PMCCPMCCPMCC

Search tips
Search criteria 

Advanced

 
Logo of jgimedspringer.comThis journalToc AlertsSubmit OnlineOpen Choice
 
J Gen Intern Med. 2007 March; 22(3): 368–373.
Published online 2007 January 20. doi:  10.1007/s11606-006-0026-y
PMCID: PMC1824725

Cultural Sensitivity in Screening Adults for a History of Childhood Abuse: Evidence from a Community Sample

Abstract

Background

A number of practice guidelines and recommendations call for the assessment of childhood abuse history among adult medical patients. The cultural sensitivity of screening questions, however, has not been examined.

Objective

To assess whether questions that inquire about childhood abuse history function differently for black and white patients.

Design

Cross-sectional telephone surveys in 1997 and 2003.

Subjects

Randomly sampled adults from Memphis, Tenn (1997, N = 832; 2003, N = 967).

Measurements

Physical, emotional, and sexual abuse scales of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire–Short Form (CTQ-SF). Standardized mean difference technique for differential item functioning to assess for possible bias in CTQ-SF items.

Results

Controlling for total physical abuse scale scores, black respondents were significantly (P < .01) more likely than white respondents to report that they had been punished with a hard object during their childhood, but less likely to report having being hit so hard that it left marks, have been hit so hard that someone noticed, or to believe they had been physically abused.

Conclusions

Inquiries that do not explicitly differentiate physical punishment from physical abuse may not be useful for black respondents because they tend to identify black respondents who report fewer clearly abusive experiences than comparable white respondents. Although untested in this study, one possible explanation is that physical discipline may be used more frequently and may play a different role among black families than among white families. These results underline the importance of attending to cultural factors in clinical history taking about childhood abuse histories.

KEY WORDS: childhood abuse, Childhood Trauma Questionnaire–Short Form, assessment

BACKGROUND

Racial and ethnic disparities in health care access, experiences, and outcomes are well documented.1 Improving patient-centered communication and the cultural sensitivity of physicians has been proposed as an important step toward improving quality of care for minority populations and eliminating health care disparities.2 Cultural sensitivity may be defined as a set of attitudes, skills, behaviors, and policies that enable individuals to establish effective interpersonal and working relationships that supersede cultural differences.3 For clinicians, culturally competent history taking is especially important when trying to understand a patient’s unique experience and its impact on current health status.

Screening for histories of childhood abuse is potentially one such experience. A history of physical or sexual abuse is reported in as many as 20–50% of patients in adult primary care settings.4 Rates of reported childhood physical and sexual abuse do not appear to differ between black and white respondents in adult primary care practice or community samples,5,6 although differences have been detected in rates of reported emotional abuse and exposure to a wide group of adverse childhood events, including household dysfunction.6,7

A growing body of research shows an association between a history of childhood maltreatment and both psychiatric and nonpsychiatric medical problems in adults. Childhood abuse has been associated with depression and anxiety,812 and medical diagnoses such as headache,13 irritable bowel syndrome,14 fibromyalgia,15 and other chronic pain conditions.16 In addition, patients with abuse histories report more health risk behaviors, such as unsafe sexual practices17,18 and alcohol and drug use,12,19 and use more health care resources.20,21

A recent review called for primary and subspecialty care physicians to screen adult patients for a history of childhood abuse as a health risk factor.4 Practice guidelines and recommendations for a number of specific psychiatric and nonpsychiatric medical conditions include an assessment of abuse history.14,2224 There are no published guidelines, however, for how and under what conditions adults should be screened for childhood abuse histories in primary care settings. Although it is generally accepted that patients will share more information when questioned about specific experiences, instead of using the term “abuse,”25,26 even specific experiences may have different implications and consequences in a cross-cultural context.

Culturally sensitive assessment of childhood abuse history requires that screening questions and interpretation of responses accurately reflect the experiences of patients rather than bias in the assessment process. If questions are unbiased, patients from different racial groups who have similar childhood abuse histories will respond similarly to individual questions about childhood abuse. Methodologically, this means that responses to a given question will be independent of racial group membership among patients who are matched on their responses to a set of related questions about childhood abuse. Otherwise, a reasonable conclusion would be that the question is assessing something related to racial group membership, but not necessarily abuse per se.27,28

It is possible, for instance, that differences in the role of physical punishment between black and white families could influence responses to screening questions about childhood physical abuse. Numerous studies have reported that black families tend to use corporal punishment with children more than other racial and ethnic groups in the United States.2931 The role of corporal punishment in black families, however, may be different than in white families. Studies have found, for instance, that the association between physical discipline and disruptive behaviors found in white children does not necessarily generalize to black children.30,32,33

The objective of this study was to assess, using a community telephone sample, whether commonly used queries about abuse history may function differently with black and white patients. Although the research literature does not document how primary care physicians in practice typically assess childhood trauma, the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire–Short Form (CTQ-SF)34,35 is the most commonly used retrospective screening tool for childhood maltreatment in medical research. A January 2006 MEDLINE search found that the CTQ-SF had been used in 70 different studies, whereas no other self-report instrument for retrospectively assessing childhood abuse had been used in more than 10 studies. We used questions about childhood abuse from the CTQ-SF to compare responses across black and white respondent groups. Because of differences between black and white families in the use of physical punishment and its relationship with negative behaviors among children, we hypothesized that one question on the CTQ-SF about punishment with a hard object would be endorsed by black respondents at comparatively higher rates than would be expected based on their responses to other questions about childhood physical abuse. To test this hypothesis and to explore other potential differences in responses to questions about childhood physical, sexual, and emotional abuse, we used data from 2 large population samples from the greater Memphis area.

METHODS

Sample Selection

The CTQ-SF was administered in 2 large population samples, the 2003 Mid-South Social Survey Research Program (MSSSRP) survey and the 1997 Memphis Area Study (MAS). In each survey, households were randomly sampled from telephone numbers in the Memphis and Shelby County Telephone System Coles directory. Eligible respondents were English-speaking residents ages 18 to 75 for the 2003 survey and ages 18 to 65 for the 1997 survey. Households were randomly designated as male or female before telephone contact. If the person answering the phone was of the specified sex, only that person could be interviewed. If the person was not of the specified sex, the interviewer asked the person to choose a household member of the specified sex. If a person of the specified sex did not live in the household, the person answering the phone became the selected respondent.

Instrument

The CTQ-SF34,35 is a 28-item retrospective self-report questionnaire designed to assess 5 dimensions of childhood maltreatment: physical abuse, emotional abuse, sexual abuse, physical neglect, and emotional neglect. In this study, we analyzed data from the 15 items of the 3 abuse scales of the CTQ-SF: physical abuse, emotional abuse, and sexual abuse. Each of the 3 abuse scales has 5 items, and the item response options reflect the frequency of maltreatment experiences (never, rarely, sometimes, often, and very often). Bernstein et al.34 reported good internal consistency of the CTQ-SF for each of the abuse scales across 4 heterogeneous samples: physical abuse = 0.83 to 0.86, emotional abuse = 0.84 to 0.89, and sexual abuse = 0.92 to 0.95. Table 1 shows the CTQ-SF items from the 3 scales used in this study with mean scores and standard deviations.

Table 1
Items from the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire–Short Form abuse scales with means and standard deviations

Administration

All interviews were done over the telephone between March 27 and May 23, 2003 for MSSSRP and between February 12 and April 25, 1997 for MAS. Each interviewer received a training manual and at least 6 hours of training, as well as 3–4 practice interviews with supervision. Computer-assisted telephone interviewing files were downloaded automatically at the completion of each interview, and files were checked for reliability in data entry. Interviewers attempted to reach each selected household at least 10–12 times before listing it as a noncontact.

Analyses

Bivariate analyses comparing black and white respondents on demographic variables were conducted by means of χ2 statistics for categorical variables and two-tailed t tests for continuous variables. We assessed potential bias across race on abuse questions by using a standardized mean difference technique for differential item functioning (DIF), which compares the item means of two groups after adjusting for differences on a matching variable.36 DIF is considered to be present when responses to an item depend on a factor (e.g., race) other than the construct that the item is designed to measure (e.g., physical abuse). DIF analysis in this study was done by comparing each item mean score for black and white respondents, adjusted for the total score on the appropriate abuse scale and demographic factors, including age, sex, marital status, and level of education (SPSS General Linear Model, Univariate, Main Effects). Demographic factors were included in the item mean adjustments to assess the independent effects of race above and beyond these factors. Adjusted means for each item were computed for black and white respondents, and an item was classified as functioning differently across black and white respondents if the mean item score was significantly different across racial groups after controlling for the appropriate total scale score and demographic factors. In addition, Cohen’s effect size d37 was also estimated for each item based on pooled standard deviations to estimate the magnitude of the difference. All analyses were conducted using SPSS version 13.0 (Chicago, Ill), and all statistical tests were two-sided with a P < .05 significance level.

RESULTS

Sample Characteristics

In the 2003 and 1997 surveys, interviews with CTQ-SF data were completed in 880 of 1,266 eligible households (69.5% response rate) and 998 of 1,303 eligible households (76.6%), respectively. Only black and white respondents were included in the analyses because of the small number of respondents who identified themselves as belonging to any other racial/ethnic group. Of 832 black and white respondents in the 2003 survey, 812 provided complete data for all abuse items, 828 for all physical abuse items, 823 for all emotional abuse items, and 821 for all sexual abuse items. Of 967 black and white respondents in the 1997 survey, there were complete data on 945 for all abuse items, 956 for all physical abuse items, 953 for all emotional abuse items, and 955 for all sexual abuse items.

As shown in Table 2, for both the 2003 and 1997 surveys, white respondents were significantly older than black respondents (P < .01) and more likely to be married or living with a partner (P < .01), have more education (P < .01), and earn more (P < .01, data for 2003 only). Black respondents tended to be more likely to be female, albeit not significantly.

Table 2
Demographic data by race for 2003 and 1997 samples

Assessment of Racial Differences in Responses to Questions About Childhood Abuse

Adjusted physical, emotional, and sexual abuse scale item means by race are shown in Table 3. In both the 2003 and 1997 surveys, the adjusted item mean for the item, “I was punished with a belt, a board, a cord, or some other hard object,” as hypothesized, was significantly higher for black respondents than for white respondents (P < .01, d = 0.18 in both surveys). In the 2003 survey, white respondents were significantly more likely to indicate that “People in my family hit me so hard that it left me with bruises or marks” (P < .01, d = −0.23) and “I believe that I was physically abused” (P < .01, d = −0.19) compared to black respondents after controlling for overall physical abuse scores and demographics. For the 1997 survey, the adjusted item means were significantly higher for white respondents on 3 items: “People in my family hit me so hard that it left me with bruises or marks” (P = .02, d = −0.13), “I got hit or beaten so badly that it was noticed by someone like a teacher, neighbor, or doctor” (P = .01, d = −0.15), and “I believe that I was physically abused” (P = .03, d = −0.13). Thus, for respondents with a given overall physical abuse scale score, black respondents were more likely to endorse having been punished with a hard object than whites, but were less likely to endorse items that reflect being hit hard enough to leave marks, being hit hard enough that it was noticed, or to state that they believe they were physically abused. For both the 2003 and 1997 data, when the item “I was punished with a belt, a board, a cord, or some other hard object” is removed from the physical abuse scale, no remaining items demonstrated significant differences across race. This was not the case when the other items identified as displaying DIF were removed from the physical abuse total score calculation. No items from the sexual abuse or emotional abuse scales from either survey demonstrated significant differences across race, controlling for the total abuse score on the appropriate scale and demographics.

Table 3
Physical abuse scale item means for black and white respondents adjusted for total physical abuse scale score, age, sex, education level, and marital status

As shown in Table 4, black respondents at higher income and education levels were more likely to have been punished with hard objects than black respondents at lower levels. There was no difference in responses to this question based on educational or income levels for white participants. Overall, black respondents tended to be more likely to endorse having been punished with hard objects across levels of educational attainment and total household income than white respondents.

Table 4
Endorsement of having been punished with hard objects by race and level of education, total household income

DISCUSSION

This study employed a frequently used childhood abuse questionnaire in 2 population-based samples to compare responses between black and white respondents to inform culturally competent history taking in clinical settings. Several items of the physical abuse scale were identified through DIF analysis to function differently across groups. After controlling for demographic differences and overall levels of physical abuse based on CTQ-SF responses, black respondents were significantly more likely to report having been “punished with a belt, a board, a cord, or some other hard object,” than white respondents, but less likely to report experiences like having been “hit...so hard that it left...bruises or marks,” “hit...so badly that it was noticed,” or “physically abused.” The differences in physical abuse responses between black and white respondents did not appear to reflect differences in socioeconomic indicators because these were controlled for in the analyses.

When individual items are found to have DIF, it is typically because they are measuring something different across groups. In this study, several items of the physical abuse scale initially exhibited DIF. When the item inquiring about having been punished with hard objects was removed from the scale, however, none of the other items functioned differently for black and white respondents. This suggests that the “punished with hard objects” item of the CTQ-SF likely measures both the physical abuse construct being measured by the other physical abuse items, as well as an additional construct that is more prevalent among black respondents. One possibility is that the item measures exposure to both physical abuse and physical discipline and that the relationship between these two constructs is not the same for black and white respondents. This would be the case, for instance, if black respondents were punished more often with hard objects than white respondents even when they did not report other clearly physically abusive experiences.

Physical discipline is common in the United States, with 94% of parents in a national phone survey reporting hand-slapping or spanking a toddler.31 It is more prevalent, however, among black compared with white families,31 and there is evidence to suggest racial and ethnic differences in associations between physical discipline and outcomes.33,38 Deater-Deckard et al.,33 for instance, found higher levels of aggressive behaviors among white adolescents who experienced physical discipline compared to white adolescents who were not physically disciplined, but comparatively lower levels of behavior problems among black adolescents who had been physically disciplined.

Primary care visits present an opportunity to screen patients for a history of childhood abuse.4 This study highlights possible differences between black and white patients’ responses to questions about potentially physically abusive experiences. The results from this study emphasize the need for culturally sensitive assessment with a patient-in-context approach to the evaluation of behaviors and life experiences.39 This requires moving beyond assumptions and providing patients with the time to provide more detailed explanations of their experiences in a safe and confidential setting. For instance, if a patient indicates that he or she was punished with hard objects, it is important to inquire about the circumstances of the punishment, including how it was carried out; what the patient thought of the punishment at the time and now; if the patient would like his/her own children to have a different or similar experience; and the patient’s understanding of childhood abuse, including examples of what does and does not constitute abuse. Because of the known adverse effects of physical abuse on both black and white patients,812,14,16,17 it is important for clinicians to screen for physical abuse, but to also understand that different questions about physical abuse may have somewhat different implications for black and white patients.

There are limitations that should be taken into consideration in interpreting results from this study. First, as is the case in most retrospective studies of childhood maltreatment, data in this study was limited to self-report. Thus, the actual relative prevalence of a history of childhood physical abuse among black and white adults is unknown because the majority of cases are never reported to authorities.40 Second, characteristics of nonresponders are not known, creating the possibility of bias. Third, we have not examined the relationship between report of physical punishment or physical abuse in childhood and adult outcomes. Thus, we cannot comment on the relationship between each of these constructs and long-term prognosis. Fourth, it is possible that changes in public discourses on abuse over the course of the sample’s maturation from childhood may have influenced responses. An additional possible limitation involves socioeconomic differences between black and white respondents, as shown in Table 4. However, because all DIF analyses controlled for demographic and socioeconomic differences, it would appear that the study’s results were because of differences in culturally related practices instead of socioeconomic differences.

In summary, this study with a large sample of both black and white respondents employed typical abuse screening questions from the CTQ-SF and found that blacks endorsed being punished with hard objects as children at much greater levels than would be expected by their responses to other questions about physical abuse. Although evidence in this study came from a community telephone sample, the results underline the importance of attending to cultural factors in clinical history taking about childhood abuse histories. They also serve, more broadly, to demonstrate empirically that specific communication and assessment strategies may function differently depending on the cultural background of patients.

Acknowledgements

Funding for the project was obtained by Dr. Forde from the Memphis Shelby Crime Commission. Dr. Ziegelstein is supported by the Miller Family Scholar Program. The authors are grateful to Ms. Cheri Smith of the Harrison Medical Library of the Johns Hopkins Bayview Medical Center for her assistance in this research.

Dr. Scher is now a member of the Department of Psychology, California State University, Fullerton, CA, USA.

Dr. Thombs is now a member of the Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada.

Potential Financial Conflicts of Interest Dr. Bernstein is the author of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire, which is published by the Psychological Corporation.

References

1. Mayberry RM, Mili F, Ofili E. Racial and ethnic differences in access to medical care. Med Care Res Rev. 2000;57(suppl 1):108–45. [PubMed]
2. Smedley BD, Stith AY, Nelson AR, eds. Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Healthcare. JAMA 2003;290:2487–8
3. Health Resources and Services Administration Bureau of Health Professions, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Bureau of Health Professions: Other definitions of cultural competence. Accessed December 21, 2005.
4. Springer KW, Sheridan J, Kuo D, Carnes M. The long-term health outcomes of childhood abuse. An overview and a call to action. J Gen Intern Med. 2003;18(10):864–70. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
5. McCauley J, Kern DE, Kolodner K, Dill L, Schroeder AF, DeChant HK, et al. Clinical characteristics of women with a history of childhood abuse: unhealed wounds. JAMA. 1997;277(17):1362–8. [PubMed]
6. Scher CD, Forde DR, McQuaid JR, Stein MB. Prevalence and demographic correlates of childhood maltreatment in an adult community sample. Child Abuse Negl. 2004;28(2):167–80. [PubMed]
7. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, et al. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. Am J Prev Med. 1998;14(4):245–58. [PubMed]
8. MacMillan HL, Fleming JE, Streiner DL, Lin E, Boyle MH, Jamieson E, et al. Childhood abuse and lifetime psychopathology in a community sample. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(11):1878–83. [PubMed]
9. Molnar BE, Buka SL, Kessler RC. Child sexual abuse and subsequent psychopathology: results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Am J Public Health. 2001;91(5):753–60. [PubMed]
10. Wise LA, Zierler S, Krieger N, Harlow BL. Adult onset of major depressive disorder in relation to early life violent victimisation: a case-control study. Lancet. 2001;358(9285):881–7. [PubMed]
11. Brown J, Cohen P, Johnson JG, Smailes EM. Childhood abuse and neglect: specificity of effects on adolescent and young adult depression and suicidality. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999;38(12):1490–6. [PubMed]
12. Kendler KS, Bulik CM, Silberg J, Hettema JM, Myers J, Prescott CA. Childhood sexual abuse and adult psychiatric and substance use disorders in women: an epidemiological and cotwin control analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57(10):953–9. [PubMed]
13. Goodwin RD, Hoven CW, Murison R, Hotopf M. Association between childhood physical abuse and gastrointestinal disorders and migraine in adulthood. Am J Public Health. 2003;93(7):1065–7. [PubMed]
14. Drossman DA, Talley NJ, Leserman J, Olden KW, Barreiro MA. Sexual and physical abuse and gastrointestinal illness. Review and recommendations. Ann Intern Med. 1995;123(10):782–94. [PubMed]
15. Walker EA, Keegan D, Gardner G, Sullivan M, Bernstein D, Katon WJ. Psychosocial factors in fibromyalgia compared with rheumatoid arthritis: II. Sexual, physical, and emotional abuse and neglect. Psychosom Med. 1997;59(6):572–7. [PubMed]
16. Green CR, Flowe-Valencia H, Rosenblum L, Tait AR. The role of childhood and adulthood abuse among women presenting for chronic pain management. Clin J Pain. 2001;17(4):359–64. [PubMed]
17. Walker EA, Gelfand A, Katon WJ, Koss MP, Von Korff M, Bernstein D, et al. Adult health status of women with histories of childhood abuse and neglect. Am J Med. 1999;107(4):332–9. [PubMed]
18. Springs FE, Friedrich WN. Health risk behaviors and medical sequelae of childhood sexual abuse. Mayo Clin Proc. 1992;67(6):527–32. [PubMed]
19. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, et al. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am J Prev Med. 1998;14(4):245–58. [PubMed]
20. Walker EA, Unutzer J, Rutter C, Gelfand A, Saunders K, VonKorff M, et al. Costs of health care use by women HMO members with a history of childhood abuse and neglect. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56(7):609–13. [PubMed]
21. Fiddler M, Jackson J, Kapur N, Wells A, Creed F. Childhood adversity and frequent medical consultations. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2004;26(5):367–77. [PubMed]
22. Fall M, Baranowski AP, Fowler CJ, Lepinard V, Malone-Lee JG, Messelink EJ, et al. EAU guidelines on chronic pelvic pain. Eur Urol. 2004;46(6):681–9. [PubMed]
23. ACOG Committee on Practice Bulletins–Gynecology. ACOG Practice Bulletin No. 51. Chronic pelvic pain. Obstet Gynecol. 2004;103(3):589–605. [PubMed]
24. American Gastroenterology Association. American Gastroenterological Association medical position statement: irritable bowel syndrome. Gastroenterology. 2002;123(6):2105–7. [PubMed]
25. Dill DL, Chu JA, Grob MC, Eisen SV. The reliability of abuse history reports: a comparison of two inquiry formats. Compr Psychiatry. 1991;32(2):166–9. [PubMed]
26. Fricker AE, Smith DW, Davis JL, Hanson RF. Effects of context and question type on endorsement of childhood sexual abuse. J Trauma Stress. 2003;16(3):265–8. [PubMed]
27. Hui CH, Triandis HC. Measurement in cross-cultural psychology: a review and comparison of strategies. J Cross-Cult Psychol. 1985;16(2):131–152.
28. Poortinga, YH. Equivalence of cross-cultural data: an overview of basic issues. Int J Psychol. 1989;24(6):737–756.
29. Day RD, Peterson GW, McCracken C. Predicting spanking of younger and older children by mothers and fathers. J Marriage Fam. 1998;60(1):79–94.
30. Wissow LS. Ethnicity, income, and parenting contexts of physical punishment in a national sample of families with young children. Child Maltreatment. 2001;6(2):118–29. [PubMed]
31. Straus MA, Stewart JH. Corporal punishment by American parents: national data on prevalence, chronicity, severity, and duration, in relation to child and family characteristics. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 1999;2(2):55–70. [PubMed]
32. Lansford JE, Deater-Deckard K, Dodge KA, Bates JE, Pettit GS. Ethnic differences in the link between physical discipline and later adolescent externalizing behaviors. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45(4):801–12. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
33. Deater-Deckard K, Dodge KA, Bates JE, Pettit GS. Physical discipline among African American and European American mothers: links to children’s externalizing behaviors. Dev Psychol. 1996;32(6):1065–72.
34. Bernstein DP, Stein JA, Newcomb MD, Walker E, Pogge D, Ahluvalia T, et al. Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27(2):169–90. [PubMed]
35. Bernstein DP, Fink L. Childhood Trauma Questionnaire: A retrospective self-report manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1998.
36. Zwick R, Thayer DT. Evaluating the magnitude of differential item functioning in polytomous items. J Educ Behav Stat. 1996;21(3):187–201.
37. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
38. Whaley AL. Sociocultural differences in the developmental consequences of the use of physical discipline during childhood for African Americans. Cult Divers Ment Health. 2000;6(1):5–12.
39. American Psychological Association. Guidelines on multicultural education, training, research, practice, and organizational change for psychologists. 2002.
40. Martin J, Anderson J, Romans S, Mullen P, O’Shea M. Asking about child sexual abuse: methodological implications of a two stage survey. Child Abuse Negl. 1993;17(3):383–92. [PubMed]

Articles from Journal of General Internal Medicine are provided here courtesy of Society of General Internal Medicine