOA gene that lead to lower enzyme concentration in the brain, may result in brain morphologic and functional changes that enhance the risk of irritability, poor self-control and aggression in the offspring. It also encourages research to evaluate whether the interaction of smoking exposure during fetal development and MAOA genotype increases the risk for conduct disorder over that incurred by mere fetal exposure to tobacco smoke.

Medical subject headings: monoamine oxidase, smoking, maternal-fetal exchange, conduct disorder

Résumé

Le tabagisme est maintenant la principale cause de maladies évitables dans le monde. L'exposition prénatale à la fumée de cigarette (EPFC) est particulièrement insidieuse parce que l'enfant à naître subit un très grand nombre des effets néfastes du tabagisme sur la santé et que cette atteinte produit des résultats comportementaux qui se manifestent seulement des années plus tard. Il y a notamment association entre l'EPFC et le trouble des conduites, que l'on a attribués pour la plupart aux effets nocifs de la nicotine sur le cerveau du fœtus. Nous posons ici en hypothèse que l'inhibition des monoamines oxydases (MAO) dans le cerveau au cours du développement du fœtus, résultat secondaire du tabagisme de la mère et qui s'ajoute à la dépendance à la nicotine, contribue probablement à ce lien. Les MAO jouent un rôle central dans l'équilibre monoaminergique du cerveau et on sait que leur inhibition au cours du développement du fœtus — mais non durant la vie adulte — produit un phénotype agressif chez des animaux de laboratoire. Cette communication appuie par la théorie et des données expérimentales le concept selon lequel l'inhibition des MAO provoquée par la fumée de cigarette dans le cerveau du fœtus, particulièrement lorsqu'elle est combinée à des polymorphismes du gène MAOA qui entraînent une baisse de la concentration d'enzymes dans le cerveau, peut produire dans celui-ci des changements morphologiques et fonctionnels qui favorisent le risque d'irritabilité, de manque de contrôle de soi et d'agression chez les enfants. Elle encourage aussi la recherche visant à évaluer si l'interaction entre l'exposition au tabagisme au cours du développement du fœtus et le génotype MAOA alourdit le risque de trouble des conduites par rapport au risque qui découle de la simple exposition du fœtus à la fumée de tabac.

Introduction

One in 5 Americans — roughly 45 million adults — smoke,1 and close to 40% are women. Estimates indicate that 1 in 10 pregnant mothers smoke cigarettes, which is a rate far higher than the 1% goal proposed by Healthy People 2010.2

Smoking during pregnancy has been identified as the single most preventable cause of illness and death among mothers and infants.3 This behaviour has been linked to many deleterious effects that put the newborn at increased risk for adverse health outcomes. For example, maternal smoking during pregnancy is dose-dependently associated with low birth weight and reduced growth of the fetal head,4 the harbingers of impaired cognition that could help explain the observed associations between prenatal cigarette smoke exposure (PCSE) and increased risks for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, antisocial behaviours and neurocognitive deficits in the offspring (see Neuman and colleagues5 for a list of references). However, the mechanisms underlying such long-term effects in the offspring have been difficult to establish, likely because of the confounding effects of many genetic, epigenetic, developmental and environmental factors, whose combined actions help train these associations. Our goal is not to present a comprehensive review of these factors and associations but to focus on the type of disruptive behaviours categorized as conduct disorders.

Conduct disorders encompass a group of aggressive and nonaggressive behavioural and emotional syndromes in children and adolescents who have difficulty following rules and behaving in socially acceptable ways. These disorders affect between 3% and 8% of preadolescent and adolescent boys and are more common in boys than in girls.6 According to some estimates, smoking during early pregnancy may account for up to 25% of externalizing (aggressive) behaviours, which include conduct disorders.6 In the past 15 years, numerous epidemiologic surveys have documented robust links between PCSE and externalizing conduct disorders.7–17 An association with aggressive behaviours was also suggested by a large longitudinal Danish study that found that maternal smoking predicted persistent criminal outcome in male offspring even after correcting for parental characteristics or perinatal complications.18 Collectively, the specificity of these effects and their timing strongly suggest that maternal smoking may contribute to the development of conduct disorders including aggressive behaviours, a hypothesis that is supported by preclinical studies.19 Disturbingly, the association could be self-perpetuating because PCSE appears to be a risk factor for nicotine dependence later in life,20 and a childhood history of conduct disorder may be a risk factor for maternal smoking during pregnancy.21 Indeed, it is possible that a significant overlap exists between the genes that confer vulnerability to conduct disorders and those that modulate smoking behaviours.

The challenging issue is to determine whether exposure to tobacco smoke while in utero can cause antisocial behaviours or whether it merely indicates the presence of other risk factors (e.g., common genetic vulnerabilities, postnatal exposure to cigarette smoke) that modulate or lead to this class of psychiatric disorders in the offspring. Included among these factors are a mother's subsequent smoking habits,22 a mother's history of antisocial behaviour17,23 and depression symptoms17 and the family's socioeconomic status10,17 or loading for alcohol dependence.24 Moreover, in one study correcting for parental depression and antisocial behaviour, general heritable influences and social deprivation virtually obliterated the association between PCSE and conduct disorders.17 Nonetheless, such negative findings do not rule out the contribution of an interaction between a specific gene(s) (not properly represented in the study's cohort), environment (smoke exposures) and development. Indeed, just as cannabis use in adolescence appears to modulate the risk for the later development of schizophreniform symptoms only in those with a specific genetic vulnerability (i.e., carriers of Val allele at the catechol-O-methyltransferase [COMT] locus),25 it is conceivable, according to the cumulative data, that PCSE may add significantly to the risk of later developing conduct disorders only in carriers of a specific genetic vulnerability, such as the low monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) expressing allele.

The complex neurobiology of conduct disorders

Neural substrates of antisocial behaviours

Although the neurobiological basis of antisocial behaviour is poorly understood, most authors agree that the symptoms associated with conduct disorder (Fig. 1A) arise from a peculiar response to emotional cues in the social environment. The underlying deficiency appears to comprise 2 related but dissociable components: antisocial attitudes and impaired behavioural inhibition, or impulsivity. Proper behavioural responses to emotionally charged stimuli rely on a circuit that involves the amygdala,29 dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate gyrus30,31 and orbitofrontal cortex32 (Fig. 1B). An impaired ability to rein in aggressive behaviours is thought to emanate from lower fear response and higher anger reactivity (amygdala) combined with impaired inhibitory control by prefrontal regions (including the prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate gyrus).

figure 2FF1
Fig. 1: Top–down view of conduct disorders. (A) Conduct disorders according to DSM-IV criteria.26 Conduct disorders can be suspected when a child displays a repetitive and persistent pattern of serious aggressive or nonaggressive misbehaviours ...

Early animal studies had suggested that cortical projections to the amygdala are critical in assigning emotional significance to stimuli.33 It has also been observed that lesions to the orbitofrontal cortex and adjacent prefrontal cortex can lead to impulsivity and aggression.34 Human brain-imaging studies also support the notion that aggressive behaviours could be partly attributed to a disrupted amygdalar-frontal circuit.35–38 For example, a study of 40 adolescents who exhibited aggressive behaviours found decreased activity in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and increased activity in the anteromedial portions of the frontal lobes, as well as activity changes in the limbic system.39 Similarly, compared with control subjects, persons with a diagnosis of conduct disorders have been reported to show a pronounced deactivation of the right dorsal anterior cingulate gyrus while experiencing negative affect-laden pictures,40 reduced right temporal lobe and right temporal grey matter volumes41 and a distinct electrophysiological defect in the frontal brain.42 Although not yet conclusive, the neuroimaging data help to delineate the brain areas associated with impulsive acts that may be dysfunctional in people with antisocial or aggressive behaviours.

The serotonergic system has been the most extensively scrutinized for its role in the development of such circuits.28 The resulting literature is vast and complex, partly owing to the fact that serotonin (5-HT) and other monoaminergic neurotransmitters also act as unconventional trophic factors, playing key roles during brain morphogenesis (see Nicotra and colleagues43 for a comprehensive review). Such complexity notwithstanding, the combined evidence appears to suggest that conduct disorders might be more strongly associated with decreases in 5-HT.44

In this context, we should pay particular attention to the amygdala, which, with its heavy serotonergic input,45 is likely to be uniquely sensitive to alterations in serotonergic neurotransmission. Serotonin appears likely to regulate the normal balance between excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmission, thus altering signal processing within the amygdala and modifying behavioural responses dependent on this processing44 (Fig. 1C). For example, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI)–mediated 5-HT increases have been reported to potentiate amygdala response to threat-relevant stimuli46 perhaps by disrupting the neural processes involved in the handling of social information.47 This mechanism could explain how genetically driven excesses in 5-HT during development might result in a lopsided prefrontocortical-amygdalar circuit (Fig. 1C) that compromises the function of neural circuits regulating emotion, negative affect and stress later in life.28,48

It seems particularly relevant to mention in this context the known 5-HT autoregulatory negative feedback, likely mediated by the 5-HT1A receptor, that ensures an appropriate level of serotonin innervation in the adult brain.49 Thus any behavioural effects of aberrant serotonergic transmission would be highly specific to developmental stage and could explain how an excess of 5-HT during fetal development could lead to decreased 5-HT in the adult brain, facilitating impulsivity and aggressive behaviour.50,51 Indeed, aggressive men may be more prone to aggression following reductions in plasma tryptophan,52 SSRIs reduce aggression in psychiatric patients,53 and 5-HT has been consistently found to exert inhibitory control over impulsive aggression (see Davidson and colleagues27 and van Goozen and colleagues53 for reviews).

However, not all studies have shown these associations. For example, while lower concentrations of 5-HIAA, a 5-HT metabolite, have been reported to predict aggressive behaviours in boys with conduct disorders,54 findings of 5-HT increases and decreases, and of no differences, have also been reported.55 Such ambiguity in the combined data may reflect 5-HT's exquisitely orchestrated developmental trajectories as well as the contribution of other factors (e.g., genetic, environmental and other monoaminergic systems) that may modulate the direction and magnitude of 5-HT's roles (neurotransmission and morphogenesis) as well as the specificity of serotonin effects as a function of the brain region or circuit it modulates.

The stress response system is likely to be another major confounding factor and a significant contributor to individual differences in antisocial behaviour. Indeed, aggressive individuals appear to be less sensitive to stress, a phenomenon that has been proposed to result from either a relatively lower capacity to mount a fear response56 or a proclivity to engage in risk-taking behaviours.57 Regardless of the underlying mechanisms, there is consistent evidence of an inverse relation between the ability to increase cortisol in response to stress and the manifestation of antisocial behaviours.44 In the context of the stepwise model of conduct disorders being developed in this article (see below), it is worth mentioning a recently published hypothesis positing that an adverse childhood environment is likely to present frequent stressful stimuli, resulting in habituation and less reactivity to stress.44

In spite of the challenge presented by such confounds, aptly reviewed elsewhere,59 it is clear that 5-HT activity can contribute to the kinds of bias in information processing that could predispose a person to pathological conditions such as aggressive or antisocial behaviours. If, as we hypothesize here, there is inhibition secondary to maternal smoking of fetal brain MAO, which regulates monoamine levels, it follows that this could affect the integrity of the underlying networks.

Direct and indirect effects of nicotine on brain development and behaviour

In tobacco smoke, there are thousands of toxic chemicals with the potential to derail key developmental processes either directly or indirectly, and nicotine is particularly relevant among them. Even though, or perhaps because, nicotinic acetylcholine receptors display many different properties and are ubiquitously expressed in the brain, their actual roles in normal brain physiology have been difficult to elucidate. Nevertheless, multiple lines of evidence suggest a significant and multifaceted role during brain development. Brain nicotinic receptors, which appear early60 and reach high levels during human gestation (Fig. 2, top panel), have been shown to affect gene expression, neuronal differentiation, synapse formation and neuronal path finding. Predictably, administration of nicotine to pregnant rats changes basal neuronal activity in fetal mesocorticolimbic structures,63 induces damage to serotonergic projections in the cerebral cortex and striatum,64 limits growth of the forebrain and reduces the number of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar vermis,65 and compromises brain repair mechanisms.66

figure 2FF2
Fig. 2: The rise and fall of MAO. (Top panel) Time course of nicotinic receptors and MAO's expression is overlaid atop that of critical events in the determination of brain morphometry during (and after) human gestation. (From: Lenroot RK, Giedd JN. Brain ...

Nicotine can also upregulate nicotinic receptors,67 which could perturb brain maturation. For example, endogenous nicotinic activity acts throughout the nervous system to switch GABAergic signalling from excitatory (during early stages of development) to inhibitory. Since nicotinic blockade can extend the period of GABAergic excitation,68 intrauterine exposure to nicotine could disrupt (delay) the timing of the switch, affecting the development of neurons and their proper integration into circuits.69 Interestingly, this may affect the broader serotonergic system because GABAergic axons traverse the developing raphe nuclei in the brainstem at a time when 5-HT neurons are just beginning to differentiate and migrate away from the ventricular zone.70 At the system level, nicotine also interacts with specific receptors in placental vasculature, disrupting the flow of blood and fluids and depressing active amino acid uptake and delivery.71 The resulting fetal undernutrition helps explain the underdevelopment of both neural and nonneural elements,72 which constitute recognized risks for internalizing and externalizing psychiatric disorders and for behavioural problems.73

Predictably, animal studies suggest several ways in which nicotine in the fetal brain could affect neurobehaviour and social development.19,74 For example, nicotine exposure during early embryonic stages abrogates the ability of 7- to 11-day-old chicks to learn a detour task, an effect associated with failure to upregulate ornithine decarboxylase activity75; as well, brain aromatase activity, known to play a role in sexual brain differentiation, is inhibited in male but not female offspring of pregnant rats infused with nicotine.76 Also, fetal nicotine exposure in rats affects the physiology of neurons in the pedunculopontine nucleus,77 a site that controls arousal, producing a more excitable offspring78; further, it alters 5-HT synaptic communication, changing the response to nicotine administration and conferring vulnerability to nicotine dependence later in adolescence.64 Taken together, these observations point to just a few of the possible complex mechanisms linking nicotine exposure to negative effects on behavioural substrates. More research is needed, however, to fully understand how these substrates are affected by the primary and secondary effects of exposure to nicotine in utero, as well as the possible (and likely) contribution of other neurotoxic components present in tobacco smoke.79

MAO and behavioural outcomes: a complex relation

In light of 5-HT's critical role in guiding brain developmental processes, it is highly pertinent that some of the more than 4000 nonnicotinic components present in tobacco smoke,80 such as 2,3,6-trimethyl-1,4-naphthoquinone and farnesyl-acetone,81 the aromatic β carbolines, norharman and harman,82 and even carbon monoxide,83 are all suspected inhibitors of MAO enzymes, which catalyze the oxidative deamination of neurotransmitters and biogenic amines (e.g., dopamine, norepinephrine, tyramine and serotonin). This may account for tobacco smoke's ability to reduce MAO activity in vitro,84 and in the periphery in vivo,85–88 and in a smoker's brain and body62,87,89,90 (Fig. 2, bottom panel). Such whole-body effects strongly suggest that the MAO inhibitory activity could reach the brain of an exposed fetus, a phenomenon that has not yet been tested; if exerted during critical times of synaptogenesis (Fig. 2), this inhibitory activity could conceivably potentiate the nicotinic damage to 5-HT–dependent morphogenetic processes.

The time-sensitive impact of alterations in morphogenetic neurotransmitters

When considering the possibility that smoke-driven inhibition of MAO also occurs in fetal tissues, it is worth noting that relatively high concentrations of MAOA enzyme activity appear significantly earlier in the developing human brain, compared with MAOB,91 which begins its rise toward adult levels perinatally92 (Fig. 2). This could be significant since MAOA (but not MAOB93) gene knockout (KO) mice display increased aggressiveness when compared with wild-type littermates.94 Also, pharmacologic inhibition of MAOA (and B) during fetal development, but not during adulthood, results in aggressive phenotypes in rodents.95,96

A depressed MAOA activity could boost the nicotine-mediated surges in serotonergic, noradrenergic and dopaminergic tone.97 Indeed, significantly higher levels of 5-HT can be found in the ventral hippocampus (+202%), frontal cortex (+96%) and dorsal raphe nucleus (+147%) of MAOA KO mice.98 In turn, tonic increases in 5-HT may contribute to some of the developmental disruptions observed in the brains of these mice, such as aberrant 5-HT projections99 and the malformations in the barrel field of the primary somatosensory cortex100 in serotonin transporter gene KO mice.

These neurochemical deficits foreshadow a growing neurobehavioural literature suggesting that early interference with the MAO system could enhance the risk of antisocial or aggressive behaviours, or both. Consistent with this notion, in preclinical94–96 and clinical101–103 studies, fetal reductions in MAOA have been linked to a range of behavioural effects related to aggression or impulsivity in the offspring. Because of the monoamines' dual role in neurotransmission and morphogenesis, however, there is no reason to expect that the behavioural outcomes of pharmacologic 5-HT manipulations during adulthood should recapitulate the effects of 5-HT disruptions during brain development.

The genetics of MAOA and conduct disorders

There is a well-established genetic contribution to the risk for conduct disorders, with both aggressive and nonaggressive forms of antisocial behaviours displaying measurable (sometimes as high as 60% of observed covariance) heritable components.104–106

Predictably, MAOs have been a major focus of research attempting to map the genetic pathways leading to conduct disorders. Interestingly, the fact that MAOs are X-linked genes may explain, at least in part, the significantly higher prevalence of conduct disorders among boys, since males carry only 1 allele that determines expression levels.

With regard to other potentially relevant genetic differences, much attention has been devoted to MAOA regulatory variants associated with either low or high levels of gene expression. The polymorphism most commonly scrutinized in this context is a variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR) in the promoter of the MAOA gene that engenders an allelic imbalance,107 so that a 4-repeat VNTR confers higher levels of reporter gene expression than a 3-repeat VNTR.108

There is growing albeit inconclusive evidence that functional variations in the MAOA gene, as well as other gene products that modulate serotonergic tone,109 modulate the risk for conduct disorders. Expectedly, and mirroring the bidirectional findings at the level of 5-HT, the issue of whether the high-or the low-activity allele confers protection is still a matter of debate. For example, and in support of the former hypothesis, maltreated children appear to be at a higher risk for developing antisocial behaviours if they carry the low, as compared with the high, MAOA allele103; the former has also been linked to socioemotional hypersensitivity.110 Also, a point (null) mutation in the structural MAOA gene has been identified in a large Dutch kindred, in which several affected male subjects (essentially human MAOA knockouts) suffered borderline mental retardation and displayed distinct abnormal behaviours including impulsive aggression, arson, attempted rape and exhibitionism.102 Conversely, associations have also been reported between the high MAOA allele and aggressiveness and impulsivity in both healthy men111 and in patients with ADHD.112 Finally, a recent brain-imaging study showed that carriers of the high-activity allele scored worse on a task that measures impulsivity, compared with the carriers of the low-activity allele, in a way that correlated with greater blood oxygen level dependent responses in several cortical areas.113

The combined evidence suggests that MAOA polymorphisms may modulate aggressive behaviour in humans. The connections to conduct disorders, however, are likely to be complex and indirect as they influence developmental processes and their interaction with environmental factors. In one study, for example, an MAOA polymorphism was found to be predictive of antisocial behaviour only in those subjects who were exposed to physical abuse during childhood.103 This observation is consistent with the results of a study in nonhuman primates showing that the interaction between a low expressing MAOA allele and psychosocial stress correlated with higher levels of aggressive behaviours in the offspring.114

PCSE and MAO: effects at the confluence of genetics, brain development and early environmental factors

Here we propose that the contribution of MAOA deficiencies toward conduct disorders are most likely restricted to an early window of vulnerability in the process of brain development. Indeed, using imaging we found no differences in MAOA levels in brain as a function of MAOA genotype in healthy adult subjects.115 These results are similar to those obtained with cortical autopsy samples116 and convergent with previous examples in which the expected influence of genotype on protein expression in the adult brain was inconsistent or very small.117 Thus the putative deleterious effects of carrying a low expressing MAOA allele or of inhibiting MAOA enzyme activity with tobacco smoke may derive from interactions with specific developmental factors, such as the lack of sufficient compensatory MAOB expression in the fetal brain during development, when the brain is undergoing major changes. Alternatively, different alleles could in theory display very different and developmentally segregated patterns of gene expression, a possibility worth investigating.

What brain developmental processes may be susceptible to the contingent variations in MAOA gene expression? A possible answer is hinted at by a study showing that a low expressing MAOA allele — the one previously linked in some studies to an increased risk of violent behaviour — was associated with smaller volumes in limbic structures (amygdala and anterior cingulate gyrus), larger volumes in lateral orbitofrontal cortex (only in male subjects) and concomitant increased amygdalar responsiveness and decreased reactivity in regulatory prefrontal regions58 (Fig. 3A and 3B).

figure 2FF3
Fig. 3: Magnetic resonance imaging data demonstrate limbic and paralimbic regional volume changes in subjects with the low-activity MAOA variant (n = 97). Plots represent the summed volumes of voxels in predefined regions of interest, normalized to volume ...

The differential impact of MAOA alleles on the morphology and function of brain regions implicated in cognitive control and the generation and regulation of emotions could explain at least part of the previously reported association between MAOA, impulsivity and aggression. Serotonin levels, which are especially sensitive to MAOA activity, appear to play a major regulatory role during neurodevelopment,118,119 particularly in the formation of a properly counterbalanced cortico-amygdalar circuit. Hence, the morphologic correlates of low MAOA levels would indicate that agents affecting MAOA early in development have the potential to affect significant long-term neurologic changes poised to hamper the emergence of normative social behaviours. Importantly, since the subjects in this study58 were psychiatrically normal, one must conclude that the observed MAOA-related structural and functional differences in corticolimbic circuitry must, by themselves, be compatible with normal mental health.

Summary and agenda for the future

Collectively, the findings described here offer a tantalizing example of how direct effects on the MAO system could modulate the complex interactions between genetic, developmental and environmental factors, as highlighted in recent models,53,120 to increase the risk of conduct disorders in the offspring. Further research on such complex interactions may provide better tools with which to assess the ambiguous nature of the available epidemiologic evidence of increased risk for conduct disorders in the offspring of mothers who smoked while pregnant.

From the evidence we have synthesized here, we propose a modified hypothetical model (Fig. 4), according to which smoking-induced MAO inhibition, when combined with allelic variations that lower MAOA gene expression, place the offspring, via the modulation of brain development trajectories, at a higher risk for developing conduct disorders later in life.

figure 2FF4
Fig. 4: Hypothetical model of the cumulative effect of various risk factors for developing conduct disorder. Normal brain development (A arrow) relies, among other factors, on carefully balanced concentrations of monoamine neurotransmitters in time and ...

The developmental processes modulated by MAO (but also by the dopamine transporter, COMT and sex specifiers) at these stages constitute a target for many converging insults, and the risk may step over a threshold when a stressful or adverse environment is added to the equation. This hypothetical model now awaits the research needed to corroborate that smoking inhibits MAO in the fetal brain and that prenatal exposure to cigarette smoke confers a higher risk for conduct disorders in those infants carrying the low MAOA allele.

Footnotes

Contributors: Drs. Baler and Volkow designed the review and wrote the article. Drs. Volkow and Fowler acquired the data, which Drs. Baler, Volkow and Benveniste interpreted. All authors reviewed the article and gave final approval for its publication.

Competing interests: None declared

Correspondence to: Dr. R.D. Baler, Office of Science Policy and Communications, National Institute on Drug Abuse, 6001 Executive Blvd., Rm. 5241, MSC 9591, Bethesda MD 20892; fax 301 480-2485; balerr/at/mail.nih.gov

References

1. Tobacco use among adults—United States, 2005. MMWR2006;55;1145-8.
2. Levy DT, Nikolayev L, Mumford E, et al. The Healthy People 2010 smoking prevalence and tobacco control objectives: results from the SimSmoke tobacco control policy simulation model (United States). Cancer Causes Control2005;16:359-71. [PubMed]
3. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Preventing chronic diseases: investing wisely in health[Chronic Disease Prevention factsheet]. Bethesda (MD): US Department of Health and Human Services; 2006.
4. Jaddoe VW, Verburg BO, de Ridder MA, et al. Maternal smoking and fetal growth characteristics in different periods of pregnancy: the generation R study. Am J Epidemiol2007;165:1207-15. [PubMed]
5. Neuman RJ, Lobos E, Reich W, et al. Prenatal smoking exposure and dopaminergic genotypes interact to cause a severe ADHD subtype. Biol Psychiatry2006;61:1320-8. [PubMed]
6. Williams GM, O'Callaghan M, Najman JM, et al. Maternal cigarette smoking and child psychiatric morbidity: a longitudinal study. Pediatrics 1998;102:e11. Available: pediatrics.aappublications.org/cgi/reprint/102/1/e11.pdf(accessed 2008 Mar 20). [PubMed]
7. Weitzman M, Gortmaker S, Sobol A. Maternal smoking and behavior problems of children. Pediatrics1992;90:342-9. [PubMed]
8. Nigg JT, Breslau N. Prenatal smoking exposure, low birth weight, and disruptive behavior disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry2007;46:362-9. [PubMed]
9. Gatzke-Kopp LM, Beauchaine TP. Direct and passive prenatal nicotine exposure and the development of externalizing psychopathology. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev2007;38:255-69. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
10. Monuteaux MC, Blacker D, Biederman J, et al. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring overt and covert conduct problems: a longitudinal study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry2006;47:883-90. [PubMed]
11. Wakschlag LS, Pickett KE, Kasza KE, et al. Is prenatal smoking associated with a developmental pattern of conduct problems in young boys? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry2006;45:461-7. [PubMed]
12. Weissman MM, Warner V, Wickramaratne PJ, et al. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and psychopathology in offspring followed to adulthood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry1999;38:892-9. [PubMed]
13. Fergusson DM, Woodward LJ, Horwood LJ. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and psychiatric adjustment in late adolescence. Arch Gen Psychiatry1998;55:721-7. [PubMed]
14. Wakschlag LS, Lahey BB, Loeber R, et al. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and the risk of conduct disorder in boys. Arch Gen Psychiatry1997;54:670-6. [PubMed]
15. Orlebeke JF, Knol DL, Verhulst FC. Child behavior problems increased by maternal smoking during pregnancy. Arch Environ Health1999;54:15-9. [PubMed]
16. Day NL, Richardson GA, Goldschmidt L, et al. Effects of prenatal tobacco exposure on preschoolers' behavior. J Dev Behav Pediatr2000;21:180-8. [PubMed]
17. Maughan B, Taylor A, Caspi A, et al. Prenatal smoking and early childhood conduct problems: testing genetic and environmental explanations of the association. Arch Gen Psychiatry2004;61:836-43. [PubMed]
18. Brennan PA, Grekin ER, Mednick SA. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and adult male criminal outcomes. Arch Gen Psychiatry1999;56:215-9. [PubMed]
19. Ernst M, Moolchan ET, Robinson ML. Behavioral and neural consequences of prenatal exposure to nicotine. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry2001;40:630-41. [PubMed]
20. Buka SL, Shenassa ED, Niaura R. Elevated risk of tobacco dependence among offspring of mothers who smoked during pregnancy: a 30-year prospective study. Am J Psychiatry2003;160:1978-84. [PubMed]
21. Kodl MM, Wakschlag LS. Does a childhood history of externalizing problems predict smoking during pregnancy? Addict Behav2004;29:273-9. [PubMed]
22. Maughan B, Taylor C, Taylor A, et al. Pregnancy smoking and childhood conduct problems: a causal association? J Child Psychol Psychiatry2001;42:1021-8. [PubMed]
23. Silberg JL, Parr T, Neale MC, et al. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and risk to boys' conduct disturbance: an examination of the causal hypothesis. Biol Psychiatry2003;53:130-5. [PubMed]
24. Hill SY, Lowers L, Locke-Wellman J, et al. Maternal smoking and drinking during pregnancy and the risk for child and adolescent psychiatric disorders. J Stud Alcohol2000;61:661-8. [PubMed]
25. Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Cannon M, et al. Moderation of the effect of adolescent-onset cannabis use on adult psychosis by a functional polymorphism in the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene: longitudinal evidence of a gene X environment interaction. Biol Psychiatry2005;57:1117-27. [PubMed]
26. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington: The Association; 1994.
27. Davidson RJ, Putnam KM, Larson CL. Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation–a possible prelude to violence. Science2000;289:591-4. [PubMed]
28. Hariri AR, Holmes A. Genetics of emotional regulation: the role of the serotonin transporter in neural function. Trends Cogn Sci2006;10:182-91. [PubMed]
29. Urry HL, van Reekum CM, Johnstone T, et al. Amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex are inversely coupled during regulation of negative affect and predict the diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion among older adults. J Neurosci2006;26:4415-25. [PubMed]
30. Rudebeck PH, Buckley MJ, Walton ME, et al. A role for the macaque anterior cingulate gyrus in social valuation. Science2006;313:1310-2. [PubMed]
31. Hadland KA, Rushworth MF, Gaffan D, et al. The effect of cingulate lesions on social behaviour and emotion. Neuropsychologia2003;41:919-31. [PubMed]
32. Rushworth MF, Behrens TE, Rudebeck PH, et al. Contrasting roles for cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex in decisions and social behaviour. Trends Cogn Sci2007;11:168-76. [PubMed]
33. McDonald AJ. Cortical pathways to the mammalian amygdala. Prog Neurobiol1998;55:257-332. [PubMed]
34. Anderson SW, Bechara A, Damasio H, et al. Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci1999;2:1032-7. [PubMed]
35. Harlow J, Harlow JM. Recovery from the passage of an iron bar through the head. Publ Mass Med Soc 1868;2:329-48; Hist Psychiatry1993;4:271-81.
36. Grafman J, Schwab K, Warden D, et al. Frontal lobe injuries, violence, and aggression: a report of the Vietnam Head Injury Study. Neurology1996;46:1231-8. [PubMed]
37. Aggleton JP. The contribution of the amygdala to normal and abnormal emotional states. Trends Neurosci1993;16:328-33. [PubMed]
38. Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio AR. The human amygdala in social judgment. Nature1998;393:470-4. [PubMed]
39. Amen DG, Stubblefield M, Carmicheal B, et al. Brain SPECT findings and aggressiveness. Ann Clin Psychiatry1996;8:129-37. [PubMed]
40. Sterzer P, Stadler C, Krebs A, et al. Abnormal neural responses to emotional visual stimuli in adolescents with conduct disorder. Biol Psychiatry2005;57:7-15. [PubMed]
41. Kruesi MJ, Casanova MF, Mannheim G, et al. Reduced temporal lobe volume in early onset conduct disorder. Psychiatry Res2004;132:1-11. [PubMed]
42. Bauer LO, Hesselbrock VM. CSD/BEM localization of P300 sources in adolescents “at-risk”: evidence of frontal cortex dysfunction in conduct disorder. Biol Psychiatry2001;50:600-8. [PubMed]
43. Nicotra A, Pierucci F, Parvez H, et al. Monoamine oxidase expression during development and aging. Neurotoxicology2004;25:155-65. [PubMed]
44. van Goozen SH, Fairchild G. Neuroendocrine and neurotransmitter correlates in children with antisocial behavior. Horm Behav2006;50:647-54. [PubMed]
45. Rainnie DG. Serotonergic modulation of neurotransmission in the rat basolateral amygdala. J Neurophysiol1999;82:69-85. [PubMed]
46. Harmer CJ, Mackay CE, Reid CB, et al. Antidepressant drug treatment modifies the neural processing of nonconscious threat cues. Biol Psychiatry2006;59:816-20. [PubMed]
47. Harmer CJ, Bhagwagar Z, Perrett DI, et al. Acute SSRI administration affects the processing of social cues in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology2003;28:148-52. [PubMed]
48. Slotkin TA, Pinkerton KE, Tate CA, et al. Alterations of serotonin synaptic proteins in brain regions of neonatal Rhesus monkeys exposed to perinatal environmental tobacco smoke. Brain Res2006;1111:30-5. [PubMed]
49. Whitaker-Azmitia PM. Behavioral and cellular consequences of increasing serotonergic activity during brain development: a role in autism? Int J Dev Neurosci2005;23:75-83. [PubMed]
50. Clarke HF, Dalley JW, Crofts HS, et al. Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion. Science2004;304:878-80. [PubMed]
51. Mehlman PT, Higley JD, Faucher I, et al. Low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations and severe aggression and impaired impulse control in nonhuman primates. Am J Psychiatry1994;151:1485-91. [PubMed]
52. Bjork JM, Dougherty DM, Moeller FG, et al. Differential behavioral effects of plasma tryptophan depletion and loading in aggressive and nonaggressive men. Neuropsychopharmacology2000;22:357-69. [PubMed]
53. van Goozen SH, Fairchild G, Snoek H, et al. The evidence for a neurobiological model of childhood antisocial behavior. Psychol Bull2007;133:149-82. [PubMed]
54. Kruesi MJ, Hibbs ED, Zahn TP, et al. A 2-year prospective follow-up study of children and adolescents with disruptive behavior disorders. Prediction by cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid, homovanillic acid, and autonomic measures? Arch Gen Psychiatry1992;49:429-35. [PubMed]
55. Mitsis EM, Halperin JM, Newcorn JH. Serotonin and aggression in children. Curr Psychiatry Rep2000;2:95-101. [PubMed]
56. Raine A, Lencz T, Bihrle S, et al. Reduced prefrontal gray matter volume and reduced autonomic activity in antisocial personality disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry2000;57:119-27; discussion 128-9. [PubMed]
57. Zuckerman M, Kuhlman DM. Personality and risk-taking: common biosocial factors. J Pers2000;68:999-1029. [PubMed]
58. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Buckholtz JW, Kolachana B, et al. Neural mechanisms of genetic risk for impulsivity and violence in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A2006;103:6269-74. [PubMed]
59. Berman ME, Tracy JI, Coccaro EF. The serotonin hypothesis of aggression revisited. Clin Psychol Rev1997;17:651-65. [PubMed]
60. Falk L, Nordberg A, Seiger A, et al. The alpha7 nicotinic receptors in human fetal brain and spinal cord. J Neurochem2002;80:457-65. [PubMed]
61. Lenroot RK, Giedd JN. Brain development in children and adolescents: insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev2006;30:718-29. [PubMed]
62. Fowler JS, Volkow ND, Wang GJ, et al. Brain monoamine oxidase A inhibition in cigarette smokers. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A1996;93:14065-9. [PubMed]
63. Park MK, Loughlin SE, Leslie FM. Gestational nicotine-induced changes in adolescent neuronal activity. Brain Res2006;1094:119-26. [PubMed]
64. Slotkin TA, Tate CA, Cousins MM, et al. Prenatal nicotine exposure alters the responses to subsequent nicotine administration and withdrawal in adolescence: serotonin receptors and cell signaling. Neuropsychopharmacology2006;31:2462-75. [PubMed]
65. Chen WJ, Parnell SE, West JR. Neonatal alcohol and nicotine exposure limits brain growth and depletes cerebellar Purkinje cells. Alcohol1998;15:33-41. [PubMed]
66. Trauth JA, Seidler FJ, McCook EC, et al. Persistent c-fos induction by nicotine in developing rat brain regions: interaction with hypoxia. Pediatr Res1999;45:38-45. [PubMed]
67. Huang LZ, Winzer-Serhan UH. Chronic neonatal nicotine upregulates heteromeric nicotinic acetylcholine receptor binding without change in subunit mRNA expression. Brain Res2006;1113:94-109. [PubMed]
68. Liu Z, Neff RA, Berg DK. Sequential interplay of nicotinic and GABAergic signaling guides neuronal development. Science2006;314:1610-3. [PubMed]
69. Represa A, Ben-Ari Y. Trophic actions of GABA on neuronal development. Trends Neurosci2005;28:278-83. [PubMed]
70. Lauder JM, Liu J, Devaud L, et al. GABA as a trophic factor for developing monoamine neurons. Perspect Dev Neurobiol1998;5:247-59. [PubMed]
71. Lips KS, Bruggmann D, Pfeil U, et al. Nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in rat and human placenta. Placenta2005;26:735-46. [PubMed]
72. Wiggins RC, Fuller G, Enna SJ. Undernutrition and the development of brain neurotransmitter systems. Life Sci1984;35:2085-94. [PubMed]
73. Grunau RE, Whitfield MF, Fay TB. Psychosocial and academic characteristics of extremely low birth weight (< or = 800 g) adolescents who are free of major impairment compared with term-born control subjects. Pediatrics2004;114:e725-32. [PubMed]
74. Law KL, Stroud LR, LaGasse LL, et al. Smoking during pregnancy and newborn neurobehavior. Pediatrics2003;111:1318-23. [PubMed]
75. Pennington SN, Sandstrom LP, Shibley IA Jr, et al. Biochemical changes, early brain growth suppression and impaired detour learning in nicotine-treated chicks. Brain Res Dev Brain Res1994;83:181-9.
76. von Ziegler NI, Schlumpf M, Lichtensteiger W. Prenatal nicotine exposure selectively affects perinatal forebrain aromatase activity and fetal adrenal function in male rats. Brain Res Dev Brain Res1991;62:23-31.
77. Mamiya N, Buchanan R, Wallace T, et al. Nicotine suppresses the P13 auditory evoked potential by acting on the pedunculopontine nucleus in the rat. Exp Brain Res2005;164:109-19. [PubMed]
78. Good CH, Bay KD, Buchanan RA, et al. Prenatal exposure to cigarette smoke affects the physiology of pedunculopontine nucleus (PPN) neurons in development. Neurotoxicol Teratol2006;28:210-9. [PubMed]
79. Swan GE, Lessov-Schlaggar CN. The effects of tobacco smoke and nicotine on cognition and the brain. Neuropsychol Rev2007;17:259-73. [PubMed]
80. Fowles J, Dybing E. Application of toxicological risk assessment principles to the chemical constituents of cigarette smoke. Tob Control2003;12:424-30. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
81. Castagnoli K, Steyn SJ, Magnin G, et al. Studies on the interactions of tobacco leaf and tobacco smoke constituents and monoamine oxidase. Neurotox Res2002;4:151-60. [PubMed]
82. Breyer-Pfaff U, Wiatr G, Stevens I, et al. Elevated norharman plasma levels in alcoholic patients and controls resulting from tobacco smoking. Life Sci1996;58:1425-32. [PubMed]
83. Hara S, Mukai T, Kurosaki K, et al. Modification of the striatal dopaminergic neuron system by carbon monoxide exposure in free-moving rats, as determined by in vivo brain microdialysis. Arch Toxicol2002;76:596-605. [PubMed]
84. Adachi M, Kasai T, Adachi T, et al. Effect of cigarette smoke on rabbit corneal cells in vitro. Tohoku J Exp Med1966;90:41-51. [PubMed]
85. Norman TR, Chamberlain KG, French MA, et al. Platelet monoamine oxidase activity and cigarette smoking. J Affect Disord1982;4:73-7. [PubMed]
86. Berlin I, Said S, Spreux-Varoquaux O, et al. Monoamine oxidase A and B activities in heavy smokers. Biol Psychiatry1995;38:756-61. [PubMed]
87. Fowler JS, Logan J, Wang GJ, et al. Low monoamine oxidase B in peripheral organs in smokers. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A2003;100:11600-5. [PubMed]
88. Fowler JS, Logan J, Wang GJ, et al. Comparison of monoamine oxidase A in peripheral organs in nonsmokers and smokers. J Nucl Med2005;46:1414-20. [PubMed]
89. Fowler JS, Logan J, Volkow ND, et al. Translational neuroimaging: positron emission tomography studies of monoamine oxidase. Mol Imaging Biol2005;7:377-87. [PubMed]
90. Fowler JS, Volkow ND, Wang GJ, et al. Inhibition of monoamine oxidase B in the brains of smokers. Nature1996;379:733-6. [PubMed]
91. Shih JC, Grimsby J, Chen K. The expression of human MAO-A and B genes. J Neural Transm Suppl1990;32:41-7. [PubMed]
92. Lewinsohn R, Glover V, Sandler M. Development of benzylamine oxidase and monoamine oxidase A and B in man. Biochem Pharmacol1980;29:1221-30. [PubMed]
93. Shih JC, Chen K. MAO-A and -B gene knock-out mice exhibit distinctly different behavior. Neurobiology (Bp)1999;7:235-46. [PubMed]
94. Cases O, Seif I, Grimsby J, et al. Aggressive behavior and altered amounts of brain serotonin and norepinephrine in mice lacking MAOA. Science1995;268:1763-6. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
95. Mejia JM, Ervin FR, Baker GB, et al. Monoamine oxidase inhibition during brain development induces pathological aggressive behavior in mice. Biol Psychiatry2002;52:811-21. [PubMed]
96. Whitaker-Azmitia PM, Zhang X, Clarke C. Effects of gestational exposure to monoamine oxidase inhibitors in rats: preliminary behavioral and neurochemical studies. Neuropsychopharmacology1994;11:125-32. [PubMed]
97. Shih JC. Cloning, after cloning, knock-out mice, and physiological functions of MAO A and B. Neurotoxicology2004;25:21-30. [PubMed]
98. Evrard A, Malagie I, Laporte AM, et al. Altered regulation of the 5-HT system in the brain of MAO-A knock-out mice. Eur J Neurosci2002;15:841-51. [PubMed]
99. Salichon N, Gaspar P, Upton AL, et al. Excessive activation of serotonin (5-HT) 1B receptors disrupts the formation of sensory maps in monoamine oxidase a and 5-ht transporter knock-out mice. J Neurosci2001;21:884-96. [PubMed]
100. Rebsam A, Seif I, Gaspar P. Refinement of thalamocortical arbors and emergence of barrel domains in the primary somatosensory cortex: a study of normal and monoamine oxidase a knock-out mice. J Neurosci2002;22:8541-52. [PubMed]
101. Kim-Cohen J, Caspi A, Taylor A, et al. MAOA, maltreatment, and gene-environment interaction predicting children's mental health: new evidence and a meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry2006;11:903-13. [PubMed]
102. Brunner HG, Nelen M, Breakefield XO, et al. Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase A. Science1993;262:578-80. [PubMed]
103. Caspi A, McClay J, Moffitt TE, et al. Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science2002;297:851-4. [PubMed]
104. Eley TC, Lichtenstein P, Stevenson J. Sex differences in the etiology of aggressive and nonaggressive antisocial behavior: results from two twin studies. Child Dev1999;70:155-68. [PubMed]
105. Cadoret RJ, Yates WR, Troughton E, et al. Genetic-environmental interaction in the genesis of aggressivity and conduct disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry1995;52:916-24. [PubMed]
106. Gelhorn H, Stallings M, Young S, et al. Common and specific genetic influences on aggressive and nonaggressive conduct disorder domains. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry2006;45:570-7. [PubMed]
107. Pinsonneault JK, Papp AC, Sadee W. Allelic mRNA expression of X-linked monoamine oxidase a (MAOA) in human brain: dissection of epigenetic and genetic factors. Hum Mol Genet2006;15:2636-49. [PubMed]
108. Sabol SZ, Hu S, Hamer D. A functional polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene promoter. Hum Genet1998;103:273-9. [PubMed]
109. Popova NK. From genes to aggressive behavior: the role of serotonergic system. Bioessays2006;28:495-503. [PubMed]
110. Eisenberger NI, Way BM, Taylor SE, et al. Understanding genetic risk for aggression: clues from the brain's response to social exclusion. Biol Psychiatry2007;61:1100-8. [PubMed]
111. Manuck SB, Flory JD, Ferrell RE, et al. A regulatory polymorphism of the monoamine oxidase-A gene may be associated with variability in aggression, impulsivity, and central nervous system serotonergic responsivity. Psychiatry Res2000;95:9-23. [PubMed]
112. Manor I, Tyano S, Mel E, et al. Family-based and association studies of monoamine oxidase A and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): preferential transmission of the long promoter-region repeat and its association with impaired performance on a continuous performance test (TOVA). Mol Psychiatry2002;7:626-32. [PubMed]
113. Passamonti L, Fera F, Magariello A, et al. Monoamine oxidase-A genetic variations influence brain activity associated with inhibitory control: new insight into the neural correlates of impulsivity. Biol Psychiatry2006;59:334-40. [PubMed]
114. Newman TK, Syagailo YV, Barr CS, et al. Monoamine oxidase A gene promoter variation and rearing experience influences aggressive behavior in rhesus monkeys. Biol Psychiatry2005;57:167-72. [PubMed]
115. Fowler JS, Alia-Klein N, Kriplani A, et al. Evidence that brain MAO A activity does not correspond to MAO A genotype in healthy male subjects. Biol Psychiatry2007;62:355-8. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
116. Balciuniene J, Emilsson L, Oreland L, et al. Investigation of the functional effect of monoamine oxidase polymorphisms in human brain. Hum Genet2002;110:1-7. [PubMed]
117. Shioe K, Ichimiya T, Suhara T, et al. No association between genotype of the promoter region of serotonin transporter gene and serotonin transporter binding in human brain measured by PET. Synapse2003;48:184-8. [PubMed]
118. Buznikov GA, Lambert HW, Lauder JM. Serotonin and serotonin-like substances as regulators of early embryogenesis and morphogenesis. Cell Tissue Res2001;305:177-86. [PubMed]
119. Luo X, Persico AM, Lauder JM. Serotonergic regulation of somatosensory cortical development: lessons from genetic mouse models. Dev Neurosci2003;25:173-83. [PubMed]
120. Dodge KA, Pettit GS. A biopsychosocial model of the development of chronic conduct problems in adolescence. Dev Psychol2003;39:349-71. [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Articles from Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience : JPN are provided here courtesy of Canadian Medical Association