PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptHHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
 
Biol Psychiatry. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 March 15.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC2268873
NIHMSID: NIHMS41617

The Genetic Covariation between Fear Conditioning and Self-Report Fears

Abstract

Background

Fear conditioning is a traditional model for the acquisition of phobias, while behavioral therapies utilize processes underlying extinction to treat phobic and other anxiety disorders. Furthermore, fear conditioning has been proposed as an endophenotype for genetic studies of anxiety disorders. While prior studies have demonstrated that fear conditioning and self-report fears are heritable, no studies have determined whether they share a common genetic basis.

Methods

We obtained fear conditioning data from 173 twin pairs from the Swedish Twin Registry who also provided self-report ratings of 16 common fears. Using multivariate structural equation modeling, we analyzed factor-derived scores for the subjective fear ratings together with the electrophysiologic skin conductance responses during habituation, acquisition, and extinction to determine the extent of their genetic covariation.

Results

Phenotypic correlations between experimental and self-report fear measures were modest and, and counter-intuitively, negative; that is, subjects who reported themselves as more fearful had smaller electrophysiologic responses. Best-fit models estimated a significant (negative) genetic correlation between them, although genetic factors underlying fear conditioning accounted for only 9% of individual differences in self-report fears.

Conclusions

Experimentally-derived fear conditioning measures share only a small portion of the genetic factors underlying individual differences in subjective fears, cautioning against relying too heavily on the former as an endophenotype for genetic studies of phobic disorders.

Keywords: twin study, psychophysiology, fears, phobias, endophenotype, fear conditioning

Introduction

Anxiety disorders in general, and phobias in particular, are highly prevalent in the general population (1). Phobias have been shown to be familial (25), and results of several small twin studies have suggested that genetic factors play an important role in a wide range of self-report phobic fears (69). The only large twin sample that has comprehensively examined the genetics of phobias in adults is the Virginia Twin Registry. Analyses from that sample have found that genetics is largely responsible for twin resemblance in females (10) and males (11), with reliability-corrected estimates of heritability that ranged from 0.46 for situational phobia to 0.67 for agoraphobia (12). Two other broad findings emerged from these analyses. First, it is likely that self-report fears and clinically-derived phobia diagnoses fall along the same liability continuum. That is, it is likely that there is substantial overlap in the genetic factors that underlie susceptibility to normal fears and pathological phobias. Second, multivariate modeling found evidence for a common genetic factor for “phobia proneness” that increased liability across classes of phobic fears, although this is somewhat at odds with the results of family studies (13).

Fear conditioning, a basic form of associative learning, is a traditional model for the acquisition of fears and phobias (14). Also, fear conditioning has long been an important experimental method for the study of etiological processes related to fear and anxiety (15). Behavioral treatment paradigms for anxiety disorders, such as exposure and response prevention, are based on the related processes of habituation and extinction. Habituation allows individuals to ignore innocuous events by producing a progressive decline in response to repeated presentations of a neutral stimulus via non-associative learning. Fear conditioning occurs when fear is associated with a neutral (conditioned) stimulus (CS) after pairing with a fear-provoking (unconditioned) stimulus (UCS) such as electric shock. The CS acquires the fear-provoking response attributes of the UCS. Extinction is the decremental response to further presentations of the CS when it is no longer paired with the UCS. A recent review and meta-analysis of fear conditioning in anxiety disorders reported significant differences between patients and control subjects for electrophysiologic responses during fear conditioning paradigms (16).

In a prior analysis, we demonstrated that the electrodermal skin conductance response (SCR) amplitudes seen in response to the habituation, acquisition, and extinction components of the classical fear conditioning paradigm are moderately heritable and share a substantial proportion of their underlying genetic factors (17). Similarly, Crider et al. showed that nonspecific electrodermal response (EDR) lability and speed of habituation exhibited genetic covariation, with heritability similar to our estimate for SCR (40–50%). The finding that experimental measures of fear conditioning are heritable and possess heritability estimates that are in the same range as those found in twin studies of anxiety disorders (5) has potentially important implications. There have been suggestions that clinical phenotypes may not reflect the underlying pathophysiological processes involved in the etiology of psychiatric disorders in general (18) and for anxiety disorders in particular (19), and, therefore, may not provide optimum targets for gene-finding strategies. One approach to overcome this limitation is to investigate potential endophenotypes that more robustly reflect processes more proximal to gene expression and are involved in the development of pathological anxiety states. In their 2003 review of the subject, Gottesman and Gould suggested a set of criteria that a putative endophenotypic measure must satisfy, including association with illness, heritability, and co-segregation with the illness in families (20). As summarized above, fear conditioning, as a potential anxiety disorder endophenotype, is both associated with anxiety pathology and is heritable. No studies have examined whether there is a genetic association (“co-segregation”) between fear conditioning and anxiety-related phenotypes. To this end, the present study attempts to elucidate the potential genetic covariation between experimentally-derived electrophysiologic measures of fear conditioning and self-report phobic fears.

Methods and Materials

Subjects

Subjects were twin pairs recruited by mailed inquiry from the Swedish Twin Registry after approval from the Swedish Twin Board. The Ethical Committee of Karolinska Hospital approved this study. The sample consisted of 90 monozygotic (MZ) and 83 dizygotic (DZ) same-sex twin pairs aged 25–38, comprised of 62 male-male and 111 female-female pairs. Zygosity determination was based upon questionnaires of physical similarity that were validated by serological measures in 96% of cases (21).

Electrophysiologic Measures

Details of the fear conditioning paradigm were presented previously (17). The outcome measures were electrodermal skin conductance response (SCR) amplitudes recorded at discrete intervals during the presentation sequences. Subjects were seated in an armchair two meters from a projection screen on which sequences of pictorial stimuli serving as CS were shown for 8-second intervals in habituation, acquisition, and extinction phases. For the habituation phase, two slides were displayed two times each. During the acquisition phase, there were eight presentations of each slide, where one (CS+) but not the other (CS−) was paired with the UCS. A mild 18 Hz AC electric current applied to each subject’s right index and middle fingers for duration of 0.5 second, individually adjusted to be “uncomfortable but not painful”, served as the UCS. This was followed by the extinction phase, in which both slides were presented again eight times each, but without pairing with the UCS. The maximum SCR measured in microsiemens starting 1–4 seconds after the stimulus onset was recorded (22;23), as this measure has been shown to provide the best internal consistency and stability to assess individual differences in conditioning (24).

Self-report Fear Measures

The main self-report fear measures included in our study were based on a questionnaire developed by us (25). Subjects were asked to rate the intensity of their fears of 16 different objects and situations on a Visual Analogue Scales (VAS) scale from 0 (no fear) to 100 (maximal fears). Treated as a quantitative variable in our analyses, such an ordinal scale provides more power for examining the relationship between fears and conditioning than a categorical variable like having a phobia or not. And, as discussed in the introduction, self report fear symptoms appear to lie on the same liability continuum as phobias. We did, however, perform exploratory analyses at the level of phobia diagnoses. For each of the 16 fear items, we rated a subject as meeting criteria for a phobia according to the standard DSM-IV subtype categorizations (animal, situational, social, etc.) (26) if (1) their VAS score was in the upper 90th percentile, (2) they perceived their fear to be excessive, and (3) they avoided the feared object or situation. In addition to these phobic fears, we asked the subjects to rate their long-term propensity towards anxiety using the Trait portion of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (27).

Statistical Analyses

SCR measures, averaged over the separate presentations for each of the five phase-pairing conditions (habituation, acquisition CS−, acquisition CS+, extinction CS−, and extinction CS+), were the main electrophysiological variables included in our analyses. Several preliminary steps were undertaken to suitably process the data before entering it into the main analyses (see Supplement for details]. In addition to using these averaged SCR variables, we also examined the slope of each of the five measures as they varied across the sequence of presentation trials (4 for habituation and 8 each for the other four measures). The slopes contain information on the decremental rates of response with repeated presentations of the same stimulus, and thus, relate to speed of habituation. The slopes were extracted from linear regressions performed by subject in the SAS PROC GENMOD routine (28) and substituted for the averaged SCR amplitudes in correlation analyses with the self-report measures.

Due to rightward skew of their distributions, we applied a square-root transformation to the VAS self-report fear ratings and the STAI. Each of the 16 fear items was significantly correlated with the others (~.2-.4), suggesting that they may make up a higher order fear factor within each subject that we denote “self-report fearfulness (SRF)”. This is consistent with twin studies reviewed above that found evidence for a common genetic factor for “phobia proneness” that increases liability across classes of phobic fears. Therefore, in order to explore the effects of this composite SRF as well as to reduce the number of individual tests, we performed a factor analysis on the 16 fear items using SAS PROC FACTOR to extract the first principal component corresponding to this SRF for use in analyses with the electrophysiologic measures.

Within-subject (phenotypic) product moment correlations between electrophysiologic and self-report fear measures were calculated. We used Huber-White corrected significance tests to account for dependency of the observations due to the twin pairs. Twin resemblance within and between measures was estimated by the polychoric correlation separately by zygosity. For those electrophysiologic-fear variable combinations with significant correlations, we performed structural equation modeling to estimate the sources of the correlation (genetic and environmental) (29). Under the basic twin model, individual differences in liability for the phenotype of interest are assumed to arise from four sources: additive genes (A), genetic dominance (D), common or familial environment (C) (e.g., social class or rearing), and individual-specific environment (E) (e.g., marital discord). Also, because we had already determined that only A and E significantly contributed to individual differences in SCR (17), and fear symptoms and phobias have similarly been shown to depend primarily on A and E in adults (5), we began our analyses with AE baseline models. We fit the models with maximum likelihood estimation of the contributions of A and E using the raw data option of the Mx statistical modeling program (30). These are expressed in terms of each factor’s proportion of total variance in liability as a2 and e2, respectively, where a and e represent the factor loadings (path coefficients) onto the phenotype.

Multivariate structural equation modeling was used to assess genetic and environmental liabilities shared between the measures. We chose a reduced (unsaturated) Cholesky to allow us to test a particular hypothesis about the covariation between electrophysiologic and self-report fear measures. (See Supplement for background on Cholesky models.) Our hypothesis is that there are only three genetic factors needed to explain the six phenotypes in the data. The first (A1) corresponds to genes that may be common to all of the measures, i.e., A1 underlies the fear conditioning electrophysiologic responses seen during habituation, acquisition, and extinction that may be shared with SRF. The second (A2) models associative learning that takes place during the fear conditioning process in the acquisition and extinction phases, which may also be shared with SRF. These two reflect our prior finding that two genetic factors are sufficient to account for individual differences in the five electrophysiologic response variables (17). Finally, we postulated a third factor (A6 – corresponding to the sixth measured variable, SRF) that accounts for individual variation in self-report fears not accounted for by the factors explaining the electrophysiologic responses. Note that it is the correlations derived from loadings of the first two factors on the self-report fears that, if significant, suggest a common genetic basis between fear conditioning and SRF. Also, we need to include six E factors in the model, as these subsume measurement error for each variable. Model parameters and indices that characterize the fit of this model are calculated, and this model is compared with sub-models created by eliminating or constraining parameters in a step-wise fashion, testing specific hypotheses about the data. Twice the difference in log-likelihood between a higher-order and sub-model yields a statistic that is asymptotically distributed as χ2 with degrees of freedom equal to the difference in their number of parameters. This permits one to use a χ2 difference test to determine if a statistically significant difference exists between models. We used Akaike’s information criterion (AIC) (31) for model selection. The lower its value, the better is the balance between explanatory power and parsimony.

Results

Factor analysis of the VAS ratings for the 16 fear items produced three significant factors with (eigenvalues, proportions of variance) of (4.91, 31%), (1.65, 10%), and (1.22, 8%), respectively. The principal components output from the FACTOR procedure are listed in Table 1. We selected the first factor, which we designated as SRF for “self-report fearfulness”, for further analysis, as it possessed substantial loadings (> 0.5) on most of the individual fear items. Table 2 shows within-subject (phenotypic) Pearson product moment correlations between the self-report fear measures and the five electrophysiologic SCR measures. Note that almost all of the correlations are negative, significantly so for 25 out of the 80 pairings. Phenotypic correlations between SRF and each electrophysiologic measure were modest but significant, prompting us to use this measure in the modeling portion of the analyses.

Table 1
Principal Components Analysis on 16 Fear Items
Table 2
Within-Subject (Phenotypic) Pearson Product Moment Correlations between Self-Report Fears and Five Electrophysiologic Measures

The numbers (and proportions) of subjects who met our criteria for phobias are (by subtype): animal – 17 (4.91%), natural environment – 22 (6.36%), situational – 12 (3.47%), agoraphobia – 8 (2.31%), blood/injection/dental – 28 (8.09%), and social 10 (2.89%). Only 3 (all negative) of 36 correlations between dichotomous phobia diagnoses and electrophysiologic measures were significantly different from zero, so we excluded these from further analyses. Similarly, all of the correlations between STAI and the electrophysiologic measures were negative but not significantly different from zero, so these were not pursued further.

Table 3 lists, separately by zygosity, the twin polychoric correlations between SRF and the five electrophysiologic measures, with values for monozygotic (MZ) pairs above the diagonal (shaded) and dizygotic (DZ) below the diagonal (unshaded). The upper left and lower right blocks contain the within-twin (twin1-twin1, twin2-twin2) correlations between any of the six measures while the lower left and upper right contain the cross-twin (twin1-twin2, twin2-twin1) correlations. It is the latter (“twin similarity”) that determines the proportion of genetic variance. The magnitude of each of the MZ cross-twin, within-trait correlations exceeds that of the corresponding DZ correlation, suggesting that each of the six measures is heritable, as has been found before. For example, the MZ T1SRF-T2SRF correlation is 0.55, while its DZ correlation is 0.20. Each of the five electrophysiologic measures is positively correlated with the others and negatively correlated with SRF, again with MZ correlations exceeding DZ, suggesting genetic covariance between the measures. For example, the MZ T1HAB-T2ACQ+ correlation is 0.37 and its DZ correlation is 0.24, while the MZ T1SRF-T2EXT- correlation is -0.23 and its DZ correlation is -0.09.

Table 3
Twin Correlations by Zygosity between Self-Report Fear Factor and Five Electrophysiologic Measures (MZ upper/shaded; DZ lower/unshaded)

Table 4 provides results from the multivariate modeling of the six measures. Again, these are restricted to contain only additive genetic (A) and individual specific environmental (E) sources of variance as indicated by prior analyses. Model I is a saturated 6-factor Cholesky constructed for model fit comparison. Model II is a reduced Cholesky with only three additive genetic factors (A1, A2, A6). It fits the data almost as well as the saturated Cholesky (Δ χ2=4.28, Δ df=9, p=.89) with lower AIC, making it the model we used for further comparisons. Model III differs from model II in that the third genetic factor (A6) was removed (constrained to zero), testing whether all of the genetic variation underlying SRF can be explained by the genetic factors for electrophysiologic responding (A1 and A2). We could definitively reject model III by a chi-squared difference test (p=.00004). Models IV and V test whether there is significant genetic correlation between SRF and A1 and A2, respectively (i.e., do either A1 or A2 explain any of the genetic variance of SRF). While we could again reject model IV by a chi-squared difference test (p=.0092), we just barely lacked the statistical power to discriminate between model II and model V (Δχ2=3.22, Δdf=1, p=.073). However, model II possessed the lowest AIC, making it best fitting by that criterion.

Table 4
Goodness-of-Fit Statistics for Multivariate Analysis of Five Electrophysiologic Measures plus Self-Report Fear Factor (SRF)

We present the parameter estimates for model II in Figure 1. The proportions of variance for the six variables due to the three genetic factors included in this model are shown in Table 5. Looking at the “Total” column in Table 5, additive genetics accounts for about 50% of the total variance for each of the measures. A1 and A2 account for only 6% and 3%, respectively, of the total variance, or about 11% and 5%, respectively, of the genetic variance of SRF. Note that all of the genetic correlations between SRF and the electrophysiologic measures are negative, in the range -0.3 and -0.4 (corresponding to the negative loadings of A1 and A2 on SRF).

Figure 1
Best-fit three genetic factor model (model II from Table 4). Only additive genetic (A) sources of variance and parameter estimates are shown for simplicity. A1 represents a general set of genetic factors that underlie individual differences in all of ...
Table 5
Proportion of Variance Accounted for by Additive Genetic Factors

When we examined the relationship between the regression slope of each of the five electrophysiologic measures with the 16 self-report fear items, we found only three correlations significant at the P<0.05 level (two positive and one negative). Furthermore, none of the correlations between the slopes and SRF was significantly different from zero, so we performed no further analysis using the slopes.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to investigate the potential genetic covariation between experimentally-derived electrophysiologic measures of fear conditioning and self-report fear measures using the twin method. We used the skin conductance response (SCR) amplitude averaged over the five phase-pairing conditions as the primary indicator variable for fear conditioning and a self-report fear (SRF) factor derived from a factor analysis of the VAS ratings of 16 fear items to indicate a subject’s general level of fearfulness. Correlation analyses and multivariate structural equation modeling were used to analyze the data.

As indicated in Table 2, modest negative phenotypic correlations were found between the majority of the VAS fear items (and the SRF derived from them) and the SCR measures. Breaking these correlations down by zygosity in Table 3, the larger cross-twin, cross trait negative correlations in MZ (upper triangle) versus DZ (lower triangle) indicates that these negative phenotypic correlations are likely derived from negative genetic correlations. This was substantiated in the modeling results (Table 4), where significant genetic covariation is seen between SRF and the five electrophysiological measures. The fact that the magnitudes of genetic correlations exceed that of the phenotypic correlations can be explained by the fact that smaller, positive environmental correlations (range 0.1-0.2) between them offset the negative genetic correlations to produce the smaller phenotypic correlations. Thus, small phenotypic correlations do not necessarily imply small genetic correlations.

Despite the existence of significant genetic correlations between SRF and the SCR measures (about -0.4), Table 5 indicates that only about 9% (0.06+0.03) of the total phenotypic variation for SRF is explained by the genetic factors A1 and A2 underlying the SCR measures, while, in total, genetic factors account for about 55% of individual variation in SRF. Thus, at least according to our data, the majority of the genetic factors underlying self-report fearfulness are not shared with SCR. Emotions have often been postulated as consisting of a subjective, a behavioral, and a neuro-physiological component (e.g. (32)). These are loosely coupled and do not always substantially correlate. If replicated, the present data suggest that electrophysiologic response during fear conditioning may be unsuitable as an endophenotypic marker for genetic investigations of pathologic fear states.

The finding of negative phenotypic and genetic correlations between self-report fears and electrophysiologic measures is, at face value, counterintuitive. That is, the more fearful a subject reported him/herself to be, the less reactive they were in the fear conditioning protocol. This is not an isolated finding among non-clinical samples. In their study of stress and fear conditioning, Jackson et al. reported large (>.43), negative correlations between trait anger, trait depression, trait anxiety, and neuroticism and differential fear conditioning responses in their female control (non-stressed) subjects (33). Similarly, Wilken et al. reported that subjects with low trait anxiety (as measured by the Trait portion of the STAI) exhibited greater electrodermal activity in response to stressful stimuli that those with high trait anxiety (34;35). “Non-intuitive” relationships with trait anxiety are not limited to peripheral autonomic measures. A recent functional imaging study examining the relationship between NEO personality measures and brain activity reported a highly significant negative correlation between NEO neuroticism and resting regional cerebral glucose metabolism in the left insular cortex, a region implicated in anxiety disorders (36).

In the only study identified examining a physiologic measure and a range of self-report phobic fears, Kawachi et al. observed decreased heart rate variability in subjects with higher levels of phobic fears (37).This finding is similar to those from a series of studies by Hoehn-Saric and colleagues in clinical samples, who report decreased autonomic flexibility in patients with several different anxiety disorders (38;39). This reduced flexibility was primarily indicated by decreased heart rate variability in patients compared with controls, but one study reported relatively weaker SCR and narrower range of both SCR and heart rate in women with generalized anxiety disorder during a psychological stress task (40). Interestingly, a recent review and meta-analysis of fear conditioning in anxiety disorders reported overall modest increases in patient compared with control subject electrophysiologic responses (16). This meta-analysis, however, is predominantly composed of studies of anxiety disorders other than phobias, and only social phobia is represented among them. It is unclear how to reconcile these diverse findings, although one may speculate that there may be qualitative differences in measures of autonomic response between highly fearful “normal” individuals and patients with anxiety disorders.

Our finding of significant association between electrophysiologic measures and self-report fear ratings was limited to the fear items, although there was a trend for a similarly negative correlation with subject trait anxiety via the STAI. In addition, few of the correlations between dichotomous phobia diagnoses and the electrophysiologic SCR measures were significantly different from zero, although those that were also were negative. In general, dichotomous measures possess lower power to detect deviations from the null hypothesis than continuous measures, and, although a fair proportion of subjects qualified for a phobic diagnosis of some kind by our definition, the absolute numbers were small. It is also somewhat surprising that we found no association between self-report fears and rates of response decrement for any of the electrophysiologic measures. There is a genetic component to habituation (41), and patients with anxiety disorders tend to show retarded habituation, particularly to anxiogenic but also to neutral stimuli (42;43). Here again, there may be qualitative differences between normal individuals with high trait anxiety and patients who suffer from anxiety disorders. Well-powered studies that directly compare electrophysiologic measures in a sample of normal subjects with a sufficiently wide range of self-reported anxiety measures as well as patients with anxiety disorders may resolve these differences.

Limitations

The results of this study are subject to several potential limitations. First, due to the immense effort and expense required for this experimental protocol, the sample size is relatively small compared to recent epidemiological twin studies, limiting the power to significantly distinguish potential sources of variance. Second, the maximum likelihood method used by Mx in the twin models assumes that the variables follow a multivariate normal distribution, which we were only able to approximate via transformation procedures. Third, the validity of interpreting increased similarity of MZ over DZ twins as representing genetic effects is predicated upon the equal environment assumption, which states that MZ and DZ twins are equally correlated for environmental experiences of relevance to the trait under study (reviewed in (44)). Although there have been several twin studies designed to detect such violations with negative findings (45) (46), we do not have any direct means of establishing the assumption’s validity in this sample. Fourth, for analyses involving dichotomous phobias, we constructed diagnoses from several self-report items involving excessiveness and avoidance rather than performing a clinical interview. Finally, the sample is limited to Swedish Caucasian subjects, and as such, the results may not generalize to other ethnic groups.

Conclusions

We found evidence for modest-sized, negative phenotypic and genetic correlations between electrophysiologic fear conditioning measures and self-report fears. While correlations with trait anxiety and phobia diagnoses were also generally negative, they were statistically non-significant. Future family or twin studies in larger samples are needed to confirm the magnitude and direction of the familial co-aggregation of fear conditioning and anxiety disorders before the former can be reliably proposed as an endophenotype for genetic studies of the latter.

Supplementary Material

Acknowledgments

Data collection was supported by the Swedish Council for Research in the Humanities and Social Sciences and the Bank of Sweden Tercentenary Foundation to Dr. Fredrikson. The data analysis and manuscript preparation was supported by a Junior Faculty Research Grant from the Anxiety Disorders Association of America and NIH grant K08 MH66277 to Dr. Hettema. Dr. Neale was supported by NIMH grant MH-65322. We wish to thank Drs. Bruce Cuthbert, Christian Grillion, Danny Pine, Scott Vrana, Peter Lang, Charles Gardner, Steven Aggen, and Jonathan Kuhn for useful discussions or suggestions regarding this study.

Financial Disclosures None.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Reference List

1. Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Zhao S, Nelson CB, Hughes M, Eshleman S, Wittchen HU, Kendler KS. Lifetime and 12-Month Prevalence of DSM-III-R Psychiatric Disorders in the United States: Results From the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1994;51:8–19. [PubMed]
2. Fyer AJ, Mannuzza S, Gallops MS, Martin LY, Aaronson C, Gorman JM, Liebowitz MR, Klein DF. Familial Transmission of Simple Phobias and Fears. A Preliminary Report. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47(3):252–6. [PubMed]
3. Fyer AJ, Mannuzza S, Chapman TF, Liebowitz MR, Klein DF. A Direct Interview Family Study of Social Phobia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1993;50:286–93. [PubMed]
4. Stein MB, Chartier MJ, Hazen AL, Kozak MV, Tancer ME, Lander S, Furer P, Chubaty D, Walker JR. A Direct-Interview Family Study of Generalized Social Phobia. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155(1):90–7. [PubMed]
5. Hettema JM, Neale MC, Kendler KS. A Review and Meta-Analysis of the Genetic Epidemiology of Anxiety Disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(10):1568–78. [PubMed]
6. Torgersen S. The Nature and Origin of Common Phobic Fears. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1979;134:343–51. [PubMed]
7. Carey G, Gottesman II. Twin and family studies of anxiety, phobic, and obsessive disorders. In: Klein DF, Rabkin J, editors. Anxiety: New research and changing concepts. New York: Raven Press; 1981. pp. 117–36.
8. Rose RJ, Miller JZ, Pogue-Geile MF, Cardwell GF. Twin-family studies of common fears and phobias. In: Gedda L, Parisi P, Nance WE, editors. Twin Research 3: Intelligence, Personality, and Development. New York: Alan R. Liss, Inc.; 1981. pp. 169–74.
9. Skre I, Onstad S, Torgersen S, Philos DR, Lygren S, Kringlen E. The Heritability of Common Phobic Fear: a Twin Study of a Clinical Sample. J Anxiety Disord. 2000;14(6):549–62. [PubMed]
10. Kendler KS, Neale MC, Kessler RC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ. The Genetic Epidemiology of Phobias in Women. The Interrelationship of Agoraphobia, Social Phobia, Situational Phobia, and Simple Phobia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49(4):273–81. [PubMed]
11. Kendler KS, Myers J, Prescott CA, Neale MC. The Genetic Epidemiology of Irrational Fears and Phobias in Men. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000 [PubMed]
12. Kendler KS, Karkowski LM, Prescott CA. Fears and Phobias: Reliability and Heritability. Psychol Med. 1999;29(3):539–53. [PubMed]
13. Fyer AJ, Mannuzza S, Chapman TF, Martin LY, Klein DF. Specificity in Familial Aggregation of Phobic Disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52(7):564–73. [PubMed]
14. Carey G. Genes, Fears, Phobias, and Phobic Disorders. Journal of Counseling and Development. 1990;68:628–32.
15. Ledoux J. Fear and the Brain: Where Have We Been, and Where Are We Going? Biol Psychiatry. 1998 Dec 15;44(12):1229–38. [PubMed]
16. Lissek S, Powers AS, McClure EB, Phelps EA, Woldehawariat G, Grillon C, Pine DS. Classical Fear Conditioning in the Anxiety Disorders: a Meta-Analysis. Behav Res Ther. 2005;43(11):1391–424. [PubMed]
17. Hettema JM, Annas P, Neale MC, Kendler KS, Fredrikson M. A Twin Study of the Genetics of Fear Conditioning. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(7):702–8. [PubMed]
18. Gottesman II, Shields J. Genetic Theorizing and Schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 1973;122(566):15–30. [PubMed]
19. Smoller JW, Tsuang MT. Panic and Phobic Anxiety: Defining Phenotypes for Genetic Studies. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155(9):1152–62. [See Comments] [PubMed]
20. Gottesman II, Gould TD. The Endophenotype Concept in Psychiatry: Etymology and Strategic Intentions. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(4):636–45. [PubMed]
21. Cederlöf R, Friberg L, Jonsson E, Kaij L. Studies on Similarity Diagnosis in Twins With the Aid of Mailed Questionnaires. Acta Genet. 1961;11:338–62. [PubMed]
22. Prokasy WF, Kumpfer KL. Classical Conditioning. In: Prokasy WF, Raskin DC, editors. Electrodermal Activity in Psychological Research. New York: Academic Press; 1973. pp. 157–202.
23. Lockhart RA. Comments Regarding Multiple Response Phenomena in Long Interstimulus Interval Conditioning. Psychophysiology. 1966;3(2):108–14. [PubMed]
24. Fredrikson M, Annas P, Georgiades A, Hursti T, Tersman Z. Internal Consistency and Temporal Stability of Classically Conditioned Skin Conductance Responses. Biol Psychol. 1993;35(2):153–63. [PubMed]
25. Fredrikson M, Annas P, Fischer H, Wik G. Gender and Age Differences in the Prevalence of Specific Fears and Phobias. Behav Res Ther. 1996;34(1):33–9. [PubMed]
26. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994.
27. Spielberger C, Gorsuch R, Luchene R. The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: Test manual for Form X. Palo Alto, Ca: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1970.
28. SAS Institute. SAS/STAT Software: Version 8. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc.; 1999.
29. Neale MC, Cardon LR. Methodology for Genetic Studies of Twins and Families. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers B.V.; 1992.
30. Neale MC, Boker SM, Xie G, Maes HH. Mx: Statistical Modeling. 5. Richmond, Va 23298: Dept of Psychiatry, Medical College of VA of VA Commonwealth Univ; 1999. Box 980126.
31. Akaike H. Factor Analysis and AIC. Psychometrika. 1987;52:317–32.
32. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. Emotion, Motivation, and Anxiety: Brain Mechanisms and Psychophysiology. Biol Psychiatry. 1998 Dec 15;44(12):1248–63. [PubMed]
33. Jackson ED, Payne JD, Nadel L, Jacobs WJ. Stress Differentially Modulates Fear Conditioning in Healthy Men and Women. Biol Psychiatry. 2006 Mar 15;59(6):516–22. [PubMed]
34. Wilken JA, Smith BD, Tola K, Mann M. Trait Anxiety and Prior Exposure to Non-Stressful Stimuli: Effects on Psychophysiological Arousal and Anxiety. Int J Psychophysiol. 2000;37(3):233–42. [PubMed]
35. Wilken J, Smith BD, Tola K, Mann M. Anxiety and Arousal: Tests of a New Six-System Model. Int J Psychophysiol. 1999;33(3):197–207. [PubMed]
36. Deckersbach T, Miller KK, Klibanski A, Fischman A, Dougherty DD, Blais MA, Herzog DB, Rauch SL. Regional Cerebral Brain Metabolism Correlates of Neuroticism and Extraversion. Depress Anxiety. 2006;23(3):133–8. [PubMed]
37. Kawachi I, Sparrow D, Vokonas PS, Weiss ST. Decreased Heart Rate Variability in Men With Phobic Anxiety (Data From the Normative Aging Study) Am J Cardiol. 1995 May 1;75(14):882–5. [PubMed]
38. Hoehn-Saric R, McLeod DR, Funderburk F, Kowalski P. Somatic Symptoms and Physiologic Responses in Generalized Anxiety Disorder and Panic Disorder: an Ambulatory Monitor Study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(9):913–21. [PubMed]
39. Hoehn-Saric R, McLeod DR. The Peripheral Sympathetic Nervous System. Its Role in Normal and Pathologic Anxiety. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 1988;11(2):375–86. [PubMed]
40. Hoehn-Saric R, McLeod DR, Zimmerli WD. Somatic Manifestations in Women With Generalized Anxiety Disorder. Psychophysiological Responses to Psychological Stress. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989;46(12):1113–9. [PubMed]
41. Lykken DT, Iacono WG, Haroian K, McGue M, Bouchard TJ., Jr Habituation of the Skin Conductance Response to Strong Stimuli: a Twin Study. Psychophysiology. 1988;25(1):4–15. [PubMed]
42. Dimberg U, Fredrikson M, Lundquist O. Autonomic Reactions to Social and Neutral Stimuli in Subjects High and Low in Public Speaking Fear. Biol Psychol. 1986;23(3):223–33. [PubMed]
43. Lader MH. Palmar Skin Conductance Measures in Anxiety and Phobic States. J Psychosom Res. 1967;11(3):271–81. [PubMed]
44. Kendler KS. Twin Studies of Psychiatric Illness: Current Status and Future Directions. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1993;50:905–15. [PubMed]
45. Scarr S, Carter-Saltzman L. Twin Method: Defense of a Critical Assumption. Behavior Genetics. 1979;9:527–42. [PubMed]
46. Hettema JM, Neale MC, Kendler KS. Physical Similarity and the Equal-Environment Assumption in Twin Studies of Psychiatric Disorders. Behav Genet. 1995;25(4):327–35. [PubMed]